10.02.2013 Views

Studies in Sound Symbolism

Studies in Sound Symbolism

Studies in Sound Symbolism

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

GOTHENBURG MONOGRAPHS IN LINGUISTICS 17<br />

<strong>Studies</strong> <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>Sound</strong> <strong>Symbolism</strong><br />

Åsa Abel<strong>in</strong><br />

Doctoral Dissertation<br />

publicly defended <strong>in</strong> Stora Hörsalen,<br />

Humanisten, Göteborg University,<br />

on May 14, 1999, at 10.00<br />

for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy<br />

Department of L<strong>in</strong>guistics, Göteborg University, Sweden, 1999


ABSTRACT<br />

This thesis <strong>in</strong>vestigates how the Swedish lexicon is structured with<br />

respect to sound symbolism, the productivity of phonesthemes and cross<br />

language similarities <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> areas of sound symbolism.<br />

The Swedish lexicon has been analyzed with emphasis on the sound<br />

symbolic properties of <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters, and to a<br />

certa<strong>in</strong> extent of vowels. Approximately 1, 000 lexemes were judged to<br />

be sound symbolic and the outcome of the analysis are tentative<br />

phonesthemes, i.e. motivated connections between mean<strong>in</strong>gs and<br />

consonant clusters.<br />

Almost all Swedish <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters and many of the f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

consonant clusters can carry sound symbolic mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Lexically<br />

<strong>in</strong>frequent clusters are utilized to a larger extent than lexically frequent<br />

clusters. No two consonant clusters have exactly the same semantic<br />

profile. Phonesthemes have different sound symbolic strength, i.e. some<br />

are clearly sound symbolic (i.e. a high percentage of the words<br />

beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with a certa<strong>in</strong> cluster are sound symbolic), and carry either<br />

one mean<strong>in</strong>g or several mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Other (candidates for) phonesthemes<br />

are weaker and not so clearly sound symbolic.<br />

The mean<strong>in</strong>gs of most phonesthemes are relatable to the senses:<br />

hear<strong>in</strong>g, vision or tactile sensation, or they are metaphorically or<br />

metonymically connected to the senses. The most common semantic<br />

features occurr<strong>in</strong>g are often relatable to synaesthesia.<br />

The productivity of phonesthemes was tested <strong>in</strong> experiments of<br />

production and understand<strong>in</strong>g. The experiments show, for<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation, that no constructed word is <strong>in</strong>terpreted as expected by<br />

all subjects, but that all of the constructed words are <strong>in</strong>terpreted<br />

correctly by some subjects. The most common semantic features found<br />

<strong>in</strong> the lexical analysis are often also the most successfully <strong>in</strong>terpreted by<br />

subjects. For production, the experiments <strong>in</strong>dicate that subjects tend to<br />

encode the semantic features <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial clusters rather than <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters. F<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters seem to be of less importance than the<br />

<strong>in</strong>itial clusters <strong>in</strong> new sound symbolic words <strong>in</strong> Swedish.<br />

For the contrastive studies, the general results are that there are both<br />

similarities and dissimilarities between the expressions <strong>in</strong> the different<br />

languages. The variation is greater for some semantic fields than for<br />

others.<br />

KEY WORDS: sound symbolism, lexical structure, synaesthesia,<br />

productivity, universals


Acknowledgements<br />

The first idea to write this thesis emerged when I was work<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the<br />

Lexical Database and Svensk Ordbok project at Språkdata <strong>in</strong> the 1980's,<br />

where I alphabetically plowed through large parts of the Swedish<br />

vocabulary.<br />

My greatest thanks goes to my supervisor Jens Allwood, for always<br />

support<strong>in</strong>g me and for be<strong>in</strong>g so consistent <strong>in</strong> always misunderstand<strong>in</strong>g<br />

what is not perfectly clear. I am also grateful to Elisabeth Ahlsén who has<br />

given me many valuable comments and especially encouraged my<br />

experiment<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

I also want to thank all my colleagues and former colleagues at the<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistics department, especially Sally Boyd, Beatrice Dorriots, Johan<br />

Hagman, Jerker Järborg, Per L<strong>in</strong>dblad, Lars Malmsten, Kerst<strong>in</strong> Nelfelt,<br />

Shirley Nicholson, Joakim Nivre, Sören Sjöström, Sven Strömqvist,<br />

Nicole Takolander, Hans Vappula, Åsa Wengel<strong>in</strong>, Ulla Veres, and all<br />

others who have helped me <strong>in</strong> various ways over the years. A special<br />

thanks goes to Cather<strong>in</strong>e Paterson who did the cover design.<br />

I also want to express my gratitude to my <strong>in</strong>formants from many parts of<br />

the world.<br />

The persons who I want to thank especially are my children Tove and<br />

Ellen who are for sure very tired of the word "avhandl<strong>in</strong>g". They have<br />

shown great patience with my <strong>in</strong>termittent absentm<strong>in</strong>dedness, but they<br />

have also shown great <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the subject of this thesis and given me<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g comments from their po<strong>in</strong>t of view. F<strong>in</strong>ally I want to thank my<br />

family, my mother and father, my sisters Ulla and Susanne and all my<br />

other friends.<br />

Göteborg <strong>in</strong> April, 2000<br />

Åsa Abel<strong>in</strong>


Contents<br />

Chapter 0 Introduction 1<br />

Chapter 1 Background 3<br />

Chapter 2 Theoretical framework 49<br />

Chapter 3 Method 71<br />

Chapter 4 Analysis of <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters 77<br />

Chapter 5 Analysis of f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters, vowels and<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ations. 143<br />

Chapter 6 Some contrastive studies <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism 189<br />

Chapter 7 Experiments with words constructed from phonesthemes 219<br />

Chapter 8 Summary and discussion 245<br />

Bibliography 273<br />

Appendix 1: <strong>Sound</strong> symbolic roots of <strong>in</strong>itial clusters<br />

Appendix 2: Interjections of cross language study<br />

Appendix 3: Test sheets of chapter 7


<strong>Studies</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism<br />

0 Introduction 1<br />

1 Background 3<br />

1.1 Purpose of the chapter 3<br />

1.2 Term<strong>in</strong>ology 3<br />

1.2.1 Onomatopoeia 3<br />

1.2.2 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism 3<br />

1.2.3 Phonestheme 4<br />

1.2.4 Ideophone 4<br />

1.2.5 Morpheme 5<br />

1.2.6 Conclusion 8<br />

1.3 Is sound symbolism the rule or the exception <strong>in</strong><br />

language? 9<br />

1.3.1 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is an exception 9<br />

1.3.2 <strong>Symbolism</strong> is fundamental to language 9<br />

1.3.3 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is both <strong>in</strong>side and outside of<br />

language 11<br />

1.3.4 Evaluation of discussion of sound symbolism 12<br />

1.4 Is sound symbolism productive or not? 12<br />

1.4.1 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is not productive 12<br />

1.4.2 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is productive 13<br />

1.4.3 Greater or lesser degree of productivity 13<br />

1.4.4 Evaluation of the discussion of productivity 13<br />

1.5 The question of etymology 14<br />

1.6 The phylogenesis of language 18<br />

1.7 Universality versus language specificity 20<br />

1.7.1 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is universal 20<br />

1.7.2 <strong>Symbolism</strong> is not universal 21<br />

1.7.3 Evaluation of the discussion of universality<br />

versus language specificity <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism 22<br />

1.8 Context 23<br />

1.9 A framework for models of sound symbolism 26<br />

1.10 Expression and content of sound symbolism 28<br />

1.10.1 Expression 29


1.10.2 Content 29<br />

1.10.3 Expression and content <strong>in</strong> different analyses 29<br />

1.10.4 Results - data from different authors 31<br />

1.10.5 Experimental results 37<br />

1.11 Possible explanations of sound symbolism 41<br />

1.11.1 Miscellaneous explanations and proprioception 41<br />

1.11.2 Synaesthesia 42<br />

1.11.3 Other neurological and biological explanations 44<br />

1.11.4 Non- biological explanations 46<br />

2 Theoretical framework 49<br />

2.1 General considerations 49<br />

2.2 Static-dynamic, conventionality and<br />

arbitrar<strong>in</strong>ess 49<br />

2.3 Semantic analysis 51<br />

2.3.1 Conceptions of mean<strong>in</strong>g 51<br />

2.3.2 Semantic features and semantic fields 52<br />

2.4 Basic relations between expression and content 54<br />

2.5 The nature of phonesthemes 57<br />

2.6 Considerations for a model 59<br />

2.6.1 Relations between the categories 63<br />

2.6.2 An explanatory model for sound symbolism 66<br />

3 Method 71<br />

3.1 Stage 1: Collection of lexical material 71<br />

3.2 Stage 2: Cross-language comparisons 74<br />

3.2.1 Cross-language thesaurus studies 74<br />

3.2.2 Cross-language <strong>in</strong>formant studies 74<br />

3.3 Stage 3: Experiments 76<br />

3.3.1 Experiments with neologisms 76<br />

3.4 Further method 76<br />

4 Analysis of the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters 77<br />

4.1 Data analysis, a short overview 77<br />

4.2 Results 80<br />

4.2.1 More and less sound symbolic clusters 80<br />

4.2.2 Proportions of motivated root morphemes. Summary 86<br />

4.2.3 Types of mean<strong>in</strong>g 88


4.3 Frequent semantic features 93<br />

4.3.1 Pejorative 93<br />

4.3.2 <strong>Sound</strong> 100<br />

4.3.3 Long th<strong>in</strong> form 105<br />

4.3.4 Quick or strong movement 107<br />

4.3.5 Wetness 111<br />

4.3.6 The most sound symbolic clusters 115<br />

4.4 Frequent clusters 116<br />

4.4.1 The cluster sl- 118<br />

4.4.2 The cluster sn- 119<br />

4.4.3 The cluster kn- 121<br />

4.4.4 The cluster kr- 124<br />

4.4.5 The cluster fn- 126<br />

4.4.6 The cluster kn- 127<br />

4.4.7 The cluster gn- 127<br />

4.4.8 The cluster spr- 129<br />

4.4.9 The cluster pj- 130<br />

4.5 Typical or unique mean<strong>in</strong>gs 131<br />

4.6 Patterns of semantic features 134<br />

4.7 Discussion and conclusions 140<br />

5 Analysis of f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters, vowels and<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ations 143<br />

5.1 F<strong>in</strong>al clusters 144<br />

5.2 Summary of the analysis of semantic features for<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al clusters 150<br />

5.3 Properties of consonant clusters of Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 152<br />

5.3.1 Summary of the analysis of properties of different<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al clusters 164<br />

5.4 Vowels 164<br />

5.4.1 Vowel pairs and triplets 164<br />

5.4.2 Vowels <strong>in</strong> light/gaze-words 166<br />

5.4.3 The vowel [P] 167<br />

5.4.4 Summary of vowels 167<br />

5.5 Comparisons of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters of roots from different sources<br />

167<br />

5.5.1 NFO4 and Sigurd (1965) 167


5.5.2 The most frequent f<strong>in</strong>al clusters <strong>in</strong> NFO4 171<br />

5.5.3 Discussion of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters <strong>in</strong> Svensk<br />

Baklängesordbok and of Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 172<br />

5.6 Comb<strong>in</strong>ations of <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters 174<br />

5.6.1 Initial cluster + no f<strong>in</strong>al cluster 175<br />

5.6.2 No <strong>in</strong>itial cluster + f<strong>in</strong>al cluster 179<br />

5.6.3 Initial cluster + f<strong>in</strong>al cluster 182<br />

5.6.4 Summary of comb<strong>in</strong>ations 185<br />

5.7 Summary and discussion of <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al clusters,<br />

and vowels 186<br />

6 Some contrastive studies <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism 189<br />

6.1 Introduction 189<br />

6.2 The Thesaurus study 189<br />

6.2.1 Method 189<br />

6.2.2 Results 190<br />

6.2.2.1Words for 'stupidity' <strong>in</strong> English 190<br />

6.2.2.2 Words for 'stupidity' <strong>in</strong> Swedish 190<br />

6.2.2.3 Words for 'surface structure' <strong>in</strong> English 191<br />

6.2.2.4 Words for 'surface structure' <strong>in</strong> Swedish 192<br />

6.2.3 Conclusions of the Thesaurus study 193<br />

6.3 Some <strong>in</strong>terjections <strong>in</strong> different languages 194<br />

6.3.1 Swedish expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections 195<br />

6.3.2 Discussion of Swedish expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections,<br />

commands and greet<strong>in</strong>gs 196<br />

6.3.3 Phonological and phonetic similarities and<br />

dissimilarities between <strong>in</strong>terjections of different languages 197<br />

6.4 Imitations of animal calls 202<br />

6.4.1 Expressions for animal calls <strong>in</strong> Swedish and other<br />

languages 202<br />

6.4.2 A test of expressions for animal calls of different<br />

languages 205<br />

6.4.3 Results from a test of expressions for animal calls<br />

<strong>in</strong> different languages 206<br />

6.4.4 Discussion of the test on identify<strong>in</strong>g animal calls 208<br />

6.5 Conclusions of studies of expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections<br />

and expressions of animal calls 208


6.6 Test of cross language <strong>in</strong>terpretation of Swedish<br />

onomatopoeic and other sound symbolic words 210<br />

6.6.1 Method 210<br />

6.6.2 Results of <strong>in</strong>terpretation of cross language Swedish<br />

onomatopoeic and other sound symbolic words 211<br />

6.6.3 Conclusions from the test on cross language<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation of Swedish onomatopoeic and other<br />

sound symbolic words 215<br />

6. 7 General conclusions and discussion of the cross<br />

language studies 217<br />

7 Experiments with words constructed from<br />

phonesthemes 219<br />

7.1 Production and understand<strong>in</strong>g 215<br />

7.1.1 Forced choice for production - from mean<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

phonological (graphic) form 223<br />

7.1.2 Forced choice for understand<strong>in</strong>g - from phonological<br />

form to mean<strong>in</strong>g 226<br />

7.1.3 Free production test from constructed words to<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs 229<br />

7.1.4 Free production from mean<strong>in</strong>g to constructed word 232<br />

7.1.5 Match<strong>in</strong>g test of nonsense words and mean<strong>in</strong>gs 238<br />

7.1.6 Summary of results of tests on <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

and sounds 239<br />

8 Summary and discussion 245<br />

8.1 The research questions were as follows: 245<br />

8.1.1 Question 1 246<br />

8.1.2 Question 2 252<br />

8.1.3 Question 3 255<br />

8.1.4 Question 4 257<br />

8.2 Comparison of the studies 258<br />

8.3 Possible explanations of onomatopoeia and sound symbolism 263<br />

8.3.1 Pejoratives 265<br />

8.3.2 Summary 266<br />

8.4 Predictions for sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> Swedish 268<br />

8.5 Ma<strong>in</strong> objectives and further research 271


Introduction<br />

The purpose of this thesis is to study some aspects of the wide field of<br />

onomatopoeia and other sound symbolism with special reference to<br />

Swedish. A large part of the study is devoted to a description, semantic and<br />

phonological, of onomatopoeia and other sound symbolism 1 of <strong>in</strong>itial and<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters, and also of vowels.<br />

One ma<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terest is the issue of productivity, which is studied ma<strong>in</strong>ly with<br />

the aid of experiments.<br />

Furthermore, the issue of universals is addressed through partial<br />

comparisons with other languages and also through tests of perception of<br />

Swedish onomatopoeia and sound symbolism.<br />

The purpose is f<strong>in</strong>ally to construct a model for sound symbolism as a<br />

central part of language, and to construct an explanatory model for the<br />

semantic aspect of sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> Swedish.<br />

The research questions of the thesis are the follow<strong>in</strong>g:<br />

1. What are the properties of sound symbolic sequences <strong>in</strong> Swedish? More<br />

specifically the questions are:<br />

Which <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters are used <strong>in</strong> sound<br />

symbolism?<br />

Which mean<strong>in</strong>gs are used <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism?<br />

How do these comb<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> phonesthemes?<br />

What are the sound symbolic characteristics of some vowels?<br />

How do <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al clusters and vowels comb<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> words?<br />

2. Are phonesthemes productive <strong>in</strong> Swedish? And, if so, are some<br />

phonesthemes more productive than others? Are neologisms created or<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> accordance with the semantic model of chapter 2 and the<br />

analysis of chapters 4 and 5?<br />

3. Are there similarities or dissimilarities, <strong>in</strong> some specific aspects of sound<br />

symbolism, between different languages?<br />

4. Do non-Swedish speakers <strong>in</strong>terpret Swedish phonesthemes <strong>in</strong> accordance<br />

with the semantic model of chapter 2 and the analysis of chapters 4 and 5?<br />

1 cf. p. 4<br />

1


1 Background<br />

1.1 Purpose of the chapter<br />

The area of onomatopoeia and other sound symbolism has not been<br />

central <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistics. Nevertheless, most l<strong>in</strong>guists have had someth<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

say on the subject, <strong>in</strong> many cases us<strong>in</strong>g different term<strong>in</strong>ologies. In this<br />

chapter there will be a thematic overview of some of the most <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g<br />

contributions. The themes that are especially important are the questions<br />

of <strong>in</strong>tegration <strong>in</strong>to language (grammar and lexicon), of productivity, of<br />

universality and of explanation. The chapter will end with a summary of<br />

forms and mean<strong>in</strong>gs of onomatopoeic and sound symbolic expressions.<br />

1.2 Term<strong>in</strong>ology<br />

There are two problems with the term<strong>in</strong>ology <strong>in</strong> this area. First, there are<br />

a lot of terms. Second, the terms are not always used <strong>in</strong> the same way, and<br />

they are seldom def<strong>in</strong>ed. The greatest confusion, if one doesn't know the<br />

author's purpose, is perhaps that the term symbolic is often used to mean<br />

sound symbolic (i. e. words or other expressions hav<strong>in</strong>g a sound structure<br />

that is not <strong>in</strong>dependent of their mean<strong>in</strong>g), and not as a contrast to<br />

<strong>in</strong>dexical and iconic (cf. Peirce (1955) and Allwood and Andersson<br />

(1976)). In this thesis, the term symbolic is therefore not used as a<br />

synonym to sound symbolic. If the term is used, it is used <strong>in</strong> the<br />

traditional way, for conventional non-motivated signs.<br />

1.2.1 Onomatopoeia<br />

Nordberg (1986) gives a def<strong>in</strong>ition. ‘‘Onomatopoeia <strong>in</strong> a restricted sense<br />

refers to imitation of natural sounds, e.g. of animals’’. I do not believe it<br />

is necessary to restrict the term onomatopoeia to natural sounds even<br />

though, of course, this is a special k<strong>in</strong>d of onomatopoeia. I will use the<br />

term onomatopoeia for all k<strong>in</strong>ds of sound imitation.<br />

1.2.2 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism<br />

Nordberg (1986) writes ‘‘<strong>Sound</strong> symbol or phonestheme ... is the<br />

synesthetic comb<strong>in</strong>ation of a certa<strong>in</strong> sound or sound sequence with a<br />

particular notion or a particular connotative content.’’ Jakobson and<br />

Waugh (1979) def<strong>in</strong>e sound symbolism as ‘‘an <strong>in</strong>most, natural association<br />

between sound and mean<strong>in</strong>g’’. Malkiel (1994) uses the term<br />

phonosymbolism to mean the same, he writes, as sound symbolism.<br />

3


I will use the term sound symbolism as a general term for an iconic or<br />

<strong>in</strong>dexical relationship between sound and mean<strong>in</strong>g, and also between<br />

sound and sound (which is onomatopoeia). Onomatopoeia is then a special<br />

case of sound symbolism. Onomatopoeia and other sound symbolism will<br />

also be termed motivated expressions.<br />

1.2.3 Phonestheme<br />

Phonesthemes are sometimes called word aff<strong>in</strong>ities or verbal aff<strong>in</strong>ities and<br />

are described as associated with ‘‘a marg<strong>in</strong>al set of vocables which are<br />

semantically fluid, more expressive than cognitive’’ (Jakobson and<br />

Waugh, 1979) or as ‘‘the group<strong>in</strong>g of similar mean<strong>in</strong>gs about similar<br />

sound’’ (Bol<strong>in</strong>ger, 1965). (The relation between phonesthemes and<br />

morphemes is discussed <strong>in</strong> 1.2.5). Householder's (1946) def<strong>in</strong>ition of a<br />

phonestheme is: ‘‘a phoneme or cluster of phonemes shared by a group of<br />

words which also have <strong>in</strong> common some element of mean<strong>in</strong>g or function,<br />

though the words may be etymologically unrelated’’ The def<strong>in</strong>ition above<br />

could be improved by chang<strong>in</strong>g ‘‘words’’ <strong>in</strong>to ‘‘morphemes’’ (for a<br />

discussion of the morpheme, see 1.2.5).<br />

Another term is psychomorphs (Markell and Hemp, 1960), which is used<br />

<strong>in</strong> approximately the same sense as phonesthemes. Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1950) also<br />

uses the term submorphemic differentials or submorphs which are<br />

described as ‘collocations of phonemes common to a set of words and<br />

suggestive of a stronger or vaguer semantic <strong>in</strong>terconnection. He uses the<br />

term affective morphemes as synonymous to phonesthemes. Nordberg<br />

(1986) co<strong>in</strong>s the term sound words (onomatopoeic elements) as the jo<strong>in</strong>t<br />

designation for ideophones, sound symbols, phonesthemes and<br />

onomatopoeia s<strong>in</strong>ce he claims that the boundaries are fuzzy. Nordberg<br />

also writes that onomatopoeic elements are not such vaguely imitative<br />

words with normal phonotactics as e.g. Swedish susa 'sigh', klucka<br />

'cluck', fladdra 'flutter', etc. but purely sound-illustrative sequences,<br />

rem<strong>in</strong>iscent of the sound balloons of comic strips.<br />

1.2.4. Ideophone<br />

Childs (1994) discusses the problem of def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g ideophones and claims<br />

that often one or more of the follow<strong>in</strong>g criteria are met: ideophones often<br />

have unusual phonological characteristics, and they often display very<br />

little morphology. Syntactically they are often set apart from the rest of<br />

4


an utterance. In some languages they constitute a separate syntactic<br />

category, <strong>in</strong> others not. Semantically they often underscore the mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of a verb, and often, but not always, they are sound symbolic. In many<br />

cases ideophones show a close connection to gestures.<br />

This k<strong>in</strong>d of phenomenon seems to correspond best to the ‘‘sound words’’<br />

of adolescent language, described by Nordberg (1986), (e.g. krch, ppff,<br />

då<strong>in</strong>g, ‘‘har hon håret så här, så här: tsscchht ’’ (‘‘she wears her hair like<br />

this, like this: tsscchh’’, etc.)) but not to phonesthemes.<br />

The term ideophone will not be used <strong>in</strong> my own analysis of onomatopoeia<br />

and sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> Swedish, partly because different authors use the<br />

term <strong>in</strong> a non-uniform way. Even if I do not treat ideophones <strong>in</strong> my own<br />

analysis I will use the term as it is described above, <strong>in</strong> discuss<strong>in</strong>g some of<br />

the authors below.<br />

1.2.5 Morpheme<br />

What exactly is the relation between phonesthemes and morphemes? Are<br />

phonesthemes really morphemes or are they someth<strong>in</strong>g different? The<br />

most common def<strong>in</strong>ition of a morpheme is ‘‘a m<strong>in</strong>imal mean<strong>in</strong>g carry<strong>in</strong>g<br />

unit’’, but that def<strong>in</strong>ition <strong>in</strong> this context seems to apply to phonesthemes<br />

rather than to what traditionally has been labeled morphemes: The lexeme<br />

bjäfs (gewgaws) can be analyzed as one morpheme. But it can, at the same<br />

time, be analyzed as consist<strong>in</strong>g of the m<strong>in</strong>imal mean<strong>in</strong>g units bj 1-<br />

'pejorative', -E- 'pejorative, and -fs 'pejorative'. It seems that these are<br />

really the m<strong>in</strong>imal mean<strong>in</strong>g carry<strong>in</strong>g units.<br />

However, these m<strong>in</strong>imal mean<strong>in</strong>g carry<strong>in</strong>g units do not build up the whole<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g of the word bjäfs. The mean<strong>in</strong>g of a sound symbolic word is<br />

more than the mean<strong>in</strong>g of its parts, but this also goes for many lexicalized<br />

compounds, e.g. blackbird means more than 'black bird'. In the case of<br />

other sound symbolic words, however, it seems that the mean<strong>in</strong>g of a<br />

word is almost always more than the mean<strong>in</strong>g of its parts. As a contrast,<br />

the mean<strong>in</strong>g of onomatopoeic words, like plask, is often not more than the<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g of its parts: pl- 'wetness' and -sk 'wetness'.<br />

1Phonemes will be marked with bold type and are not put between slashes. The symbol P<br />

will be used for the short rounded close-mid vowel, e.g. the first vowel <strong>in</strong> muttra<br />

(mutter).<br />

5


Also, <strong>in</strong> a productive perspective, neologisms are sometimes created or<br />

understood out of (one or more) m<strong>in</strong>imal mean<strong>in</strong>g units (phonesthemes)<br />

and these can thus be seen as build<strong>in</strong>g blocks for word mean<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Distributionally, phonesthemes are not whole words (lexemes) but parts<br />

of words and could thus not qualify as free morphemes. They could<br />

perhaps be described as bound morphemes, i. e. as affixes. However,<br />

there is a fact that contradicts this <strong>in</strong>terpretation, on the expression side:<br />

bound morphemes <strong>in</strong> Swedish can <strong>in</strong>clude a vowel or consist of a s<strong>in</strong>gle<br />

consonant or of a consonant cluster. The doma<strong>in</strong> of the phonestheme is<br />

often a consonant cluster. This shows that the expression side of<br />

phonesthemes is only partially different from other bound morphemes, <strong>in</strong><br />

Swedish. But there are also other more problematic facts on the<br />

expression side <strong>in</strong> traditional morphology (for a discussion see e.g.<br />

Källström (1988), e.g. suppletion (be, am are, is, was, were) and<br />

<strong>in</strong>flect<strong>in</strong>g and fusioned decl<strong>in</strong>ation (e.g. f<strong>in</strong>na, fann, funnen), so it is not<br />

clear that this would dist<strong>in</strong>guish phonesthemes from morphemes.<br />

Another problem can be illustrated by the word flämta (pant), where flis<br />

a phonestheme imitat<strong>in</strong>g movement. The rest of the word -ämta would<br />

have to be called a restmorph. Restmorphs comprise a problematic, but<br />

not unusual, category <strong>in</strong> morphology. A way to avoid too many<br />

restmorphs is to say that <strong>in</strong> sound symbolic words there are mean<strong>in</strong>g units<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependent dimensions to morphemes, e. g. <strong>in</strong> the morphemes bjäfs<br />

and flämta; bjäfs and flämta are mean<strong>in</strong>g units on one level which at the<br />

same time conta<strong>in</strong> bj- (<strong>in</strong> bjäfs ) and fl- (<strong>in</strong> flämta), with pejorative<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs on another level. Examples such as these show that it is a<br />

problem to determ<strong>in</strong>e how far the morphological analysis should go (see<br />

Källström, 1988).<br />

Phonesthemes can be placed <strong>in</strong> a hierarchy between phonemes and<br />

morphemes, where morphemes and phonesthemes both are mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

bear<strong>in</strong>g units while phonemes are not (figure 1.1).<br />

6


morphemes<br />

phonesthemes<br />

phonemes<br />

Figure 1.1. The hierarchy between phonemes and morphemes.<br />

Phonesthemes are built up from phonemes, morphemes can (partly) be<br />

built up from phonesthemes, but phonesthemes are never built up of<br />

morphemes.<br />

The morpheme is a connection between expression and mean<strong>in</strong>g. When<br />

the expression varies, e.g. dog-s [z] - cat-s [s] they are called allomorphs.<br />

A parallel dist<strong>in</strong>ction could be made between e.g. the pejoratives pj-, bj-,<br />

fj- which can be called ‘‘allophonests 2’’ to a phonestheme (with pejorative<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g), <strong>in</strong> an item-and-arrangement analysis. (Alternatively, pj-, bjand<br />

fj- can be analyzed as labial obstruent + j.) These examples show that<br />

these types of analyses can be done with phonesthemes, as well as with<br />

regular morphemes.<br />

A certa<strong>in</strong> consonant cluster can sometimes be the expression of a<br />

phonestheme, but sometimes not, (e.g. kl- kladdig (sound symbolic),<br />

klöver (not sound symbolic). The phonestheme appears <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction with<br />

the word mean<strong>in</strong>g, when it fits the expression 3. That means that we have<br />

homonymy/polysemy <strong>in</strong> phonesthemes, e.g. kl- can mean 'adhesion' as <strong>in</strong><br />

kladdig (sticky), klibbig (sticky), kletig (smeary), klick (dollop), 'wetness'<br />

as <strong>in</strong> klafsa (squelch), it can be 'onomatopoeic' as <strong>in</strong> klang (clang),<br />

klappra (clatter), klatsch (slap), klicka (click), kl<strong>in</strong>g (t<strong>in</strong>kle), klirra<br />

(j<strong>in</strong>gle), klämta (toll), klucka (lap) and it can mean a 'certa<strong>in</strong> form' as <strong>in</strong><br />

klimp (lump), kloss (block), klot (ball), klubba (club), klump (lump) and<br />

it is 'pejorative' as <strong>in</strong> kludda (daub), klotter (doodle), etc, cf. chapter 4.<br />

2This term will, however, not be used further.<br />

3There can also be <strong>in</strong>teraction between different sounds <strong>in</strong> the word, cf. analysis <strong>in</strong> 5.5.<br />

7


We also have a variation between some phonesthemic mean<strong>in</strong>g and none,<br />

e.g. we have no phonesthemic mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> klöver (clover). Certa<strong>in</strong><br />

consonant clusters are almost always phonesthemic, e.g. pj-, while others<br />

are less phonesthemic, e.g. tr-. This means that there is a variation <strong>in</strong> the<br />

strength of the connection between content and expression <strong>in</strong><br />

phonesthemes, a quantitative dimension of the morphology.<br />

Consequently, there are arguments for and aga<strong>in</strong>st whether phonesthemes<br />

are morphemes or not. The mean<strong>in</strong>g of the concept morpheme is vague,<br />

but I suggest that phonesthemes belong to morphology. It is one type of<br />

morpheme, a type which is special on the semiotic and semantic side: the<br />

relation to what is denoted is often iconic or <strong>in</strong>dexical and what is denoted<br />

are often sounds, experiences of the senses, emotions, etc.<br />

Phonesthemes are also special <strong>in</strong> that they have a low degree of autonomy.<br />

They are bound morphemes which often can be analytically dist<strong>in</strong>guished<br />

<strong>in</strong> a fashion similar to free morphemes. They will be referred to as<br />

phonesthemes and regarded as a type of morpheme which, because of<br />

their dependent and motivated nature, perhaps could rather be called<br />

submorphemes.<br />

1.2.6 Conclusion<br />

Above, there has been a survey of different terms and usages.<br />

Subsequently, the term onomatopoeia will be used to mean all k<strong>in</strong>ds of<br />

sound imitation, phonestheme will be used to mean bound submorphemic<br />

(cf. above) str<strong>in</strong>gs (e.g. consonant clusters) which have <strong>in</strong> common a<br />

certa<strong>in</strong> element of mean<strong>in</strong>g or function. The relation between sound and<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g is often iconic or <strong>in</strong>dexical, as well as symbolic. The term sound<br />

symbolism will be used for the general phenomenon of motivated<br />

relations between sound and mean<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g onomatopoeia. Thus<br />

sound symbolism, <strong>in</strong> a sense, is used almost oppositely to the sense of<br />

symbolism that was suggested by Peirce, i. e. it focuses on what he called<br />

icons and <strong>in</strong>dexes but not symbols. Most of the words discussed also<br />

conta<strong>in</strong> a conventional arbitrary element which means we are deal<strong>in</strong>g with<br />

what could be called iconic and <strong>in</strong>dexical symbols. (For further<br />

discussion, see chapter 2.)<br />

There also seems to be a further term<strong>in</strong>ological issue here, <strong>in</strong> that sound<br />

symbolism tends to be reserved for universal phenomena and<br />

8


phonesthemes for language specific phenomena. I would, however, prefer<br />

to use sound symbolism as a more general term and use the term universal<br />

sound symbolism when this is the issue.<br />

The relationship between the most important terms discussed here can be<br />

illustrated <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g way (figure 1.2).<br />

sound symbolism or<br />

motivated expressions<br />

onomatopoeia other sound symbolism<br />

phonesthemes<br />

free morphemes<br />

phonesthemes<br />

9<br />

free morphemes<br />

Figure 1.2. The relationship between some of the most important terms<br />

discussed <strong>in</strong> 1.2.<br />

1.3. Is sound symbolism the rule or the exception<br />

<strong>in</strong> language?<br />

1.3.1 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is an exception<br />

Saussure (1916) who held the view that l<strong>in</strong>guistic signs are arbitrary<br />

wrote that ‘‘onomatopoeic words are never organic elements of a<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic system’’. Bühler (1933/1969) said that onomatopoeia is a<br />

reversion, s<strong>in</strong>ce language has evolved beyond primitive needs and means<br />

of self-expression. The genesis of language is a measure of its success at<br />

arbitrary symbolic (i. e. not what I call sound symbolic) representation.<br />

1.3.2 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is fundamental to language<br />

Already von der Gabelentz (1891) wrote about the ‘‘sound symbolic<br />

feel<strong>in</strong>g’’, the experience that sound and mean<strong>in</strong>g are <strong>in</strong>alienably<br />

<strong>in</strong>terconnected for the naive members of the speech community. For


example, naive Germans would say ‘‘that Frenchmen are silly when they<br />

name e<strong>in</strong> Pferd Schewall’’ (Jakobson and Waugh, p. 182).<br />

An opposite view was held by Saussure's contemporary Jespersen (1922<br />

a), who went as far as to claim that ‘‘languages <strong>in</strong> the course of time grow<br />

richer and richer <strong>in</strong> symbolic’’ (that is sound symbolic) ‘‘words’’ and<br />

‘‘develop towards a greater number of easy and adequate expressions –<br />

expressions <strong>in</strong> which sound and sense are united <strong>in</strong> a marriage-union<br />

closer than was ever known to our remote ancestors’’.<br />

To the extent that African so called ideophones are discussed (by e.g.<br />

Samar<strong>in</strong>, 1978 4 ) these are usually considered part of language proper.<br />

One might question if this is because ideophones play a much more central<br />

role <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> African languages than <strong>in</strong> European languages, or whether<br />

there is another, freer tradition <strong>in</strong> describ<strong>in</strong>g non-European and primarily<br />

spoken languages. Diffloth (1976) writes that <strong>in</strong> describ<strong>in</strong>g languages with<br />

a structure remote from ones own language, it is often self-evident to<br />

segment roots <strong>in</strong>to smaller, significant units with their own sound<br />

symbolic value, thereby touch<strong>in</strong>g on the subject of how grammatical<br />

tradition may obscure <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g phenomena <strong>in</strong> a language.<br />

Lakoff and Johnson (1980), show<strong>in</strong>g many examples, claim that <strong>in</strong> all of<br />

language, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g syntax, there are many cases of non-arbitrary<br />

relations between form and mean<strong>in</strong>g. This seems to po<strong>in</strong>t to a view of<br />

non-arbitrar<strong>in</strong>ess be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tegrated <strong>in</strong> language and not <strong>in</strong> a separate<br />

category.<br />

A phenomenon <strong>in</strong> Swedish which is similar to ideophones (cf. 1.2.4) are<br />

the ‘‘sound words’’ of adolescent language, described by Nordberg<br />

(1986), e.g. krch, ppff, då<strong>in</strong>g. These are usually not considered to belong<br />

to the core of Swedish, if described at all. However, they show <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g<br />

similarities to e.g. the ideophones <strong>in</strong> Gbeya (a dialect of Gbaya, spoken <strong>in</strong><br />

Central Africa), (Samar<strong>in</strong>, 1978); they show reduplication, lengthen<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

phonotactic ‘‘divergencies’’ and sentence f<strong>in</strong>al position. The greatest<br />

4<br />

Samar<strong>in</strong> def<strong>in</strong>es ideophony as ‘‘the foreground<strong>in</strong>g of phonological elements <strong>in</strong> word (or<br />

lexeme) composition <strong>in</strong> both spontaneous creations and fully <strong>in</strong>stitutionalized lexicons,<br />

usually associated with semantic categories of an attributive nature, commonly affective,<br />

and sometimes also <strong>in</strong> true onomatopoeia’’<br />

10


difference to Gbeyan seems to be on the semantic side; Gbeyan words<br />

seem to have more specific mean<strong>in</strong>gs, that can be assigned dictionary-type<br />

def<strong>in</strong>itions, e.g. ndadak ndadak ‘‘(wood that) doesn’t split well <strong>in</strong><br />

chopp<strong>in</strong>g’’.<br />

1.3.3 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is both <strong>in</strong>side and outside<br />

of language<br />

Householder (1946), writes that the vocabulary of English falls <strong>in</strong>to three<br />

parts with regard to arbitrar<strong>in</strong>ess of structure: 1) those clearly and<br />

completely arbitrary, their mean<strong>in</strong>g unaffected by the sound, 2) those<br />

made up, <strong>in</strong> whole or part, of phonesthemes, 3) those belong<strong>in</strong>g primarily<br />

to the first group but with their mean<strong>in</strong>g colored or altered <strong>in</strong> vary<strong>in</strong>g<br />

degree by secondary association with phonesthemes.<br />

In the Oxford Dictionary of English Etymology (1967) the consonant wr -<br />

is a separate entry mean<strong>in</strong>g ‘‘consonant comb<strong>in</strong>ation occurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>itially <strong>in</strong><br />

many words imply<strong>in</strong>g twist<strong>in</strong>g or distortion’’. The dictionary further<br />

states that the mean<strong>in</strong>g of twist<strong>in</strong>g has many correspondences <strong>in</strong> other<br />

Germanic languages5. Wescott (1975) uses the term ‘‘microlanguage’’ for the core of language,<br />

which is subject to well-known grammatical rules – i.e. conventional<br />

language. For other doma<strong>in</strong>s of speech, e.g. baby talk, exclamations,<br />

verbal art – ‘‘language that is alienated from conventionally structured<br />

speech’’, he co<strong>in</strong>s the cover term ‘‘allolanguage’’. He says, ‘‘one of the<br />

characteristics of allolanguage is a closer relation between sound and sense<br />

than obta<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> microlanguage’’, ‘‘a retention ... of an older and simpler<br />

manner of self expression alongside one that is more recent and<br />

complex.’’ <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism, be<strong>in</strong>g one aspect of allolanguage, is thus<br />

part of language, but on the periphery, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Wescott.<br />

<strong>Sound</strong>s can enter a language by means of sound symbolic words (e.g.<br />

H<strong>in</strong>ton, 1986). Also, sound changes often do not affect sound symbolic<br />

words, so that phonemes that should have disappeared <strong>in</strong> a language or<br />

have become restricted to certa<strong>in</strong> environments are still to be found <strong>in</strong><br />

sound symbolic vocabulary (as po<strong>in</strong>ted out by H<strong>in</strong>ton, Nichols, Ohala<br />

5Cf. the Swedish clusters kr- and sn-, <strong>in</strong> chapter 4.<br />

11


1994). E. g. <strong>in</strong>itial r- is rare <strong>in</strong> the non sound-symbolic vocabulary of<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish (Austerlitz, 1994).<br />

There are also suggestions of tendencies to use a more reduced phonemic<br />

<strong>in</strong>ventory <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism (e.g. Oswalt 1994).<br />

1.3.4 Evaluation of discussion of sound symbolism<br />

The view of the difference between sound symbolic words and other<br />

words assumed <strong>in</strong> this thesis is the follow<strong>in</strong>g: For most words, the<br />

ord<strong>in</strong>ary speaker will, on reflection, agree that there is no motivation for<br />

them (e.g. for horse) but he/she will say that there is a motivation for<br />

sound symbolic words. Of course there will be a border area where<br />

different speakers will disagree or f<strong>in</strong>d it hard to judge.<br />

Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1950) takes a radical methodological approach to morphology<br />

<strong>in</strong> general, with phonesthemes (which are also called affective morphemes<br />

<strong>in</strong> contrast to neutral morphemes, <strong>in</strong> spite of the impossibility of stat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

where the neutral ends and the affective beg<strong>in</strong>s) as one type of possible<br />

output from a strictly synchronic analysis. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Bol<strong>in</strong>ger, the<br />

alternative approach of tak<strong>in</strong>g etymology <strong>in</strong>to account is not available<br />

s<strong>in</strong>ce ord<strong>in</strong>ary, naive speakers' judgements are important for discover<strong>in</strong>g<br />

morphemes. Bol<strong>in</strong>ger suggests the existence of numerous phonesthemes<br />

but also claims that they are too fluid to be penned with limits. One<br />

possible way to evaluate morphemes, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Bol<strong>in</strong>ger, would be<br />

through lexical and textual frequencies (cf. chapter 4). Bol<strong>in</strong>ger claims<br />

that roughly half of the words, <strong>in</strong> English, beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with gl- have the<br />

implication ‘‘visual’’. Bol<strong>in</strong>ger claims that as percentages go this is better<br />

than some of the paradigmatic suffixes (with regard to a constant<br />

association of mean<strong>in</strong>g and form), though of course gl- is never more<br />

than sporadically productive.<br />

1.4 Is sound symbolism productive or not?<br />

1.4.1 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is not productive<br />

Samar<strong>in</strong> (1978), <strong>in</strong> his studies of Gbeyan, has not found support for the<br />

creation of new ideophones (cf. 1.2.4). In traditional etymology the<br />

explanation of new co<strong>in</strong>ages is often just by ‘‘analogy’’ with one other<br />

word (which implies non-productivity).<br />

12


1.4.2 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is productive<br />

Most of the above mentioned l<strong>in</strong>guists who are specifically <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong><br />

the phenomenon and who view it as an <strong>in</strong>tegral part of language, also<br />

regard it as productive. For example, Nordberg (1986) who studied sound<br />

words, deals almost exclusively with new co<strong>in</strong>ages.<br />

1.4.3 Greater or lesser degree of productivity<br />

Rhodes (1994) discusses onomatopoeia, aural images (mapp<strong>in</strong>g sound onto<br />

sound) and forms based on aural images. He dist<strong>in</strong>guishes between ‘‘wild’’<br />

and ‘‘tame’’ words, these be<strong>in</strong>g the ends of a scale. ‘‘At the extreme wild<br />

end the possibilities of the human vocal tract are utilized to their fullest to<br />

imitate sounds of other than human orig<strong>in</strong>. At the tame end the imitated<br />

sound is simply approximated by an acoustically close phoneme or<br />

phoneme comb<strong>in</strong>ation.’’<br />

Bol<strong>in</strong>ger does an assonance-rime analysis of English monosyllables (cf.<br />

Bol<strong>in</strong>ger 1950) where the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant(s) constitute the assonance and<br />

the rema<strong>in</strong>der of the syllable is the rime. He argues that assonance-rime<br />

analysis (of tame words) is morphology because assonances and rimes do<br />

not comb<strong>in</strong>e productively. That, however, does not mean that a<br />

construction is frozen. He <strong>in</strong>troduces the term ‘‘active’’ for constructions<br />

that produce monosyllables cont<strong>in</strong>uously, at a slow rate.<br />

1.4.4 Evaluation of the discussion of productivity<br />

The hypothesis that will be tested <strong>in</strong> the experiments is that phonesthemes<br />

are productive to a greater or lesser degree, i.e. that some phonesthemes<br />

are more productive than others. The <strong>in</strong>termittent occurrence of new<br />

forms <strong>in</strong> speech, prose and fiction, (which fit <strong>in</strong>to a pattern), especially <strong>in</strong><br />

child literature, constitutes an argument for this claim. The opposite view<br />

would mean that new co<strong>in</strong>ages would be phonetically and semantically<br />

haphazard. However, with that view, the fairly wide-spread and easy<br />

comprehension of new forms would be difficult to account for. The<br />

concept of phonestheme <strong>in</strong>volves stronger or weaker productivity.<br />

The dist<strong>in</strong>ction between understand<strong>in</strong>g and production might also be<br />

fruitful. When be<strong>in</strong>g presented with deliberately constructed nonsense<br />

words <strong>in</strong> the experiments of this thesis, listeners have no objections to or<br />

difficulties <strong>in</strong> assign<strong>in</strong>g some <strong>in</strong>terpretation to them (cf. 7.1.3).<br />

13


Another problem, <strong>in</strong> this area of research is to decide where the<br />

borderl<strong>in</strong>e goes between lexicalized and more temporary, newly created,<br />

forms. In other words, what I experience as a neologism can be an<br />

established word <strong>in</strong> a subgroup or an (ext<strong>in</strong>ct) dialectal word. 6<br />

1.5 The question of etymology<br />

In an etymological perspective this part of the vocabulary is less static <strong>in</strong><br />

one aspect, more static <strong>in</strong> another; onomatopoeic words are constantly<br />

recreated, but this also makes them keep much the same form throughout<br />

the ages, as they don't always undergo general changes of sound and<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g Jespersen (1922 a) gives the example of cuckoo which has not<br />

changed its vowel from [u] to [U], as <strong>in</strong> cut, but is pronounced [kuku]. An<br />

onomatopoeic word is constra<strong>in</strong>ed by the sound it imitates.<br />

In account<strong>in</strong>g for onomatopoeic and other sound symbolic expressions, a<br />

synchronic explanation will be given, irrespective of whether the<br />

explanation is historically ‘‘true’’ or ‘‘false’’. Some examples will be given<br />

of what some critics of classical etymology have stated. First, however, it<br />

should be po<strong>in</strong>ted out that there seems to be a general agreement that<br />

onomatopoeic (and also sound symbolic) words usually do not undergo<br />

the same phonological changes as other words, e.g. accord<strong>in</strong>g to Grimm's<br />

law, as long as they still have a sound imitative mean<strong>in</strong>g. They constantly<br />

get renewed and sometimes reshaped by fresh imitation. Wescott (1975)<br />

writes: ‘‘Thus, the reconstructed proto-<strong>in</strong>doeuropean forms *pap- ‘‘teat’’,<br />

*tut- ‘‘to hoot’’ and *kuku- ‘‘cuckoo’’, appear <strong>in</strong> English as pap, toot, and<br />

cuckoo rather than, as comparative philologists would normally predict,<br />

*faf, *thuth and *houhg.’’<br />

6<br />

Exist<strong>in</strong>g forms, like place-names, can also be given a (new) mean<strong>in</strong>g, befitt<strong>in</strong>g their<br />

form, as <strong>in</strong> the book ‘‘The mean<strong>in</strong>g of Liff’’ (Adams & LLoyd, 1983), which states that<br />

‘‘In life, there are many hundreds of common experiences, feel<strong>in</strong>gs, situations and even<br />

objects which we all know and recognize, but for which no words exist. On the other<br />

hand, the world is littered with thousands of spare words, which spend their time do<strong>in</strong>g<br />

noth<strong>in</strong>g but loaf<strong>in</strong>g on signposts po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g at places...’’ These names and mean<strong>in</strong>gs are<br />

then paired like e.g., Cranleigh: a mood of irrational irritation with everyone and<br />

everyth<strong>in</strong>g, Burbage: The sound made by a lift full of people all try<strong>in</strong>g to breathe politely<br />

through their noses, or Plymouth: to relate an amus<strong>in</strong>g story to someone without<br />

remember<strong>in</strong>g that it was they who told it to you <strong>in</strong> the first place.<br />

14


On the other hand, Jespersen (1922 a) also remarks that ‘‘words that have<br />

been symbolically expressive may cease to be so <strong>in</strong> consequence of<br />

historical development, either phonetic, semantic or both.’’ An example,<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to Jespersen, is the word crow which is not now so good an<br />

imitation of the sound made by the bird as OE crawe was. And wh<strong>in</strong>e,<br />

pipe were better imitations when the vowel was still i as <strong>in</strong> Danish hv<strong>in</strong>e,<br />

pibe. But the sound made by a small bird is still pronounced with an i <strong>in</strong><br />

peep.<br />

Furthermore, writes Jespersen, some words have <strong>in</strong> the course of time<br />

become more expressive than they were at first. This phenomenon he calls<br />

secondary echoism or secondary symbolism. Patter ‘‘to talk rapidly or<br />

glibly’’ is to all <strong>in</strong>tents a truly symbolical word, even though it comes<br />

from pater (paternoster) and at first meant to repeat that prayer.<br />

An <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g example <strong>in</strong> Swedish is (atjo) prosit as a polite reaction to a<br />

sneeze, of which the latter word is probably apprehended as<br />

onomatopoeic, at least by children. (The word atjo is of course motivated.<br />

It is a conventionalization of a sound from a bodily reaction and the<br />

relation between the sound and the mean<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>in</strong>dexical (cf. the crossl<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

comparison <strong>in</strong> chapter 5). The Lat<strong>in</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>al mean<strong>in</strong>g of prosit<br />

(which is ‘‘måtte det gagna’’ (may it be of use)) is probably quite dead <strong>in</strong><br />

the mental lexicons of most speakers. On the other hand, accord<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

Jespersen (1922 a), there are words which we feel <strong>in</strong>st<strong>in</strong>ctively to be<br />

adequate to express the ideas they stand for and others, the sound of which<br />

are felt to be more or less <strong>in</strong>congruous with their signification. These<br />

feel<strong>in</strong>gs of adequacy or <strong>in</strong>congruity are both examples of etymological<br />

creativity. A Swedish example of <strong>in</strong>congruity is perhaps munter (joyful) 7.<br />

Jespersen (1922 a, b) talks about ‘‘symbolism at work’’ where both sound<br />

and sense fit. Also, he writes, through changes <strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g, too, some<br />

words have become more expressive than they were formerly. His<br />

example is m<strong>in</strong>iature, which, because of the i, has come to mean ‘‘a small<br />

picture’’ <strong>in</strong>stead of ‘‘image pa<strong>in</strong>ted with a m<strong>in</strong>imum of vermilion’’.<br />

Jespersen claims that ‘‘sound symbolism makes some words more fit to<br />

survive’’. The word roll, <strong>in</strong> French rouler, etc. derived from Lat<strong>in</strong> rota +<br />

7Because of the [P], cf. 5.4.3.<br />

15


dim<strong>in</strong>utive end<strong>in</strong>g -ul- ga<strong>in</strong>ed its popularity <strong>in</strong> English, Dutch, German,<br />

and Scand<strong>in</strong>avian languages, because the sound is suggestive of the sense.<br />

He also talks of sound symbolism be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> action when borrow<strong>in</strong>g words<br />

from other languages and when co<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g nouns, verbs, etc. from (place)<br />

names. To sum up then, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Jespersen, onomatopoeia and other<br />

sound symbolism do not always date back to the earliest times, and it is<br />

mostly un<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to lay any aspects of ‘‘correct etymology’’ on words<br />

affected by onomatopoeia and sound symbolism.<br />

Wescott (1978) would make the same claim for most words, but especially<br />

for slang and proper names. He takes a polygenetic view of word orig<strong>in</strong>s<br />

and assumes that ‘‘lexical ancestry is relative rather than absolute <strong>in</strong><br />

nature’’ and that because of this derivational relativity ‘‘there is a gradual<br />

‘fade-out’ <strong>in</strong> the etymological antecedence of any lexeme and that this<br />

fade-out effect, <strong>in</strong> turn, leads <strong>in</strong>escapably to subjectivity <strong>in</strong> the assessment<br />

of degrees of lexical ancestry’’. Of the various processes that contribute to<br />

the development of polygenetic lexemes are, e.g. sound repetition,<br />

<strong>in</strong>different varieties, elision or ‘‘chopp<strong>in</strong>g’’, the conversion of spoken<br />

language <strong>in</strong>to written language and the consequent feedback effect of<br />

writ<strong>in</strong>g on speech. 8<br />

Von der Gabelentz (1891) observes historically ‘‘false’’ but synchronically<br />

‘‘true’’ etymologies based on collective agreement with<strong>in</strong> a given speech<br />

community. He writes that words l<strong>in</strong>ked together by both sound and<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g manifest ‘‘elective aff<strong>in</strong>ities’’ (wahlverwandtschaften) able to<br />

modify both the shape and the content of the words <strong>in</strong>volved. Here, it is<br />

natural to refer to Householder's (1946) def<strong>in</strong>ition of a phonestheme: ‘‘a<br />

phoneme or cluster of phonemes shared by a group of words which also<br />

have <strong>in</strong> common some element of mean<strong>in</strong>g or function, though the words<br />

may be etymologically unrelated.’’ (Traditionally, words are said to be<br />

etymologically related if they can be traced back to the same word, but<br />

not if they conta<strong>in</strong> the same phonestheme, as a part of words. It would<br />

probably be fruitful for etymological study to explore the concept of<br />

8<br />

There is another k<strong>in</strong>d of etymology, namely folk etymology, which I believe is an<br />

expression for the same mental process as <strong>in</strong> productive sound symbolism. There seems<br />

to exist a human <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ation for motivated signs. Instead of us<strong>in</strong>g sounds that fit the<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g, exist<strong>in</strong>g words are reshaped through us<strong>in</strong>g other but similar-sound<strong>in</strong>g<br />

morphemes to make the result<strong>in</strong>g word more morphologically transparent.<br />

16


phonestheme, i.e. sound symbolic l<strong>in</strong>ks could be just as valid as other<br />

morphological l<strong>in</strong>ks.)<br />

Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1968) also holds the view that traditional etymology is not<br />

relevant for expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g phonesthemes (for methodological reasons, cf.<br />

above). He also describes phonesthemes like this: ‘‘Given a particular<br />

word for a particular th<strong>in</strong>g, if other words for similar th<strong>in</strong>gs come to<br />

resemble that word <strong>in</strong> sound, then, no matter how arbitrary the<br />

relationship between sound and sense was to beg<strong>in</strong> with, the sense is now<br />

obviously tied to the sound. The relationship between sound and sense is<br />

still arbitrary, as far as the outside world is concerned (and would appear<br />

that way absolutely to a foreigner), but with<strong>in</strong> the system it is no longer<br />

so’’ (p. 242). Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1950) also argues that when two expressions are<br />

encountered <strong>in</strong> the same area of greater or lesser specificity of mean<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

and are also similar <strong>in</strong> form, they are likely to exercise a k<strong>in</strong>d of magnetic<br />

attraction one upon the other. The attraction may be extremely remote.<br />

Most speakers of English, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1950), when they hear<br />

ambush, are likely to th<strong>in</strong>k of someone hid<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the bushes. Likewise<br />

with hierarchy: one tends to hear the element higher. (This phenomenon<br />

could be called '<strong>in</strong>terpretive folk etymology'.) Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1950) is very<br />

consistent <strong>in</strong> his synchronic (and spoken language) approach, which<br />

sometimes leads to absurdities. His ma<strong>in</strong> arguments for the irrelevance of<br />

etymology is that it doesn't match speakers' judgements and that it isn't<br />

compatible with morpheme convergence and divergence.<br />

Malkiel (1994) discusses the role of phonosymbolic (i.e. sound symbolic)<br />

<strong>in</strong>terference <strong>in</strong> the sound development of words. He especially discusses<br />

the example of the older French word for close developed from Lat<strong>in</strong><br />

claudere be<strong>in</strong>g replaced (<strong>in</strong> Late Antiquity and the Middle Ages) by the<br />

word fermer under the <strong>in</strong>fluence of the word for iron (ferrum), a then<br />

newly <strong>in</strong>troduced, highly prestigious metal.<br />

It is sometimes po<strong>in</strong>ted out that every word has its own history, which is<br />

an anomalistic po<strong>in</strong>t of view (<strong>in</strong> the sense of antique Greek-Roman<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>ction of ‘‘anomaly vs. analogy’’). But one could also talk about<br />

different rule systems conflict<strong>in</strong>g. Malkiel, for example, refers to<br />

Meillet's (1931) discussion on phonetic m<strong>in</strong>i-systems expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

exceptional lexical units handed down from one generation to another.<br />

17


These m<strong>in</strong>i-systems could well have existed side by side with the basic<br />

phonological structure.<br />

Rhodes (1994) writes that the abundant etymological dictionary comments<br />

like ‘‘orig<strong>in</strong> uncerta<strong>in</strong>’’ or ‘‘prob. ak<strong>in</strong> to’’ for English monosyllables<br />

should conv<strong>in</strong>ce one that these are <strong>in</strong>novated cont<strong>in</strong>uously, but at a slow<br />

rate (active comb<strong>in</strong>ation, <strong>in</strong> contrast to productive and frozen). There is<br />

also, he writes, an ongo<strong>in</strong>g re<strong>in</strong>terpretation of forms of various historical<br />

sources as new <strong>in</strong>stances of assonances (<strong>in</strong>itial consonants). An example is<br />

the largely Germanic sl- liquid (slop, slush) classifier (a type of<br />

phonestheme) which also, semantically, <strong>in</strong>cludes the sl- <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>ate sluice<br />

(< Vulgar Lat<strong>in</strong> *exclusa), (Swe: sluss), <strong>in</strong> spite of different etymological<br />

orig<strong>in</strong>s.<br />

1.6 The phylogenesis of language<br />

From the question of etymology it is natural to go on to a related question<br />

<strong>in</strong> which onomatopoeia and sound symbolism have figured, namely the<br />

question of the phylogenesis of language.<br />

Plato, <strong>in</strong> the dialogue Cratylos, treats the contest between the two basic<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic forces, convention and nature. The argument concerned the<br />

nature of names; Cratylos meant that names were given by nature,<br />

Socrates that they were conventional.<br />

Most theories of the orig<strong>in</strong> of language have built on onomatopoeia, the<br />

‘‘bow-wow theory’’ (e.g. Herder, 1772) (cf. Müller, 1861) for the names<br />

‘‘bow-wow’’, etc) or on gestures, (e.g. Herman Paul, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Fano,<br />

1962). Other variants are ‘‘div<strong>in</strong>e orig<strong>in</strong>’’, (e.g. Süssmilch, 1767) or ‘‘the<br />

pooh-pooh theory’’ (e.g. Rousseau, 1822), which says that the orig<strong>in</strong> of<br />

language lies <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terjectional, emotional cries triggered by strong feel<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

of fear, pa<strong>in</strong>, lust, etc. The ‘‘yo-he-ho theory’’ says that <strong>in</strong> collective<br />

labor, rhythmically produced spoken sounds help coord<strong>in</strong>ate the actions of<br />

many <strong>in</strong>dividuals. ‘‘The s<strong>in</strong>g-song theory’’ (e.g. Jespersen, 1922 a) means<br />

that the orig<strong>in</strong> of language lies <strong>in</strong> dance, song and related expressive<br />

vocaliz<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Max Müller (1861), (who has co<strong>in</strong>ed humorous expressions) is a<br />

representative for the ‘‘d<strong>in</strong>g-dong theory’’, which might appear to give a<br />

sound symbolic explanation. It is, however, based on his reconstruction of<br />

18


400-500 Indo-European roots. He writes that man had an <strong>in</strong>nate<br />

<strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ation to associate certa<strong>in</strong> types of sound with certa<strong>in</strong> types of objects<br />

and actions which echoed <strong>in</strong> him <strong>in</strong> a way which is analogous to an<br />

object's resonance when struck. The reconstructed Indo-European roots<br />

are an outgrowth of the d<strong>in</strong>g-dong effect. Müller would not, as would e.g.<br />

Jespersen, say that sound symbolism is productive <strong>in</strong> language, but that<br />

this <strong>in</strong>st<strong>in</strong>ct to give articulate expression for rational concepts <strong>in</strong> the<br />

human m<strong>in</strong>d has disappeared s<strong>in</strong>ce there is no longer a need for it, once<br />

language is established.<br />

One theory of how spoken language has developed from gestural language<br />

is ‘‘the mouth-gesture theory’’ (Paget, 1930 and Jóhannesson, 1949). It<br />

says, briefly, that the organs of speech tend to move <strong>in</strong> unison with hand<br />

and arm movements when these are used <strong>in</strong> sign language or when us<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tools. If such movements of the speech organs are accompanied by<br />

vocalizations, then the result<strong>in</strong>g sounds (that are similar to sounds <strong>in</strong><br />

articulated speech) eventually get the same mean<strong>in</strong>g as the gestures. The<br />

gestures of the organs of articulation are recognized by the hearer because<br />

the hearer unconsciously reproduced <strong>in</strong> his m<strong>in</strong>d the actual gesture which<br />

had produced the sound.<br />

Jóhannesson (1949) presents material from both Indo-European and<br />

Semitic sources. In Indo-European languages, he claims that about 5% of<br />

the words could be attributed to <strong>in</strong>terjection, probably of emotion, and<br />

10% to onomatopoeia, leav<strong>in</strong>g 85% expla<strong>in</strong>able as direct or <strong>in</strong>direct<br />

derivations from mouth-gesture.<br />

Other theories of the orig<strong>in</strong> of language have focused more on why and<br />

less on how language appeared, e.g. because of social need (Révész, 1946)<br />

or cerebral development or as a consequence of early child language<br />

development. An example of the later is ‘‘the babble-luck theory’’<br />

(Thorndike, 1943), which is to be understood as a theory of the selective<br />

re<strong>in</strong>forcement of <strong>in</strong>itial, random babbl<strong>in</strong>g, which later became words.<br />

Thorndike has been criticized for not expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g how the parents of the<br />

earliest babblers were able to shape <strong>in</strong>fant vocalizations.<br />

Some l<strong>in</strong>guists (explicitly) believe <strong>in</strong> monogenesis, others <strong>in</strong> polygenesis<br />

of human language. For a much more extensive review on the subject, see<br />

Hewes (1977). I will not take a stand on these theories of the orig<strong>in</strong> of<br />

19


language, but suspect that the solution probably lies <strong>in</strong> a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of<br />

different theories, one of them be<strong>in</strong>g the bow-wow variant.<br />

The forego<strong>in</strong>g section leads up to the question of the role of onomatopoeia<br />

and sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> the ontogenesis of language. One of the more<br />

recent <strong>in</strong>vestigations, Williams (1991), concerns the phenomenon of [da]<br />

universally hav<strong>in</strong>g a deictic function <strong>in</strong> early language acquisition. She<br />

f<strong>in</strong>ds support for the mechanism of cross-modal transfer exist<strong>in</strong>g as a<br />

bridge to the acquisition of language and she holds that this can shed some<br />

light on sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> adult language. The cross-modal transfer<br />

doesn't disappear entirely once it has fulfilled its function, even though<br />

this aspect may later be regarded as superfluous. Without any other<br />

comparison this is similar to Müller's (1861) views on the genesis of<br />

language.<br />

1.7 Universality versus language specificity<br />

One important question, which is not always clearly accounted for, is that<br />

of the universality or language specificity of sound symbolism.<br />

Universality can be a feature of the semantic side, the expressive side or<br />

more abstractly.<br />

1.7.1 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is universal<br />

Samar<strong>in</strong> (e.g. 1978) writes that it is a universal fact that all languages use<br />

onomatopoeia and sound symbolism, but that the expressions differ. He<br />

does not make any claims for the semantic side, other than that all<br />

languages have an expressive function.<br />

Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1965) writes that the tendency of forms to mold themselves on<br />

other forms with like mean<strong>in</strong>gs and of mean<strong>in</strong>gs to mold themselves on<br />

other mean<strong>in</strong>gs, conveyed by like words, is universal. Allot (1973), us<strong>in</strong>g<br />

data from many languages, claims that there is a tendency for words that<br />

sound similar to have similar mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> languages not known to be<br />

historically related.<br />

The mean<strong>in</strong>g of [i] has been well studied, and claims have been made for<br />

its universality <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism (Ultan, 1978; Jespersen, 1933).<br />

There seems to be a general agreement that phonesthemes are language<br />

specific, a conclusion which is not necessary; besides, phonesthemes are<br />

20


often similar <strong>in</strong> related languages. A reasonable claim as concerns<br />

universality is that ‘‘the <strong>in</strong>tral<strong>in</strong>guistic variation recapitulates the<br />

<strong>in</strong>terl<strong>in</strong>guistic variation’’ (Allwood, 1985). Wescott (1975) handles the<br />

same phenomena by claim<strong>in</strong>g that what is not <strong>in</strong> the microlanguage<br />

(conventionally structured speech) is to be found <strong>in</strong> the allolanguage<br />

(speech which is alienated from conventionally structured speech e.g.<br />

<strong>in</strong>fantile babbl<strong>in</strong>g, non-grammatical <strong>in</strong>terjection, poetry and song). He<br />

claims universality <strong>in</strong> processes like reduplication and lengthen<strong>in</strong>g. Some<br />

experimental l<strong>in</strong>guists have explicitly tackled the question of universality<br />

by test<strong>in</strong>g people speak<strong>in</strong>g different languages, e.g. Osgood (1962) (see<br />

1.10.5).<br />

Allwood (1983) discusses language <strong>in</strong> general, about the relationship<br />

between language and thought and its connection with universal and<br />

relativistic standpo<strong>in</strong>ts. The conclusions relevant to onomatopoeia and<br />

other sound symbolism are, <strong>in</strong> brief, that universal traits <strong>in</strong> language<br />

mostly concern the semantic, not the expression side and that semantic<br />

universals are most likely <strong>in</strong> areas that are biologically grounded, e.g.<br />

motoric and perceptual activities. Examples are spatial relations and color<br />

terms.<br />

1.7.2 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is not universal<br />

Some l<strong>in</strong>guists seem to have taken the fact that there are differences<br />

between languages concern<strong>in</strong>g (presumptive) onomatopoeia and other<br />

sound symbolism as a proof of the non-existence of sound symbolism, on<br />

the grounds that if sound symbolism did exist, it should be universal <strong>in</strong><br />

form and content. This conclusion rests on some unarticulated<br />

assumptions concern<strong>in</strong>g the way <strong>in</strong> which sound symbolism ought to be<br />

accounted for, e. g. as <strong>in</strong>nate, cf. Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1968), ‘‘If there were a real<br />

connection between the sound of a word and its mean<strong>in</strong>g, a person who<br />

did not know the language would be able to guess the word if he knew the<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g and guess the mean<strong>in</strong>g if he heard the word. This almost never<br />

happens, even with words that imitate sounds’’.<br />

Austerlitz (1994) has studied vowels <strong>in</strong> e.g. F<strong>in</strong>nish and suggests that there<br />

is a language-specific correlation between recentness and exploitability <strong>in</strong><br />

sound symbolism. In F<strong>in</strong>nish the vowels (and the consonants) are<br />

unequally rooted <strong>in</strong> the system. The vowel ö /ø/ is the most recent arrival<br />

21


(and is secondary, and marked, as all front rounded vowels are) <strong>in</strong> the<br />

sound system and it is the vowel which is the most sound symbolic.<br />

1.7.3 Evaluation of the discussion of universality<br />

versus language specificity <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism<br />

An important question is whether phonesthemes are language specific, as<br />

Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1950) claims. If so, then the orig<strong>in</strong> of each phonestheme could<br />

be attributed solely to chance, a ‘‘cluster<strong>in</strong>g effect’’ (which would be<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> itself). Another possibility is that phonesthemes show<br />

universal tendencies, even if ‘‘only’’ <strong>in</strong> cases concern<strong>in</strong>g semantic<br />

features. If they show universal tendencies, the explanation could partly<br />

have to do with <strong>in</strong>nateness, and partly <strong>in</strong>volve a basic relationship between<br />

environment and <strong>in</strong>dividual.<br />

But it may be that, <strong>in</strong>stead of the dichotomy between certa<strong>in</strong> sounds which<br />

are universal and certa<strong>in</strong> phonesthemes which are language specific, one<br />

should dist<strong>in</strong>guish between (a) universal sound symbolism perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g to<br />

certa<strong>in</strong> sounds and sound clusters and (b) the phenomenon of<br />

‘‘phonesthemicity’’, i. e. the tendency of certa<strong>in</strong> sound-mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ations to mold themselves on other comb<strong>in</strong>ations, perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g to all<br />

sounds and clusters, etc. In the latter case the result could well be<br />

language specific while the thrust for analogy itself is universal. The next<br />

question is if the cluster<strong>in</strong>g effect is enhanced <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> types of mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

or sound. There might also be a frequency effect emanat<strong>in</strong>g from<br />

syntagmatic context.<br />

If the semantic-phonetic relationships of motivated words could be<br />

analytically treated one by one, my assumption is that the existence of<br />

universality <strong>in</strong> phonesthemes on the phonetic side (i. e. that e.g. imitation<br />

of ‘‘wet sounds’’ is done with the same speech sounds <strong>in</strong> different<br />

languages) is most likely at a level of (comb<strong>in</strong>ations of) dist<strong>in</strong>ctive<br />

features, e.g. voiceless, fricative, etc.<br />

On the semantic/functional side phonesthemes are probably partly<br />

universal and partly language specific. The result here probably depends,<br />

to a great extent, on how abstract the semantic categorization is. Also,<br />

some semantic fields are more likely to conta<strong>in</strong> sound symbolism, e.g.<br />

[DIMINUTIVE].<br />

22


Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Allwood, as referred to above (1983), the universals <strong>in</strong><br />

general to be expected, viz. universals of content are likely to be related<br />

to perception and perhaps to motoric behavior. This proposal does not,<br />

however, expla<strong>in</strong> all semantic categories <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism.<br />

Whorf (1956) has an argument for universals of content: ‘‘<strong>in</strong> the<br />

psychological experiments, human subjects seem to associate the<br />

experiences of bright, cold, sharp, hard, high, light (<strong>in</strong> weight), quick,<br />

high-pitched, narrow, and so on <strong>in</strong> a long series, with each other; and<br />

conversely, the experiences of dark, warm, yield<strong>in</strong>g, soft, blunt, low,<br />

heavy, slow, low-pitched, wide, etc. <strong>in</strong> another long series. This occurs<br />

whether the WORDS for such associated experiences resemble them or<br />

not, but the ord<strong>in</strong>ary person is likely to NOTICE a relation to words only<br />

when it is a relation of likenesses to such a series <strong>in</strong> the vowels and<br />

consonants of words’’ (p. 267 f).<br />

It is not always clear if the authors mentioned above have discussed<br />

understand<strong>in</strong>g, production or both. It is most clear <strong>in</strong> the experiments<br />

conducted, cf. 1.10.5).<br />

1.8 Context<br />

Words (and parts of them) are always perceived <strong>in</strong> a context, which<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluences their <strong>in</strong>terpretation. The types of context that are <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g<br />

for sound symbolism are:<br />

i) phonetic/phonological and semantic<br />

ii) only semantic<br />

iii) situational<br />

The <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the mean<strong>in</strong>g connected with e.g. a consonant cluster<br />

can thus be <strong>in</strong>fluenced by phonetic/phonological and semantic<br />

(phonesthemic) context e.g. other consonants, vowels, <strong>in</strong>tonation, etc.<br />

connected with a certa<strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The <strong>in</strong>terpretation can also be <strong>in</strong>fluenced (e.g. disambiguated) by semantic<br />

context only. For example, a consonant cluster can have as a weak<br />

phonestheme mean<strong>in</strong>g 'wetness'; the cluster pl-, which is otherwise<br />

ma<strong>in</strong>ly 'pejorative', can also mean 'wetness'. In the context of other words<br />

23


hav<strong>in</strong>g to do with wetness (and e.g. a word like r<strong>in</strong>na (flow)) a neologism<br />

beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with pl- could adopt a mean<strong>in</strong>g of wetness.<br />

The disambiguation of a consonant cluster can also depend on the speech<br />

situation <strong>in</strong> which e.g. a neologism or an ambiguous word is uttered, as<br />

for all words. An example of an ambiguous word is klabb (‘‘wet snow’’<br />

or ‘‘chunk of wood’’). The cluster kl- can mean 'wetness' (wet snow) or<br />

'short wide form' (chunk of wood).<br />

It is probable that at least some motivated words, e.g. the sound words of<br />

teenagers (like tssccht <strong>in</strong> ‘‘she wears her hair like this : tssccht’’), are<br />

more dependent on situational context than more arbitrary lexical<br />

morphemes; a person, who was not present <strong>in</strong> the situation were the word<br />

‘‘tssccht’’ was uttered, cannot understand exactly how the girl's hair<br />

looked.<br />

Grammont (1933), who was ma<strong>in</strong>ly <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the evocative value of<br />

vowels, especially <strong>in</strong> word forms reduplicated with a vowel change, <strong>in</strong><br />

different languages (e.g. ritsch-ratsch, piff-paff-puff) claimed that the<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g of a vowel manifests itself when it is prompted by the mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of the text or when it at least does not stand <strong>in</strong> contradiction to it. The<br />

degree depends on the subjectivity of speakers and listeners as well as on<br />

situations, e.g. affective speech and poetry be<strong>in</strong>g favorable.<br />

As will be seen <strong>in</strong> chapter 7, the experiments <strong>in</strong> the present work are<br />

ma<strong>in</strong>ly done without variation of context. The reason for this is the desire<br />

to know how much (if any) of the sound symbolic mean<strong>in</strong>g is conveyed by<br />

certa<strong>in</strong> consonant clusters themselves, without context consist<strong>in</strong>g of<br />

vowels, other consonants, other words, <strong>in</strong>tonation, gestures, etc.<br />

However, <strong>in</strong> these experiments situational context is present from another<br />

aspect. As is shown <strong>in</strong> chapter 4, some consonant clusters have a higher<br />

frequency of motivated words with a certa<strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g while other<br />

consonant clusters are less dom<strong>in</strong>ated by motivated words. This is often<br />

reflected <strong>in</strong> the results of the tests described <strong>in</strong> chapter 7. It is reasonable<br />

to believe that neologisms beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with not so clearly profiled clusters<br />

are more dependent on the l<strong>in</strong>guistic or extral<strong>in</strong>guistic context for their<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation.<br />

24


An example of phonetic/phonological and semantic (phonesthemic)<br />

context could be an <strong>in</strong>vented name like Pjäfser which isn't very attractive<br />

because of the pejorative pj- and the pejorative -fs (like <strong>in</strong> hafs, slafs,<br />

tjafs, krafs, rufs, bjäfs). These two clusters make the weakly pejorative -<br />

E- come to life and add to the pejorative impression. (In addition to this<br />

the suffix -er also has a pejorative nuance.)<br />

The cluster fl-, which ma<strong>in</strong>ly means 'quick movement' also has the<br />

phonestheme mean<strong>in</strong>g 'pejorative' (and few others). In a neologism like<br />

flafs it is likely that the whole word will be <strong>in</strong>terpreted as a pejorative<br />

because of the pejorative end<strong>in</strong>g -fs. The less common pejorative mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of fl- is activated because of the phonological/semantic context of -fs.<br />

From this we can see that words with sound symbolism are neither more<br />

context dependent nor less context dependent than other words. Certa<strong>in</strong><br />

clusters are more context dependent, while other are less context<br />

dependent, depend<strong>in</strong>g on the lexical sound symbolic strength of the cluster<br />

(cf. chapter 4).<br />

While the cluster pj- is mostly pejorative, another clearly sound symbolic<br />

cluster kl- carries several mean<strong>in</strong>gs, i.e. 'sound', 'talk<strong>in</strong>g', 'pejorative',<br />

'wetness', 'adhesion' and 'short wide form'. What determ<strong>in</strong>es the mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of a neologism beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with kl- must be either phonesthemic, other<br />

semantic or situational context. Look<strong>in</strong>g at the examples <strong>in</strong> Appendix 1<br />

there seems to be a tendency that kl- words mean<strong>in</strong>g 'short wide form'<br />

are monosyllabic and end<strong>in</strong>g with a gem<strong>in</strong>ate consonant. They do not end<br />

with fricative clusters; these appear here to be reserved for 'sound' and<br />

'wetness' mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Phonesthemic context seems to be important but it is<br />

not possible, at this stage, to give rules for this. (Cf. the discussion <strong>in</strong><br />

Allwood (1982) about mean<strong>in</strong>g potential and contextual conditions for<br />

different mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Cf. also 5.6.)<br />

The <strong>in</strong>fluence of context expla<strong>in</strong>s why the same sound (sequence) mostly<br />

unambiguously can appear <strong>in</strong> both sound symbolic words (as<br />

phonesthemes) and <strong>in</strong> non-sound symbolic words (as just a phoneme<br />

sequence).<br />

Nerman (1954) makes an <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g analysis of the sound symbolism of<br />

vowels and consonants <strong>in</strong> Swedish poetry. He writes that they always stand<br />

25


<strong>in</strong> a context, and really the whole poem, or at least the stanza, ought to be<br />

quoted for every example and that all means conspire: content, rhythm<br />

and r<strong>in</strong>g. However, his analysis is not preceded by a lexical<br />

(paradigmatic) analysis.<br />

1.9 A framework for models of sound symbolism<br />

Models of onomatopoeia and sound symbolism must take such factors <strong>in</strong>to<br />

account as discussed <strong>in</strong> 1.3-1.8. One factor is the position of onomatopoeia<br />

and other sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> language, (i. e. <strong>in</strong> grammar and lexicon).<br />

Are they primarily central or somewhere on the periphery of language? I<br />

claim that onomatopoeia and sound symbolism are central and <strong>in</strong>side<br />

language and a part of morphology.<br />

However, some newly created motivated expressions (like the sound<br />

words of teenagers, e.g. krch, ppff, då<strong>in</strong>g) seem to be more context<br />

dependent than most established adjectives, nouns and verbs and therefore<br />

on the periphery of language - words are created <strong>in</strong> subgroups and are<br />

used for a limited time (cf. Kots<strong>in</strong>as, 1994). The study of sound<br />

symbolism naturally leads to the study of processes of language<br />

development: How do words and sounds enter language or disappear from<br />

language?<br />

Phonesthemes vary <strong>in</strong> regard to productivity. The question of<br />

productivity also <strong>in</strong>volves the issue of production of new expressions vs.<br />

understand<strong>in</strong>g of such expressions. <strong>Sound</strong> symbolic neologisms are not<br />

created all the time, but are created now and then. But when they are<br />

created, they are easily understood by the listener; nonsense words can be<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpreted almost immediately.<br />

Onomatopoeia and other sound symbolism are probably driv<strong>in</strong>g forces <strong>in</strong><br />

etymological development (and some claim they were so <strong>in</strong> the genesis of<br />

language).<br />

The general framework has to <strong>in</strong>clude a view of language as a dynamic<br />

phenomenon with components of different dignity for different functions.<br />

<strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is always possibly present, but new expressions (words<br />

or phonesthemes) can be seen as float<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> and out of language. Whether<br />

or not they are actually ‘‘<strong>in</strong> language’’ depends on situation, stylistic<br />

context, time span under study and geographical area studied.<br />

26


The description and explanation of sound symbolic phenomena is related<br />

to the question of universality. If there are universals <strong>in</strong> (different aspects<br />

of) sound symbolism, this is compatible with <strong>in</strong>nateness of sound<br />

symbolism.<br />

Many of the above mentioned po<strong>in</strong>ts are relevant for most aspects of<br />

language, but perhaps more typically so for onomatopoeia and other<br />

sound symbolism, which are often more productive and universal. This<br />

leads back to the first question of their status <strong>in</strong> language. Other sound<br />

symbolism and onomatopoeia are different from other expressions,<br />

consider<strong>in</strong>g some of the po<strong>in</strong>ts mentioned above (productivity and<br />

universality). But, above all, they are different because of the nonarbitrary<br />

dimension.<br />

The dimensions of the framework are:<br />

1) universality - language specificity<br />

2) <strong>in</strong>nateness<br />

3) degree of conventionalization<br />

4) productivity<br />

5) centrality <strong>in</strong> language (historically, genetically, frequentially)<br />

6) types of context determ<strong>in</strong>ation<br />

and they can be related 9 <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g way:<br />

<strong>in</strong>nate<br />

universal<br />

not <strong>in</strong>nate language specific<br />

conventional<br />

conventionalization<br />

centrality productivity type of context<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>ation<br />

Figure 1.3 A framework model for sound symbolism.<br />

27<br />

actual sound<br />

symbolic mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

9The arrows <strong>in</strong> the model <strong>in</strong> figure 1.3 stand for different types of relationships, which<br />

are spelled out <strong>in</strong> the text below.


The model <strong>in</strong> figure 1.3 shows the follow<strong>in</strong>g: Assum<strong>in</strong>g that sound<br />

symbolism is central <strong>in</strong> language, this fact is compatible with both<br />

<strong>in</strong>nateness and non-<strong>in</strong>nateness of sound symbolism. If it is <strong>in</strong>nate it must<br />

be universal and if it is not <strong>in</strong>nate it is language specific (or universal due<br />

to pure chance). In both cases new expressions can be produced, based on<br />

<strong>in</strong>nate capacity and phonesthemes which are pre-exist<strong>in</strong>g because of<br />

<strong>in</strong>nateness or because of convention. Naturally there are no <strong>in</strong>nate forms<br />

that are unaffected by convention, and therefore universals, like i<br />

connected to smallness, are not absolute. Smallness does not implicate i<br />

and i does not implicate smallness. Language specific expressions can be<br />

said to be created by convention while universal expressions are affected<br />

by convention.<br />

Context affects the mean<strong>in</strong>g of all expressions. The mean<strong>in</strong>g potentials of<br />

both the language specific conventional clusters like kl- ('sound',<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g', 'wetness', 'adhesion', 'shortwide form' and 'pejorative') and<br />

more universal phonesthemes like i (smallness, high pitch, light) are<br />

disambiguated by context (phonesthemic, situational, etc.).<br />

A lexical description of sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> Swedish is presented <strong>in</strong><br />

chapters 4 and 5. The lexical description, which shows the mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

potentials for consonant clusters, treats sound symbolism as a part of<br />

language and not as an exception. The description is a basis for<br />

predictions of sound symbolism (chapter 8). It does not claim<br />

universality. The explanatory model of chapter 2, which draws on the<br />

above discussion and describes <strong>in</strong> detail the nature of the motivated<br />

relation between expression and mean<strong>in</strong>g of sound symbolic words, is,<br />

however, easily relatable to many languages.<br />

1.10 Expression and content of sound symbolism<br />

Before I cont<strong>in</strong>ue with the presentation of more concrete results obta<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

by various l<strong>in</strong>guists, I will make some comments about the expressions<br />

and contents of onomatopoeia and other sound symbolism, i.e. phonemes,<br />

sounds, phonological features, semantic fields, and level of semantic<br />

categories.<br />

28


1.10.1 Expression<br />

The expressive side of onomatopoeia and other sound symbolism is<br />

usually described <strong>in</strong> terms of phonemes, i.e. as belong<strong>in</strong>g to the language<br />

<strong>in</strong> question, or as sounds (phones) not belong<strong>in</strong>g to the phonemic<br />

<strong>in</strong>ventory of the language <strong>in</strong> question. More seldom it is described <strong>in</strong><br />

terms of some k<strong>in</strong>d of phonological or phonetic features.<br />

In this presentation the phonemes are written as <strong>in</strong> the literature, but one<br />

has to keep <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d that phonemes are relative to the phoneme system of<br />

the language under consideration.<br />

1.10.2 Content<br />

In terms of content, especially if one is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> similarities between<br />

languages, it is convenient to work with semantic fields (Trier, 1934,<br />

Lehrer, 1974) and semantic features. When deal<strong>in</strong>g with certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of<br />

onomatopoeia, this is not so important; dogs sound about the same <strong>in</strong><br />

different countries, so different expressions can be fairly<br />

straightforwardly compared. Interjections, on the other hand, often<br />

express different aspects of human emotions and are therefore often more<br />

difficult to compare.<br />

While an expression such as aj (ouch) can probably be translated fairly<br />

easily <strong>in</strong>to other languages, many other exclamations are probably better<br />

described with semantic components <strong>in</strong> relation to a semantic field, rather<br />

than just translated to the nearest synonym. E.g. ohoj (ahoy) has an<br />

expression with a similar content (EwE) <strong>in</strong> Ososo, but this is not used to<br />

adults. Schas (shoo) <strong>in</strong> Icelandic has a special expression when directed to<br />

sheep (hau hau <strong>in</strong>stead of sch). Hoppsan (whoops) <strong>in</strong> Slovenian is<br />

translated differently depend<strong>in</strong>g on whether the surprise is experienced <strong>in</strong><br />

a negative or positive way (Oho: and Oi).<br />

1.10.3 Expression and content <strong>in</strong> different analyses<br />

It is often difficult to compare the results obta<strong>in</strong>ed by different l<strong>in</strong>guists<br />

because of different levels of analytic semantic categories. The categories<br />

can be very broad, e.g. th<strong>in</strong>gs and appearances (Jespersen, 1922 a) or<br />

more narrow, e.g. color (Samar<strong>in</strong>, 1978). Also, the categories differ<br />

from one l<strong>in</strong>guist to another. In the context of onomatopoeia and other<br />

sound symbolism it is difficult but perhaps desirable to strive towards a<br />

29


ase level à la Brown (1958). Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1950) is of the op<strong>in</strong>ion that the<br />

level of specificity determ<strong>in</strong>es which morpheme analysis is made. ‘‘The<br />

lower the specificity of mean<strong>in</strong>g, the larger is the number of forms that<br />

may be subsumed under one morpheme.’’ As an example of this, he gives<br />

Nida's characterization of the 'suffix' /-´r/ i hammer, ladder, spider,<br />

otter, badger, and water as hav<strong>in</strong>g some sort of 'grammatical mean<strong>in</strong>g'.<br />

In spite of these difficulties I will try to compare the semantic<br />

categorizations of different l<strong>in</strong>guists, especially those who have done a<br />

more extensive analysis.<br />

The most elaborated taxonomy for onomatopoeia and other sound<br />

symbolism is Jespersen's (1922 a). He describes it as ‘‘a prelim<strong>in</strong>ary<br />

enumeration of the most obvious classes, with a small fraction of the<br />

examples ... collected.’’ The classes are:<br />

1. direct imitation, e.g. splash, klonk (onomatopoeia)<br />

2. orig<strong>in</strong>ator of the sound, e.g. cuckoo, or nicknames of nations from<br />

recurr<strong>in</strong>g words.<br />

3. movement (<strong>in</strong>separable from sound), e.g. flicker, snatch, slide<br />

4. th<strong>in</strong>gs and appearances (this seems to be form and light), e. g. ‘‘ a<br />

thick stick, a knot of wood, a bit of food, a protuberance, a small<br />

hill’’; gloom, light, dunkel<br />

5. states of m<strong>in</strong>d, e.g. grunt, sulky, also pejoratives, e.g. bosh<br />

6. size and distance, e.g. teeny<br />

7. length and strength of words and sounds; this gives an emotional<br />

effect, e.g. Danish langsommelig or Hungarian short words for commands<br />

and long words for entreaty; the category also mirrors perfective, plural,<br />

distance <strong>in</strong> time and space.<br />

A few po<strong>in</strong>ts of criticism of Jespersen's taxonomy are the follow<strong>in</strong>g:<br />

1) The basis of division is not homogeneous. The basic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple is<br />

semantic, but how this is applied is also open to discussion. The ma<strong>in</strong><br />

critique is, however: a) category 4 is a very broad semantic category, it<br />

covers almost everyth<strong>in</strong>g. I believe Jespersen wants to emphasize the<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation of visual perception. If so, then auditory perception ought<br />

to be treated <strong>in</strong> the same way, as th<strong>in</strong>gs and appearances; b) class 7 is the<br />

result of a categorization based on form and not on content.<br />

30


2) The categories are not mutually exclusive, either empirically (many<br />

words can be placed under both 1 and 3) or, analytically. Category 7 has<br />

several types of content which characterize other categories, e.g.<br />

'distance' and 'emotional effect').<br />

3) The categories are not exhaustive. It is, for example, evident that there<br />

is sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> the semantic spheres of 'wetness' and 'dryness'.<br />

The categories suggested <strong>in</strong> this thesis (see chapter 2) agree on some<br />

po<strong>in</strong>ts with those of Jespersen though they are a bit more detailed. The<br />

category 'th<strong>in</strong>gs and appearances' takes up examples which would be<br />

classified as 'form' and 'light' by me. The class of 'pejoratives' of the<br />

present thesis falls under Jespersen's 'states of m<strong>in</strong>d'. The category<br />

'orig<strong>in</strong>ator of the sound' is just a normal extension of mean<strong>in</strong>g from Act<br />

to Actor, (e. g. sökande (pres. part). – sökande (noun), (see Malmgren,<br />

1988), which also applies to words which are not sound symbolic <strong>in</strong> any<br />

way. Jespersen's last category does not have a correspondence <strong>in</strong> this<br />

thesis but is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g and deals with, e.g. ‘‘the emotional value of some<br />

'mouth-fill<strong>in</strong>g' words’’, e.g. evig – ev<strong>in</strong>derlig (eternal), vex – aggravate<br />

and slang words like splendidious for splendid.<br />

None of the authors below has tried to relate the categories which they<br />

have found to each other, <strong>in</strong> order to expla<strong>in</strong>, other than partially, the<br />

phenomena of sound symbolism. In contrast, one important aim of this<br />

thesis is to give an explanatory model for the semantic categories that<br />

reoccur <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> Swedish.<br />

1.10.4 Results - data from different authors<br />

The results presented <strong>in</strong> this section are primarily second-hand data, a<br />

large part of them from the authors discussed <strong>in</strong> 1.1-1.8. These data<br />

arrived at by earlier l<strong>in</strong>guists will be summarized, without any evaluation.<br />

Therefore, this section is a guide to what sounds and mean<strong>in</strong>gs different<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guists have studied. Comparisons between the authors are difficult for<br />

several reasons; e.g. Bol<strong>in</strong>ger claims there is an unclear boundary<br />

between neutral and affective morphemes, Wescott <strong>in</strong>cludes the whole of<br />

allolanguage, others have studied contrast, etc.<br />

None of the authors has claimed to give a complete <strong>in</strong>ventory of sound<br />

symbolism, usually they are e.g. simply illustrat<strong>in</strong>g an argument.<br />

31


Sometimes they just mention sound symbolic contents without giv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

examples of the relevant sounds or sound comb<strong>in</strong>ations.<br />

Table 1 Phonesthemic sounds and mean<strong>in</strong>gs of different authors.<br />

Examples are given <strong>in</strong> regular spell<strong>in</strong>g except for some IPA-symbols. The<br />

language described is English if noth<strong>in</strong>g else is written.<br />

Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1950):<br />

sound content<br />

gl- phenomena<br />

light<br />

of<br />

fl- phenomena<br />

movement<br />

of<br />

itr <strong>in</strong>termittent<br />

ow steady<br />

Er <strong>in</strong>tense<br />

kr- bent<br />

-amble locomotion<br />

-ust surface formation<br />

-usty old<br />

-lessness <strong>in</strong>difference<br />

i dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

-utter discont<strong>in</strong>uity<br />

-ash hit, fragments<br />

tw- twist<strong>in</strong>g motion<br />

st- arrest<br />

sp-t rush of liquid<br />

str-p l<strong>in</strong>e hav<strong>in</strong>g<br />

st-nt<br />

breadth<br />

piece of<br />

performance<br />

s k swift movement<br />

-ump awkward, heavy<br />

Bloomfield (1933):<br />

sound content<br />

32<br />

fl- mov<strong>in</strong>g light<br />

fl- movement <strong>in</strong> air<br />

gl- unmov<strong>in</strong>g light<br />

sl- smoothly wet<br />

k r - noisy impact<br />

skr- grat<strong>in</strong>g impact or<br />

sound<br />

sn- breath-noise<br />

s n - quick separation<br />

snor<br />

movement<br />

creep<br />

dJ- up-and-downmovement<br />

b - dull impact<br />

-E violent movement<br />

-E´ big light or noise<br />

-awns quick movement<br />

-im (´ ) small light or<br />

noise<br />

-Um clumsy<br />

-Et(´ ) particled<br />

movement<br />

Humboldt (1836/1907, German)<br />

sound content<br />

st- firmness<br />

n - sharp cutt<strong>in</strong>g<br />

w - random movement<br />

u hollow and dark


Rhodes (1994):<br />

Classifiers:<br />

sound content<br />

st- 1 dimensional (stick, staff, stem)<br />

str- 1 dimensional<br />

flexible<br />

33<br />

(str<strong>in</strong>g, strand, strip)<br />

fl- 2 dimensional (flap, flat, floor)<br />

S/sk - 2 dimensional, flexible (sheet, scarf)<br />

n - 3 dimensional (knob, knot, node, nut)<br />

sp- cyl<strong>in</strong>drical (spool, sp<strong>in</strong>e, spike)<br />

dr-/tr- liquid (dr<strong>in</strong>k, dra<strong>in</strong>, trickle, trough)<br />

Paths:<br />

sound content<br />

tr-/dr- simple (track, trip, drive, drag)<br />

p-/b- ‘‘anchored’’ (push, pop, bump, bounce)<br />

j-/tS - short (jerk, jiggle, jagged, chop)<br />

w - back and forth (wag, wiggle, wobble)<br />

p - abrupt onset (pop, p<strong>in</strong>g, peep)<br />

b - abrupt, loud onset (boom, bang, beep)<br />

b l - loud, air-<strong>in</strong>duced sound (blat, blast, blab)<br />

kl- abrupt onset (clank, click, clip, clop)<br />

r - irregular onset (rip, roar, roll)<br />

y loud, vocal tract noise (yell, yap, yak)<br />

Q- low pitch, slow onset (thump, thud)<br />

p l - abrupt onset (pl<strong>in</strong>k, plop,<br />

plunk)<br />

k r - abrupt onset (creak, crack, crunch)<br />

tS- irregular onset (chirp, cheep, chatter)<br />

w- poorly resolvable onset (whiz, whack, wham)<br />

z- poorly resolvable onset (zip, z<strong>in</strong>g, zap, zoom)<br />

dr- liquid (drip, dra<strong>in</strong>, drop, drizzle)<br />

sl- liquid (slop, slush)<br />

fl- liquid (flow, flush, flood)<br />

m - liquid (mud, mush, mire, marsh)


Plato:<br />

(sounds and mean<strong>in</strong>gs discussed<br />

<strong>in</strong> Cratylos, on Greek))<br />

form content<br />

r movement<br />

i all f<strong>in</strong>eness (it<br />

can penetrate<br />

ph, ps, s, z<br />

everyth<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

everyth<strong>in</strong>g<br />

similar to<br />

airstreams<br />

d, t b<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g and<br />

stand<strong>in</strong>g still<br />

l glid<strong>in</strong>g<br />

movement<br />

gl someth<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sticky<br />

n - <strong>in</strong>side (it is<br />

pronounced<br />

<strong>in</strong>side the<br />

mouth)<br />

a size (largeness?)<br />

e (eta) length (long?)<br />

o roundness<br />

Sapir (1921):<br />

sound process content<br />

reduplication distribution,<br />

plurality,<br />

repetition,<br />

customary<br />

activity,<br />

<strong>in</strong>crease of size,<br />

added <strong>in</strong>tensity,<br />

cont<strong>in</strong>uance<br />

34<br />

Malkiel (1978):<br />

sound process content<br />

reduplication<br />

+vowel change<br />

Jespersen (1918):<br />

disorder,<br />

confusion,<br />

rubbish, thrash<br />

sound content<br />

m roundness<br />

Jespersen (1922 a):<br />

no sounds content<br />

direct imitation<br />

orig<strong>in</strong>ator of<br />

the sound<br />

movement<br />

th<strong>in</strong>gs and<br />

appearances<br />

states of m<strong>in</strong>d<br />

size and<br />

distance<br />

length and<br />

strength of<br />

words and<br />

sounds<br />

Wescott (1975): (content is often<br />

not mentioned)<br />

sound content<br />

u w emotive<br />

z ‘‘an unusual<br />

semantic<br />

function’’<br />

‘‘sound<br />

repetition’’


‘‘sound<br />

alternation’’<br />

‘‘allol<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

prefix’’<br />

‘‘pentestheme’’<br />

Householder (1946)<br />

no sounds content<br />

dislike<br />

destruction<br />

projection<br />

protuberance<br />

(short and<br />

roundish)<br />

collectives: heap<br />

or pile,<br />

cluster or knot,<br />

large<br />

shapeless piece,<br />

<strong>in</strong>def<strong>in</strong>ite<br />

number or<br />

amount<br />

35<br />

thick, coarse,<br />

soft substance<br />

dull, loud,<br />

<strong>in</strong>dist<strong>in</strong>ct noise<br />

coward, failure<br />

or deceiv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

past tense, past<br />

participle<br />

deficient <strong>in</strong><br />

some desirable<br />

quality<br />

Sigurd, B. (1965) (Swedish):<br />

sound mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

fj- pejorative<br />

fn- pejorative<br />

pj- pejorative<br />

-ms pejorative<br />

-mp pejorative<br />

-sk pejorative<br />

-b(e)l pejorative<br />

-m(e)l pejorative<br />

Some generalizations<br />

Some phonesthemes are the same <strong>in</strong> English and Swedish, e.g. fl-<br />

‘‘phenomena of movement’’, e.g. flicker, flutter; fladdra, flaxa, while<br />

others are different <strong>in</strong> English and Swedish, e.g. English: fl-‘‘mov<strong>in</strong>g<br />

light’’ e.g. flicker, gl- ‘‘unmov<strong>in</strong>g light’’, e.g. gleam, and Swedish bl-,<br />

gn-, e.g. blänka, gnistra ‘‘light’’ (not to mention the discrepancies<br />

between different analysts of English). Therefore, there is not complete<br />

universality of expression on the phoneme level, s<strong>in</strong>ce for example<br />

English does not use the cluster gn- for light phenomena (it is not even a<br />

consonant cluster <strong>in</strong> English).


There are sound symbolic contents <strong>in</strong> some languages which do not occur<br />

<strong>in</strong> Swedish, e.g. <strong>in</strong> the field of color. This <strong>in</strong>dicates that all sound<br />

symbolic contents are not the same <strong>in</strong> all languages. On the other hand,<br />

size (dim<strong>in</strong>utive) stands out for itself s<strong>in</strong>ce it seems to occur <strong>in</strong> almost all<br />

languages – and <strong>in</strong> a similar phonetic form (Ultan, 1978).<br />

The categories on the content side of the lists above can be summarized as<br />

belong<strong>in</strong>g to the follow<strong>in</strong>g semantic fields. This is one possible<br />

classification, ma<strong>in</strong>ly with a departure <strong>in</strong> the senses:<br />

Hear<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sound, noise (dull, loud, <strong>in</strong>dist<strong>in</strong>ct, big, small, rush<strong>in</strong>g liquid)<br />

Vision<br />

light (small, big, mov<strong>in</strong>g, dark)<br />

Touch<br />

surface structure or substance (thick, coarse, soft)<br />

Movements<br />

(random, twist<strong>in</strong>g, swift, locomotion, up and down, quick, violent; or<br />

stop)<br />

Forms<br />

(bent, l<strong>in</strong>e hav<strong>in</strong>g breadth, projection or protuberance, round, hollow)<br />

M<strong>in</strong>d<br />

attitude, emotive (<strong>in</strong>difference, dislike)<br />

Pejorative<br />

(old, awkward, heavy, coward, failure, deceiv<strong>in</strong>g, deficient, clumsy,<br />

disorder, confusion, rubbish, thrash)<br />

Size<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

augmentative (<strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> size, added <strong>in</strong>tensity)<br />

Number<br />

collectives (heap or pile, cluster or knot, <strong>in</strong>def<strong>in</strong>ite number)<br />

<strong>in</strong>termittent (distribution <strong>in</strong> space, plurality, repetition, discont<strong>in</strong>uity)<br />

Various<br />

liquid<br />

steady, firmness<br />

destruction (hit, fragments, sharp cutt<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

<strong>in</strong>side<br />

tense (past tense, past participle)<br />

piece of performance<br />

unusual semantic function<br />

36


Three of the features are perceptive: 'hear<strong>in</strong>g', 'vision' and 'touch'. Taste<br />

and smell, however, do not occur. Perception of 'movement' is often cooccurr<strong>in</strong>g<br />

with perception of sound, a contiguity relationship (cf.<br />

discussion of H<strong>in</strong>ton, Nichols and Ohala (1994) <strong>in</strong> 1.11). 'Form' is<br />

likewise, perceptually, closely connected with 'vision' and secondarily also<br />

with 'touch' (cf. Brown, 1958).<br />

The semantic category 'm<strong>in</strong>d' (attitudes and emotions) is a category that<br />

probably comes naturally and can be seen as <strong>in</strong>dexically related to<br />

expressions for dislike, etc. This may also be the connection to at least<br />

some pejorative expressions. The size categories can be seen as iconically<br />

related to speech sound (<strong>in</strong> accordance with Ohala, 1994).<br />

It seems, from this overview of the literature, that the most common<br />

semantic categories are related to the three senses hear<strong>in</strong>g, vision, touch<br />

(situated <strong>in</strong> the cortex of the human bra<strong>in</strong>, <strong>in</strong> contrast with smell and<br />

taste) or they are metaphorically, metonymically, <strong>in</strong>dexically or iconically<br />

related to the senses.<br />

1.10.5 Experimental results<br />

Sapir (1929), (who communicated with Jespersen on sound symbolism and<br />

who wrote his master's thesis on J.G. Herder's (1772) "Essay on the<br />

orig<strong>in</strong> of speech") wrote about ‘‘latent expressive symbolism’’, a type of<br />

relationship, e.g. <strong>in</strong> words like teeny and t<strong>in</strong>y, which is ‘‘directly<br />

expressive of the difference <strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g’’. He conducted some<br />

experiments. In one of them more than 500 subjects were asked to decide<br />

which of the nonsense words mil and mal meant a large table and which<br />

meant a small table. 80% agreed that mal is better suited to the large<br />

table. An <strong>in</strong>vestigation by Bentley and Varon (1933) <strong>in</strong>dicated that [a]<br />

sounds are felt to be larger than [i] sounds <strong>in</strong> the proportion 4:1, and also<br />

that [a] is rounder and [i] is more angular <strong>in</strong> the proportion 3:1 and [a] is<br />

softer and [i] is harder <strong>in</strong> the proportion 2:1.<br />

Sapir's experiments were further developed by Newman (1933). He tested<br />

both vowels and consonants with respect to the small-large and the brightdark<br />

dimensions. His results were the follow<strong>in</strong>g: vowels agree with<br />

articulatory position (front-back:small-large); also consonants agree with<br />

37


articulatory position (labial-dental: small-large); accentuation of vowels is<br />

heavily <strong>in</strong> favor of largeness and darkness.<br />

Newman (1933) also made a study of a Thesaurus with respect to the<br />

categories of 'greatness', 'smallness', 'size' and 'littleness'. He did not,<br />

however, f<strong>in</strong>d great support for sound symbolism here.<br />

The subject of correspondence between mean<strong>in</strong>g of speech sound<br />

sequences and abstract graphic figures was <strong>in</strong>vestigated by Usnadze (1924)<br />

and Köhler (1930). Two nonsense l<strong>in</strong>e draw<strong>in</strong>gs and two nonsense words<br />

maluma and takete were presented to subjects who were asked to decide<br />

which sound matched which draw<strong>in</strong>g. The overwhelm<strong>in</strong>g majority<br />

assigned maluma to the rounded figure and takete to the angular one. This<br />

result has been shown for several other languages (Holland and<br />

Wertheimer, 1964; Davis, 1961).<br />

The longest series of experiments concerns the question of whether, and<br />

to what degree, lexical oppositions <strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g bear any consistent lawful<br />

relationship to the symbolic properties of sounds. Tsuru and Fries (1933)<br />

<strong>in</strong>itiated match<strong>in</strong>g experiments, where verbal data <strong>in</strong> the manner of<br />

Köhler's maluma study were used. Lists of pairs of opposites <strong>in</strong> two<br />

different languages were prepared, e.g. big/small - gross/kle<strong>in</strong>, and<br />

presented orally. Subjects who knew only one of these languages were to<br />

match the correspond<strong>in</strong>g words. They succeeded with a certa<strong>in</strong>ty<br />

exceed<strong>in</strong>g chance. Other l<strong>in</strong>guists have followed try<strong>in</strong>g to elim<strong>in</strong>ate the<br />

semantic cues, e. g. by match<strong>in</strong>g two languages that are both unknown to<br />

the speakers. The results were less successful <strong>in</strong> these cases as no semantic<br />

dimension could be available to the subject.<br />

A recent cont<strong>in</strong>uation of these experiments was done by Lapolla (1994).<br />

S<strong>in</strong>ce many of the earlier results have had methodological weaknesses he<br />

started a new set of experiments, on tonal morphology <strong>in</strong> Mandar<strong>in</strong><br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese. The results show that there is a cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic tendency toward<br />

associat<strong>in</strong>g acoustically acute 10 segments with 'small' category words, and<br />

acoustically grave 11 segments with 'big' category words. These results are<br />

expla<strong>in</strong>ed with reference to the ‘‘frequency code’’ theory developed by<br />

10high frequency energy<br />

11low frequency energy<br />

38


Ohala (1994). This theory implies that sound symbolism is a manifestation<br />

of a larger ethological phenomenon that is also seen <strong>in</strong> the vocal<br />

communication and certa<strong>in</strong> facial expressions 12 of other species. In<br />

<strong>in</strong>tonational communication of affect and <strong>in</strong> sound symbolic vocabulary<br />

there are sound-mean<strong>in</strong>g correlations where high F0 signifies smallness,<br />

non-threaten<strong>in</strong>g attitude, desire for goodwill of the receiver, etc, whereas<br />

low F0 conveys largeness, threat, self-confidence, and self-sufficiency.<br />

The common connections between segments (consonants and vowels) with<br />

low frequency energy 13 and largeness and segments with high frequency<br />

energy and smallness are also <strong>in</strong> accordance with the frequency code.<br />

Wilde (1958), Müller (1960) and others have studied another area of<br />

motivated expressions, namely the expressions of emotions. The present<br />

thesis will not <strong>in</strong>vestigate this area, see however Abel<strong>in</strong> and Allwood<br />

(1984). Wisseman (1954) studied the creation of onomatopoeic words<br />

from noises. Various noises were presented to the subjects, who were not<br />

able to observe how they were produced. They were then <strong>in</strong>structed to<br />

<strong>in</strong>vent or select names for the noises. The research design has been<br />

criticized, e.g. Hörmann (1979), but it seems that vowels represented the<br />

pitch and qualitative color feature of the noise: the i sound imitates a<br />

light, spiky noise, the u sound a low dark noise. The number of syllables<br />

<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>vented words was not proportional to the length of the noise; it<br />

reflected sections of the noise sequence. A noise with an abrupt beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g<br />

was represented by a word beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with a voiceless plosive p, t, k. A<br />

gradually start<strong>in</strong>g noise was described by a word beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with s or ts.<br />

Also Osgood (1962) studied (universal) phonetic symbolism. Osgood's<br />

semantic differential (Osgood et al, 1957) test<strong>in</strong>g the basic dimensions of<br />

‘‘value’’, ‘‘strength’’ and ‘‘potency’’ has been widely used <strong>in</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g<br />

emotional content <strong>in</strong> words, also for phonetic symbolism. Nonsense<br />

syllables were rated on the semantic differential. The results for<br />

American and Japanese speakers were almost identical. For both groups<br />

12The smile is connected with high F2 (and higher formant frequencies) s<strong>in</strong>ce retract<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the corners of the mouth shortens the vocal tract and thus raises those frequencies.<br />

13In consonants, voiceless obstruents have higher frequency than voiced, and dental,<br />

alveolar, palatal and front velars have higher frequencies (of bursts and frication noise)<br />

than labials and back velars. Dentals have higher frequencies of formant transitions than<br />

labials. High front vowels have higher F2 and high back vowels the lowest F2.<br />

39


front consonants (e. g. p) were more pleasant than back consonants (e.g.<br />

g); high frequency sounds were associated with smallness and impotence.<br />

However, it seems that his semantic categories are too restricted and the<br />

task of plac<strong>in</strong>g words/concepts on a 1–7 scale is not really feasible.<br />

<strong>Symbolism</strong> of French vowels was studied by Chasta<strong>in</strong>g (1958). i is treated<br />

<strong>in</strong> association with acuteness, smallness, lightness, rapidity, and closeness.<br />

Also consonantal oppositions were studied by Chasta<strong>in</strong>g (1965, 1966).<br />

Stops are hard, cont<strong>in</strong>uants soft, r is rough, strong, hard, etc. <strong>in</strong> contrast<br />

to l which is smooth, weak, light-weighted, etc.<br />

Fonagy (1963) compared i and u <strong>in</strong> Hungarian. In his <strong>in</strong>vestigations of<br />

children and adults i was quicker than u for 94%, smaller for 88%,<br />

prettier for 83%, friendlier for 82%, harder for 71 %, whereas u was<br />

thicker for 98%, hollower and darker for 97%, sadder and blunter for<br />

92%, more bitter for 86% and stronger for 80% . The responses to r<br />

were that r was wild, pugnacious, manly, roll<strong>in</strong>g, harder for the<br />

overwhelm<strong>in</strong>g majority.<br />

A recent study by Sereno (1994) concerned lexical organization.<br />

Depart<strong>in</strong>g from the results of a lexical study concern<strong>in</strong>g which vowels are<br />

the most common <strong>in</strong> English verbs and nouns, a reaction time experiment<br />

was performed. In this experiment the subjects categorized nouns and<br />

verbs. The results showed that there was a connection between syntactic<br />

class and phonological form <strong>in</strong> English. Verbs, with front vowels (which<br />

are lexically most frequent) were recognized faster than verbs with back<br />

vowels, while nouns with back vowels (which are lexically more frequent)<br />

were recognized more quickly than nouns with front vowels. This<br />

connection is <strong>in</strong>dependent of the frequency of the words. It is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to note this dist<strong>in</strong>ction between front and back vowels. The dist<strong>in</strong>ction<br />

between front and back vowels is perceptually salient and often also<br />

occurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> size-sound symbolism.<br />

Sereno (1994) thus proposes that noun/verb-categories and front/back<br />

classification of vowels (i. e. acoustical-perceptual classification) are<br />

organization pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of the lexicon and explicitly coded.<br />

As we have seen, there has been quite a large amount of different k<strong>in</strong>ds of<br />

experimentation <strong>in</strong> this area. The tests, which have been commented upon<br />

40


above, have mostly been concerned with isolated consonants (i. e. not with<br />

consonant clusters) and vowels and, semantically, with contrast<strong>in</strong>g ends on<br />

a scale (semantic oppositions). The experiments of this thesis treat<br />

Swedish (mostly) and more specifically the semantic value (not necessarily<br />

<strong>in</strong> opposition to some other category) of <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters.<br />

1.11 Possible explanations of sound symbolism<br />

The different explanations of sound symbolism are of vary<strong>in</strong>g types but<br />

have often focussed on synaesthesia and proprioception and, generally, on<br />

the question of a biological or non-biological base.<br />

1.11.1 Miscellaneous explanations and<br />

proprioception<br />

Let us now further <strong>in</strong>vestigate some of the ideas concern<strong>in</strong>g the nature of<br />

the relations between mean<strong>in</strong>g and expression <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism (cf. fig.<br />

2.3). This discussion will then serve as a basis for a suggestion of an<br />

explanatory model of sound symbolism to be presented <strong>in</strong> chapter 2.<br />

Different explanations have been put forward by different l<strong>in</strong>guists. Some<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guists like Publius Nigidius Figulus, 98–45 BC (accord<strong>in</strong>g to Jespersen,<br />

1922 a, p. 396), Fónagy (1963) or Peterfalvi (1965) have speculated on<br />

the connection between articulatory movements and mean<strong>in</strong>g. Nigidius<br />

Figulus claimed that <strong>in</strong> pronounc<strong>in</strong>g vos one puts forward one's lips and<br />

sends out breath <strong>in</strong> the direction of the other person, while this is not the<br />

case with nos 14 (i.e. an <strong>in</strong>dexical relation).<br />

Peterfalvi (1965) claims that vowels articulated towards the exterior of<br />

the body are judged to be ‘‘light’’ whereas those articulated towards the<br />

<strong>in</strong>terior of the body are judged to be ‘‘dark’’ because ‘‘the further you<br />

penetrate the body, the darker it is there.’’ It seems that he is <strong>in</strong>fluenced<br />

by sense analogy, i.e. the word dark can be used for different pe r ceptual<br />

phenomena. Fonagy (1963) claims that various movements of the tongue<br />

and the jaw bear likeness to different emotions.<br />

These and similar theories connect articulation with mean<strong>in</strong>g directly and<br />

disregard the acoustic (or visual) l<strong>in</strong>k <strong>in</strong> a communicative situation. As<br />

for the area of non-verbal communication, Fonagy's and Peterfalvi's<br />

14This is of course not correct.<br />

41


‘‘gestures’’ cannot even have an observer, if one excepts that front<br />

articulations can be seen. However, there might be proprioception<br />

<strong>in</strong>volved.<br />

Bol<strong>in</strong>ger (1968) suggests the follow<strong>in</strong>g metaphor: ‘‘the digital island floats<br />

on an analog sea’’. Digital stands for consonants and vowels, which are<br />

arbitrary, analogue stands for phenomena like prosody and gestures,<br />

which are not completely arbitrary. ‘‘... now and then a bit of the<br />

analogue sea washed over the digital island’’, e.g. when i stands for<br />

smallness and o for largeness. ‘‘The size of the mouth cavity ... is matched<br />

with the mean<strong>in</strong>g’’. If there is someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> this explanation, a number of<br />

factors are left out, e.g. the acoustic l<strong>in</strong>k. The explanation is a bit better if<br />

one compares the frequency of acoustic energy <strong>in</strong> i with that <strong>in</strong> o, or a,<br />

which is connected with the size of the mouth cavity and other vocal tract<br />

cavities. (see e.g. Jakobson, Fant, Halle, 1957).<br />

Darw<strong>in</strong> (1872) proposes a possible explanation for sound symbolic words<br />

related to emotions, e.g. disgust (related to 'pejorative'), and for<br />

<strong>in</strong>terjections. The explanation is based on the <strong>in</strong>st<strong>in</strong>ctive contractions of<br />

facial muscles connected with a certa<strong>in</strong> emotion. This type of explanation<br />

would mean that pejoratives can be classified as <strong>in</strong>dexical (cf. discussion<br />

<strong>in</strong> chapter 2).<br />

A recent <strong>in</strong>vestigation on the topic of universal sound symbolism <strong>in</strong><br />

deictic words was done by Traunmüller (1996). He found that pairs of<br />

demonstratives <strong>in</strong> which there is a vocalic opposition have an advantage <strong>in</strong><br />

the struggle for existence <strong>in</strong> languages when F2' is higher <strong>in</strong> the proximal<br />

than <strong>in</strong> the distal form; he also found that nasals are preferred <strong>in</strong> first<br />

person pronouns while stops and other obstruents are preferred <strong>in</strong> second<br />

person pronouns. He also offers explanations that <strong>in</strong>volve aff<strong>in</strong>ities with<br />

the association of pitch with size, the proprioceptive qualities of speech<br />

sounds and oral po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g gestures.<br />

1.11.2 Synaesthesia<br />

Sometimes it is stated <strong>in</strong> pass<strong>in</strong>g that sound symbolism can be expla<strong>in</strong>ed by<br />

synesthesia, i.e. neurological connections between the sound side and the<br />

semantic side of a word, morpheme or phonestheme, when the mean<strong>in</strong>g is<br />

<strong>in</strong> some way connected to one of the other senses, e.g. 'sight', 'touch' or<br />

to categories perceived with several of the senses, e.g. 'form', 'surface<br />

42


structure', 'movement'. (This resembles Müller's (1861) ‘‘d<strong>in</strong>g dong<br />

theory’’.)<br />

Luria's (1977) patient, the mnemonist S. is a well known synesthete.<br />

When he heard a tone vibrat<strong>in</strong>g with 50 cycles/second at 100 dB he saw a<br />

brown stripe aga<strong>in</strong>st a darker background with red tongues at the edges.<br />

At the same time he experienced the taste of sweet-sour borsjtj. When he<br />

heard [r] he always saw a ragged l<strong>in</strong>e. Persons with synaesthesia are<br />

unusual, and the experiences vary from person to person (cf. e.g.<br />

Cytowic, 1989).<br />

Aristotle claimed that the senses were clearly separated from each other.<br />

Newton, on the other hand, tried to f<strong>in</strong>d a numerical correlation between<br />

e.g. the wave length of green light and a certa<strong>in</strong> frequency of sound.<br />

However, he could not f<strong>in</strong>d this.<br />

The problems with an explanation <strong>in</strong> terms of synaesthesia are several.<br />

Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Cytowic (1989) synaesthesia is an idiosyncratic<br />

phenomenon, which varies between the persons who experience it.<br />

Cytowic argues that ‘‘rather than be<strong>in</strong>g merely a more <strong>in</strong>tense form of<br />

metaphoric speech, one can look at cross-modal metaphor as an abstract,<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic derivative of the stuff of synaesthesia’’. Jakobson and Waugh<br />

(1979), however, mention different f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of tendencies of the<br />

correlation of speech sounds with colors: redness of a, yellowness and<br />

whitishness of e and i and darkness of o and u.<br />

Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Marks (1990) the synaesthetic connections do not vary too<br />

much between the experiences of different persons. For example, the<br />

frequency of the second formant of vowels can be related to black (dark)<br />

and white (light) so that [u] has a low frequency and dark color while [i]<br />

has a high frequency and light color.<br />

In order to give a more conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g explanation, one would need to do it<br />

<strong>in</strong> terms of neurophysiology. For example Freud (1891) suggested that<br />

language could be represented <strong>in</strong> a ‘‘field’’ <strong>in</strong> the border area between the<br />

temporal, parietal and occipital lobes, where all properties of the<br />

perception of a th<strong>in</strong>g were connected <strong>in</strong> a network (smell, taste, visual<br />

appearance, sound, etc.)<br />

43


Consider<strong>in</strong>g the apparent similarities between the semantic categories <strong>in</strong><br />

s y n a e s t h e s i a – color, form, motion, texture, lum<strong>in</strong>escence,<br />

dynamics (but also numbers, days of the week and months of the year,<br />

moral judgements) – s e n s e a n a l o g i e s (sense analogies are<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic metaphors, e.g. the expressions dark tones, warm colors, cf.<br />

Abel<strong>in</strong>, 1988) and s o u n d s y m b o l i s m , i.e. sense related categories,<br />

one might hypothesize a common ground for synaesthesia on the one hand<br />

and language phenomena like sense analogies and sound symbolism on the<br />

other.<br />

This is <strong>in</strong> agreement with Geschw<strong>in</strong>d (1965, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Cytowic, 1989):<br />

language depends on stable <strong>in</strong>termodal associations, especially visualauditory<br />

and tactile-auditory. These are the most common modes of<br />

synaesthesia. The relation between sound and mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> sound<br />

symbolism should then stand for a neurological connection; this can be<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpreted as an <strong>in</strong>dexical relation s<strong>in</strong>ce it is not iconic or conventional.<br />

Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Cytowic (1989), there is beh<strong>in</strong>d synaesthesia a purely<br />

neurological process which is connected to certa<strong>in</strong> cell clusters. He studied<br />

the blood flow to cortical association centra for vision-hear<strong>in</strong>g-touch<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g subjects synaesthetic experiences. He expected an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> these<br />

association centra but did not f<strong>in</strong>d such an <strong>in</strong>crease. Instead, the<br />

connections seemed to occur <strong>in</strong> the limbic system, which was studied by<br />

<strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>in</strong>g electric stimulation dur<strong>in</strong>g operations. Cytowic argues that, <strong>in</strong><br />

some <strong>in</strong>dividuals, the developmentally earlier, ‘‘suppressed’’ limbic<br />

system sometimes overrides the cortex and that the boundaries between<br />

the senses then disappear.<br />

Related to the issue of synaesthesia and language universals is the study of<br />

verbs of perception <strong>in</strong> 50 languages by Viberg (1984). This study showed<br />

that there are implicational universals <strong>in</strong> the order:<br />

SEE>HEAR>FEEL>TASTE>SMELL. That is, if a language has only one<br />

verb of perception, the basic mean<strong>in</strong>g is 'see'. If it has two, the basic<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs are 'see' and 'hear' etc.<br />

1.11.3 Other neurological and biological<br />

explanations<br />

A specific, neurological explanation for why languages often represent<br />

movement with the same sort of sound symbolic forms that they use for<br />

44


the representation of non-l<strong>in</strong>guistic sounds is found <strong>in</strong> H<strong>in</strong>ton, Nichols and<br />

Ohala (1994): ‘‘ Certa<strong>in</strong> rhythmic movements often directly produce<br />

sound. But beyond that, the rhythms of sound and the rhythms of<br />

movement are so closely l<strong>in</strong>ked <strong>in</strong> the human neural system that they are<br />

virtually <strong>in</strong>separable. This is illustrated <strong>in</strong> the very natural human<br />

physical response to rhythmic music, <strong>in</strong> the forms of hand clapp<strong>in</strong>g, foot<br />

tapp<strong>in</strong>g, danc<strong>in</strong>g, rhythmical physical labor, etc. ... humans ... are also<br />

capable of the reverse : translat<strong>in</strong>g rhythmic movements <strong>in</strong>to sounds,<br />

<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g sound-symbolic language forms.’’ (pp. 3-4) In other words, at<br />

least part of ‘‘the sound symbolic feel<strong>in</strong>g’’ is not someth<strong>in</strong>g that one has<br />

learned.<br />

Related to the area of synaesthesia and sense analogy is the research area<br />

of bimodal perception e.g. <strong>in</strong> the form of the McGurk effect (e.g.<br />

Massaro et al 1993). Various experiments show how e.g. auditory<br />

perception is <strong>in</strong>fluenced by e.g. visual or tactile perception, and po<strong>in</strong>ts to<br />

an <strong>in</strong>terconnection of the senses. Which phonemes (or sound types) that<br />

are perceived do not, then, depend purely on acoustic <strong>in</strong>formation but also<br />

on <strong>in</strong>formation from other senses.<br />

Ohala (e.g.1994) discusses the frequency code and offers a solution for<br />

sound symbolism of dim<strong>in</strong>utives and augmentatives. In contrast with<br />

Brown (1958) his theory po<strong>in</strong>ts to <strong>in</strong>nateness through an ethologically<br />

based, phonetically plausible theory for why sound symbolism exists <strong>in</strong><br />

language. He identifies a l<strong>in</strong>k between sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> vowels,<br />

consonants, tones and <strong>in</strong>tonation. The common factor is high-low F0 (of<br />

tones and <strong>in</strong>tonation), noise frequency (of obstruents) or F2 (of vowels<br />

and sonorant consonants). High frequency is connected to smallness, low<br />

frequency to largeness. However, <strong>in</strong> Swedish there are several examples<br />

of <strong>in</strong>itial [p] usually denot<strong>in</strong>g smallness <strong>in</strong> spite of be<strong>in</strong>g grave ([p] has<br />

noise at low frequencies), e.g. pipa, pil, pilla, pilt, p<strong>in</strong>gla, p<strong>in</strong>ne, pippi,<br />

(however followed by [i], which has F2 at a high frequency). Another<br />

possible explanation of why [p] can denote smallness is the fact that [p] has<br />

weak noise.<br />

Clark and Clark (1977) discuss different k<strong>in</strong>ds of categories, perceptual,<br />

e.g. color, shape, spatiality, cognitive categories, e.g. number, negation,<br />

evaluation, cause and effect, time, and social categories, e.g. k<strong>in</strong>ship<br />

terms, personal pronouns.<br />

45


1.11.4 Non-biological explanations<br />

A non-biological explanation of size-sound symbolism is offered by<br />

Brown (1958), who claims that associations between, e.g., sound and size<br />

are simply learned through experience. Large objects usually produce<br />

dark (low frequency) sounds when pushed or moved <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> ways,<br />

whereas small objects produce bright (high frequency) sounds, (i. e. the<br />

relation is <strong>in</strong>dexical). Thus, universality does not have to imply<br />

<strong>in</strong>nateness. Brown (1958) also anticipates the studies on multimodal<br />

perception and writes that perception does not have to be connected to a<br />

particular receptor but is a matter of the whole body.<br />

Lakoff and Johnsson (1980), expla<strong>in</strong> part of sound symbolism with<strong>in</strong> the<br />

framework of their theory for metaphors. Underly<strong>in</strong>g sound symbolism is<br />

the ‘‘conduit metaphor’’ def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a spatial relationship between form and<br />

content: ‘‘l<strong>in</strong>guistic expressions are conta<strong>in</strong>ers’’, and their mean<strong>in</strong>gs are<br />

the content of those conta<strong>in</strong>ers. We expect small conta<strong>in</strong>ers to have small<br />

contents, large conta<strong>in</strong>ers to have large contents. Therefore: more form is<br />

more content. Examples of this is reduplication: He ran and ran and ran,<br />

He is very, very tall or lengthen<strong>in</strong>g: He is bi-i-i-i-i-ig.<br />

Diffloth (1994) goes aga<strong>in</strong>st ma<strong>in</strong>stream work on sound symbolism with<br />

unusual data and unusual explanations. He shows data from Bahnar, a<br />

language of Vietnam, <strong>in</strong> which i expresses largeness and a expresses<br />

smallness. He claims that there is nevertheless an iconic basis for this – <strong>in</strong><br />

proprioceptive sensation. He concludes that iconicity can be both<br />

physiologically motivated and culturally relative at the same time. From<br />

this follows, he proposes, that iconic patterns, be<strong>in</strong>g language specific,<br />

must be described <strong>in</strong> the grammars of language. He criticizes current<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic models for not be<strong>in</strong>g able to describe a direct relation between<br />

phonetics and semantics. Also, phonetics is unable to represent the<br />

phonetic parameters needed for Bahnar, e.g. perceived size of the tongue<br />

<strong>in</strong> the oral cavity. He proposes an aesthetic component of the grammar.<br />

His discussions on a proprioceptive explanation are very <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g but<br />

seem isolated <strong>in</strong> comparison with the elaborate Frequency Code theory of<br />

Ohala (see 1.10.5). Nevertheless, I th<strong>in</strong>k, it is preferable to give an<br />

explanation of Bahnar’s size-sound symbolism rather than discard<strong>in</strong>g it as<br />

an exception. A multimodal model for sound symbolism should<br />

preferably <strong>in</strong>corporate the facts of Bahnar, i. e. proprioception.<br />

46


Hamano (1994) has studied palatalization <strong>in</strong> Japanese sound symbolism.<br />

Palatalization of alveolar stops and fricatives is associated with<br />

‘‘childishness’’ and ‘‘immaturity’’. He connects this to studies on language<br />

acquisition report<strong>in</strong>g palatalization as one of the universal characteristics<br />

of early stages of children's language acquisition. Palatalization is also<br />

reported as one of the commonest features of baby-talk, i.e. child directed<br />

speech (Snow and Ferguson, 1977). A possible explanation of the Swedish<br />

pejorative clusters pj-, bj-, fj- which do not fit <strong>in</strong>to a sense related<br />

pattern (see chapters 2 and 4) could be that they have another orig<strong>in</strong>, <strong>in</strong><br />

child directed speech. Consider<strong>in</strong>g Hamanos f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs these clusters can be<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpreted as marked (they are unusual), rather than hav<strong>in</strong>g their orig<strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terjections. Hamanos explanation is basically <strong>in</strong>dexical (causal).<br />

A study of the frequency of Swedish consonant clusters <strong>in</strong> runn<strong>in</strong>g text<br />

(Stenström, 1984) shows that the seven most <strong>in</strong>frequent <strong>in</strong>itial consonant<br />

clusters are fj-, bj-, pj-, mj-, spl-, vr-, nj- (where nj- is the most<br />

unusual). In other words, bj-, pj- and fj- are among the most <strong>in</strong>frequent<br />

clusters, also <strong>in</strong> runn<strong>in</strong>g text.<br />

A pragmatic explanation for a part of sound symbolism, namely<br />

vocatives, is given by Jacobsen (1994). In Nootka languages there seem to<br />

be two ma<strong>in</strong> sound-symbolic tendencies that shape vocative forms:<br />

saliency and brevity. Saliency means that the word conta<strong>in</strong>s a prom<strong>in</strong>ent<br />

syllable that will attract the attention of the addressee. For example, nonhigh<br />

vowels are more salient because they are <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sically more <strong>in</strong>tense;<br />

fall<strong>in</strong>g pitch is argued to be more salient because it <strong>in</strong>volves a rapid<br />

change <strong>in</strong> pitch. The notion of saliency can, however, be criticized. Even<br />

if non-high vowels are acoustically more <strong>in</strong>tense, they are perceived as<br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g the same strength as high vowels. Also, a rais<strong>in</strong>g F0 can be as<br />

quick as a fall<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The discussion of different explanations of sound symbolism is cont<strong>in</strong>ued<br />

<strong>in</strong> 8.3 where the arguments are related to the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of the lexical and<br />

experimental studies of this thesis.<br />

47


2 Theoretical framework<br />

2.1 General considerations<br />

The preced<strong>in</strong>g chapter was an overview of term<strong>in</strong>ological issues and<br />

general questions concern<strong>in</strong>g onomatopoeia and other sound symbolism.<br />

This chapter will penetrate further <strong>in</strong>to the relation between mean<strong>in</strong>g and<br />

expression <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism. There will be an attempt at an explanation<br />

of the relation between expression and mean<strong>in</strong>g, with the aid of the<br />

concepts 'icon' and '<strong>in</strong>dex'.<br />

In this thesis the standpo<strong>in</strong>t is that language <strong>in</strong> important ways is a<br />

psychological phenomenon. The semantics of lexical analysis is therefore<br />

not concerned with referents, but with reference: correspondence between<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic form and external data. For a discussion on this subject, see<br />

Allwood and Andersson (1976).<br />

The validity of the model presented, for example concern<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

explanations or the number and types of semantic categories provided, will<br />

be supported <strong>in</strong> terms of coherence, e.g. recurrent relations between<br />

expression and content <strong>in</strong> the analyses, and by consensus, between subjects<br />

<strong>in</strong> the experiments.<br />

2.2 Static-dynamic, conventionality and arbitrar<strong>in</strong>ess<br />

The vocabulary of language is both static and dynamic (<strong>in</strong> the sense of<br />

undergo<strong>in</strong>g change). Some parts are less static than other parts, e.g. sound<br />

symbolic words can be less static <strong>in</strong> both a diachronic and a microgenetic<br />

perspective (i.e., dur<strong>in</strong>g the development of language <strong>in</strong> a situation) than<br />

more arbitrary words are, s<strong>in</strong>ce sound symbolic words can easily be<br />

created. The phonesthemes, however, are stable and sound symbolic<br />

words are created with the assistance of phonesthemes.<br />

Thus, onomatopoeic or other sound symbolic neologisms come and go, but<br />

the phonesthemes, out of which neologisms can be created, are stable over<br />

49


longer periods of time. An example (concern<strong>in</strong>g the pejorative fj-) from<br />

SAOB (Ordbok över svenska språket, utgiven av Svenska Akademien) is<br />

the word fjåka (now, if exist<strong>in</strong>g at all, only <strong>in</strong> some dialects) which meant<br />

'våp, narr, tok' (silly or crazy person). The first written <strong>in</strong>stance is from<br />

1732; there are similar words with similar mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> Norwegian dialects.<br />

The orig<strong>in</strong> of the word is described as "unclear". In other words, fjåka is no<br />

longer exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> standard Swedish, but the phonestheme fj- 'pejorative'<br />

seems to have been the same <strong>in</strong> 1732 as today; the phonesthemes are more<br />

stable than the words. Another more recent example is the word slobb. It<br />

was created for a newspaper article (around 1985) and describes the sound<br />

of porridge land<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a bowl. The same expression may be created aga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong><br />

another context. One phonestheme used is the phonestheme sl- 'wetness'.<br />

In words, the connection between expression and content can be described<br />

as arbitrary or motivated. The motivated words can be + or - conventional;<br />

neologisms are -conventional. In reality a word cannot be completely nonconventional<br />

because the phonesthemes of sound symbolic neologisms are<br />

not. As discussed <strong>in</strong> connection with the model shown <strong>in</strong> figure 1.3,<br />

language specific expressions are created by convention while expressions<br />

emanat<strong>in</strong>g from <strong>in</strong>nate (universal) processes are affected by convention.<br />

arbitrary door<br />

+conventional -conventional<br />

motivated shriek iiiiik<br />

The field of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> this thesis consists of the motivated expressions,<br />

both + and - conventional. Arbitrar<strong>in</strong>ess is a precondition for<br />

conventionalization. Arbitrary words are thus always conventional while<br />

motivated words can be conventionalized or not. In language there can not<br />

be non-conventional arbitrary words. Somebody can <strong>in</strong>vent words that fit<br />

50


<strong>in</strong>to this square but nobody would understand them. The present claim is<br />

that the non-conventional can be understood if it is motivated. In some<br />

cases non-conventional words can be said to be constructed out of<br />

conventional phonesthemes, e.g. the expression fnölp is constructed for<br />

someth<strong>in</strong>g that is 'silly' (i.e. 'pejorative').<br />

The perspective on words <strong>in</strong> this thesis is synchronic, not diachronic. This<br />

is also the case as concerns semantic relations and extension of mean<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Language is not static, but <strong>in</strong> the microperspective static enough.<br />

2.3 Semantic analysis<br />

2.3.1 Conceptions of mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Mean<strong>in</strong>g is more or less context dependent, especially so <strong>in</strong> the reference of<br />

a lexeme. The mean<strong>in</strong>gs of concrete nouns like horse or spoon are less<br />

context dependent than pronouns like he or it.<br />

Lexemes can be claimed to have a central, <strong>in</strong>tensional mean<strong>in</strong>g. It is<br />

possible to describe this mean<strong>in</strong>g as prototypes, mean<strong>in</strong>g components or<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>ctive features. One can also make a dist<strong>in</strong>ction between core mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

and the vaguer emotive and associative mean<strong>in</strong>gs and sometimes between<br />

denotation and connotation. Another conception, proposed by Allwood<br />

(1989) is 'mean<strong>in</strong>g potential'. This means, briefly, that e.g. a word form has<br />

a union of potential mean<strong>in</strong>gs, one of which is decided on by different<br />

contextual conditions. Homonymy only has to be resorted to <strong>in</strong> word<br />

classification where the different mean<strong>in</strong>gs are not relatable via semantic<br />

relations like metonymy or metaphor.<br />

The concept of mean<strong>in</strong>g potential is useful also <strong>in</strong> the analysis of<br />

phonesthemes, and will be used <strong>in</strong> this thesis. A certa<strong>in</strong> consonant cluster<br />

can have one or several mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Where there are several mean<strong>in</strong>gs many<br />

of these are usually relatable to each other, e.g. light – form lett<strong>in</strong>g through<br />

light – look<strong>in</strong>g – gaze – reflect<strong>in</strong>g smooth surface – movement on such a<br />

surface (gl-), which all can be characterized as metonymical (<strong>in</strong>dexical).<br />

Other examples are sound – wetness – adhesion – pejorative (kl-) and<br />

51


wetness – smooth surface – quick movement – long th<strong>in</strong> form (sl-). (Some<br />

of these are described <strong>in</strong> more detail <strong>in</strong> chapter 4.6.)<br />

The mean<strong>in</strong>gs of onomatopoeic and other sound symbolic neologisms are<br />

very context sensitive. What does, for example, the word slasli (which is<br />

newly created) mean? Well, someth<strong>in</strong>g hav<strong>in</strong>g to do with wetness. If we<br />

get more context, like <strong>in</strong> the test match<strong>in</strong>g columns of neologisms, where<br />

each column has a special mean<strong>in</strong>g, (cf. table 7.8), subjects correctly pair<br />

word columns with mean<strong>in</strong>gs. The results depend on the possibility to<br />

compare the words <strong>in</strong> the different columns and compare these with the<br />

different suggested mean<strong>in</strong>gs. We get an effect from phonological and<br />

semantic context.<br />

Emotive and associative mean<strong>in</strong>gs are often not considered part of the core<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g of words. I will not adopt this standpo<strong>in</strong>t but I claim that<br />

associative/emotive mean<strong>in</strong>gs belong to the mean<strong>in</strong>g potential of words and<br />

phonesthemes. I claim that, apart from the lexicalized emotive/associative<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs which <strong>in</strong>dividual lexemes may have, there are also phonesthemes<br />

as parts of <strong>in</strong>dividual lexemes, e. g. sl- mean<strong>in</strong>g wetness <strong>in</strong> e. g. slaska<br />

(splash), slafsa (squelch), slabba (splash), slem (slime).<br />

2.3.2 Semantic features and semantic fields<br />

The status of different types of semantic units, i.e. how concepts are best<br />

described, has been the subject of a long debate (see Allwood and<br />

Andersson (1976), Allwood (1989), Kukkonen (1989), Lehrer (1974),<br />

Miller and Johnsson Laird (1976) and Lyons (1977)). The most important<br />

units proposed are essences (def<strong>in</strong>ed by necessary and sufficient<br />

conditions) discussed e.g. by Aristotle and semantic features or<br />

components, discussed by e.g. Katz and Postal (1964), Leech (1969). The<br />

semantic features can be of different k<strong>in</strong>ds, e.g. <strong>in</strong> the form of a restricted<br />

number of primitive (universal) dist<strong>in</strong>ctive features with +, – values (i. e.<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g components) or more generally: features found empirically <strong>in</strong><br />

semantic analyses and which are possible to decompose further (i. e.<br />

semantic features or mean<strong>in</strong>g postulates). Mean<strong>in</strong>g components and<br />

semantic features have also proven to be a useful tool <strong>in</strong> systematic<br />

52


lexicology. In this thesis are used semantic features, i.e. features found<br />

empirically <strong>in</strong> the semantic analysis, but not mean<strong>in</strong>g components, <strong>in</strong> the<br />

sense of a restricted number of apriori primitive features.<br />

Semantic fields can be characterized, us<strong>in</strong>g semantic features, as consist<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of words (lexemes) with related mean<strong>in</strong>gs which have at least one common<br />

semantic feature <strong>in</strong> common and are analyzed through mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

components or semantic features. Fields can be used to show paradigmatic<br />

relations between the words (Lehrer, 1974) or syntagmatic relations<br />

(Porzig, 1934).<br />

Examples of semantic fields relevant for this study are 'pejoratives' and<br />

'form'. The aim was not to do a semantic field analysis. Instead fields and<br />

properties of fields will be used <strong>in</strong> the discussions, e.g. sometimes the field<br />

of 'pejoratives' is discussed, sometimes the feature [PEJORATIVE] and<br />

sometimes words like fjantig (fussy) or fjollig (foolish). In this way<br />

different authors and different <strong>in</strong>formants who have given <strong>in</strong>formation<br />

related to semantic fields at different levels of abstraction could be <strong>in</strong>cluded<br />

<strong>in</strong> the discussion .<br />

One aim of this thesis is to give a psychologically valid description and the<br />

pr<strong>in</strong>ciple assumed here is that the appropriate level is the one that works<br />

best <strong>in</strong> the analysis. Dur<strong>in</strong>g data collection the root morpheme level was<br />

preferred. The mean<strong>in</strong>gs of roots were later, <strong>in</strong> the analysis, decomposed<br />

<strong>in</strong>to semantic features.<br />

S<strong>in</strong>ce some of the studies concern a number of words with similar (and<br />

vague) content, with<strong>in</strong> a restricted number of semantic categories, which<br />

are translated <strong>in</strong>to different languages, it is helpful to work with semantic<br />

fields, and semantic features. An example: when translat<strong>in</strong>g to an unknown<br />

language, and with <strong>in</strong>formants of vary<strong>in</strong>g proficiency <strong>in</strong> Swedish (or<br />

English) it is difficult to ask for word to word translations of e.g. words like<br />

oh, ah, whoops (to take some <strong>in</strong>terjections) but easier to ask for words with<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g of positive surprise or negative mood etc.<br />

53


It can also be the case that an expression, for example for pa<strong>in</strong>, can be<br />

translated differently <strong>in</strong> different contexts. For example there is, <strong>in</strong> Ososo, a<br />

special expression of pa<strong>in</strong> for when somebody has died.<br />

Other <strong>in</strong>terjections, like atjo (for a sneeze) are easily translatable word for<br />

word, e.g. [haptsI] <strong>in</strong> Hungarian and [Itçími] <strong>in</strong> Nigerian Ososo.<br />

2.4 Basic relations between expression and content<br />

The field of <strong>in</strong>terest which was analyzed here can be def<strong>in</strong>ed as<br />

"synchronously motivated verbal signs". By this def<strong>in</strong>ition non-verbal signs<br />

such as gestures, sign-posts etc are excluded. For practical reasons the<br />

greater part of prosody such as sentence <strong>in</strong>tonation, phrase <strong>in</strong>tonation etc<br />

was also excluded.<br />

<strong>Sound</strong> symbolism can be divided <strong>in</strong>to three types:<br />

Free morphemes: 1) onomatopoeic<br />

2) expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections<br />

Non-free morphemes: 3) sound symbolic phonesthemes<br />

(these can be part of 1) and 2))<br />

The basic dist<strong>in</strong>ctions between these three groups are:<br />

1) onomatopoeia is ma<strong>in</strong>ly iconic, e.g. mjau, plask<br />

2) expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections are ma<strong>in</strong>ly <strong>in</strong>dexical, e.g. aj, atjo<br />

3) sound symbolic phonesthemes are motivated, but not so clearly iconic<br />

or <strong>in</strong>dexical, e.g. glänsa, fjanta<br />

The three groups can be characterized <strong>in</strong> more detail:<br />

1) Onomatopoeia is realized <strong>in</strong> complete free morphemes e.g. mjau, voff.<br />

Onomatopoeia is a verbal imitation of any sound. The relation between<br />

expression and content has an element of arbitrar<strong>in</strong>ess. The mean<strong>in</strong>g is a<br />

54


(sort of) sound. The expression side is more or less conventionalized, that<br />

is, adjusted to the phonology of the language <strong>in</strong> question.<br />

The expression has universal traits with a language specific superstructure<br />

which is affected by the phonology of the <strong>in</strong>dividual language. The relation<br />

between expression and content is basically iconic. (The sound of the<br />

rooster can serve as an illustration; it is (with <strong>in</strong>formants' transcriptions) <strong>in</strong><br />

Swedish kuckeliku, <strong>in</strong> German kikeriki, <strong>in</strong> Serbocroatian kukuricu,<br />

Macedonian kukurica, Italian chicchiricchi, Syrian kuckylyku, Urdu<br />

kokelongkong). Figure 2.1 illustrates onomatopoeia:<br />

sound<br />

step1: iconic relation<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic form<br />

(step2:<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic expression)<br />

conventionalization<br />

Figure 2.1 Schematic illustration 1 of onomatopoeia.<br />

2) Expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections are also realized <strong>in</strong> complete free morphemes<br />

e.g. atjo, aj, oj. The relation between expression and content is nonarbitrary.<br />

The expression can be described as stand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>direct causal<br />

relation to the content which is a bodily or mental reaction. The expression<br />

is more or less conventionalized and there are universal traits <strong>in</strong> the<br />

expressions, naturally with a language specific superstructure. Its base is<br />

<strong>in</strong>dexical. Figure 2.2 can illustrate:<br />

1 L<strong>in</strong>guistic form is a phonetic form associated with a mean<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

55


emotion, bodily reaction<br />

step 1: <strong>in</strong>dexical relation<br />

expression (cry, i.e.<br />

sound)<br />

step 2: iconic relation<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic form<br />

step 3: conventionalization<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

expression<br />

Figure 2.2 Schematic illustration of expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections.<br />

That is, a bodily reaction produces a sound, e.g a sneeze or a scream. That<br />

scream etc. is then verbally imitated (like <strong>in</strong> onomatopoeia) and then<br />

conventionalized <strong>in</strong>to a word of a language.<br />

The more vivid the connection between e.g. an emotion and its verbal<br />

expression is, the stronger the causal component is. But the form of the<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic sign is not an icon of the emotion etc.! The causal relation is<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>sofar as the expression could not be just any k<strong>in</strong>d of<br />

expression.<br />

Probably many signs are a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of iconicity, <strong>in</strong>dexicality and<br />

symbol. Atjo is a good example of a word were the <strong>in</strong>dexical relation (a<br />

natural relation) is clear. The sound of the sneeze is imitated and then<br />

conventionalized - the symbol for a sneeze differs <strong>in</strong> different languages.<br />

The word aj is not so clearly <strong>in</strong>dexical; the connection with pa<strong>in</strong> is not as<br />

obvious as the connection between the sneeze and the sound of a sneeze,<br />

even though the sound for pa<strong>in</strong> can be seen as dependent on the shape of<br />

the mouth cavity while, e.g. one is scream<strong>in</strong>g from pa<strong>in</strong>. The mouth is<br />

opened wide, caus<strong>in</strong>g an [a]-like sound <strong>in</strong> all languages (see Darw<strong>in</strong>,<br />

1872).<br />

56


3) Phonesthemes are realised systematically with<strong>in</strong> complete (traditional)<br />

morphemes, e.g. fl- <strong>in</strong> flicker, flutter. In sound symbolic phonesthemes the<br />

relation between expression and content is of a type which is experienced<br />

as motivated by the typical speaker of a language. (In a more compell<strong>in</strong>g<br />

degree than the feel<strong>in</strong>g, e.g. by English speakers that horse is a better<br />

expression than häst, for that four-legged animal.) The expression side is<br />

language specific, and conventional, but the content side has universal<br />

traits.<br />

The mean<strong>in</strong>g of a phonestheme is experienced by the speaker/listener as<br />

somehow correspond<strong>in</strong>g to the expression. The ma<strong>in</strong> problem is to describe<br />

the type of relation that exists between, e.g. fl- and 'quick or strong<br />

movement'. What the relation might be, <strong>in</strong> detail and <strong>in</strong> relation to iconicity<br />

and <strong>in</strong>dexicality, is discussed <strong>in</strong> sections 2.5 and 2.6.<br />

2.5 The nature of phonesthemes<br />

First there is the problem with onomatopoeic phonesthemes like fr- <strong>in</strong> e.g.<br />

frusta or gn- <strong>in</strong> e.g. gnägga, gnälla. In the k<strong>in</strong>d of analysis undertaken <strong>in</strong><br />

this chapter these words ought to belong to category 1, onomatopoeic<br />

words, s<strong>in</strong>ce they are iconic (even though <strong>in</strong> a word like gnägga it is<br />

conventionalized almost beyond recognition). However, I have preferred to<br />

put the onomatopoeic phonesthemes <strong>in</strong> category 3, even though they are<br />

not problematic semiotically, because they are often metaphorically<br />

connected with other phonesthemes. Gn- which is 'sound', like <strong>in</strong> (gnissla,<br />

gnälla) is metaphorically connected with gn- : 'way of talk<strong>in</strong>g' (like <strong>in</strong><br />

gnata, gnola, gnälla). Here is aga<strong>in</strong> the problem of dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

(which also holds for other morphology) s<strong>in</strong>ce 'way of talk<strong>in</strong>g' really<br />

belongs to the same mean<strong>in</strong>g potential as 'sound' does. In this analysis,<br />

however, 'way of talk<strong>in</strong>g' is isolated s<strong>in</strong>ce it is both frequent and<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g can be stated and should be considered <strong>in</strong> a model for<br />

phonesthemes:<br />

57


1. Some sounds/sound comb<strong>in</strong>ations are (judged to be) better suited for<br />

some (types of) mean<strong>in</strong>gs, with<strong>in</strong> a given language or for many languages.<br />

2. Some mean<strong>in</strong>gs are better suited for be<strong>in</strong>g expressed with some of these<br />

sounds/sound comb<strong>in</strong>ations.<br />

Why? It isn´t plausible that the only explanation is that a number of words<br />

happen to have similar phonetic structures l<strong>in</strong>ked to mean<strong>in</strong>gs and that this<br />

"tie" lately has become productive, which would suggest that<br />

diachronically the phonesthemes would be arbitrary. In that case the<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g dimensions (see chapter 4) would not be so few and they ought to<br />

be more difficult to relate to each other. The relatable mean<strong>in</strong>g dimensions<br />

can be structure <strong>in</strong>ternal proofs (coherence).<br />

The expression has universal traits (especially if the mean<strong>in</strong>g of the<br />

phonestheme can be related to onomatopoeia.) The content of the<br />

phonesthemes is partly universal and partly language specific because of an<br />

<strong>in</strong>teraction between e.g. <strong>in</strong>nateness on the one hand and environment on the<br />

other (see discussion on explanations of sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> 1.11)<br />

The phonesthemes that appear from the lexical analysis (chapter 4) are<br />

prelim<strong>in</strong>ary until either there have appeared neologisms that support them,<br />

the more the better, or until they have found support <strong>in</strong> tests.<br />

Figure 2.3 can illustrate the problem of expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g other sound symbolism,<br />

i.e., phonesthemes which are neither iconic or <strong>in</strong>dexical at first sight.<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic expression<br />

?-relation<br />

Figure 2.3 Schematic illustration of other sound symbolism.<br />

58


There is a motivated relation between mean<strong>in</strong>g and form, but the question<br />

is: How should the relation illustrated by the arrow best be described?<br />

The analogous representation for a conventional, arbitrary sign would be:<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistic expression<br />

purely conventional relation<br />

Figure 2.4 Schematic illustration of a conventional, arbitrary sign.<br />

without an arrow, show<strong>in</strong>g that the form is not motivated by the mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(see figure 2.4). What exactly does then the arrow represent <strong>in</strong><br />

phonesthemes? It can be stated that it represents an ord<strong>in</strong>ary speakers'<br />

<strong>in</strong>tuition that the form is motivated by the content. In section 1.11 it was<br />

shown how this <strong>in</strong>tuition can be expla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> different ways, and it was<br />

suggested that the relation is <strong>in</strong> fact <strong>in</strong>dexical or iconic. It can be concluded<br />

that most of the explanations discussed <strong>in</strong> 1.11 support the idea that the<br />

relation is <strong>in</strong>dexical, iconic or a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of both.<br />

2.6. Considerations for a model<br />

As concerns more specified explanations an eclectic approach to<br />

explanation of sound symbolism would be to say that most of the<br />

explanations discussed <strong>in</strong> 1.11 could be valid, either simultaneously or for<br />

different types of sound symbolism.<br />

However, some explanations seem more plausible than others. One basic<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>ction is that between <strong>in</strong>nateness and learn<strong>in</strong>g. Innateness can be of at<br />

least three different k<strong>in</strong>ds. It can mean: 1) that there are <strong>in</strong>nate abilities by<br />

nature of biological endowment, 2) that there are <strong>in</strong>nate specific<br />

59


predispositions for the ability, which develops <strong>in</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong> way over time,<br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction with the environment, and 3) that there are <strong>in</strong>nate nonspecific<br />

predispositions, the development of which heavily relies on the<br />

environment. Learn<strong>in</strong>g is compatible with the last two types of <strong>in</strong>nateness.<br />

It seems that there are recurr<strong>in</strong>g semantic features, which also are easy to<br />

relate to each other. This suggests that a model of <strong>in</strong>nate predispositions for<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong> connections between expression and content is the most<br />

plausible one. If there were no such predispositions, the semantic<br />

categories <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism (if there were sound symbolism at<br />

all) would most probably be haphazard. As we will see <strong>in</strong> chapter 4 the<br />

semantic categories <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism are not unlimited but restricted to<br />

a number of types. In the analysis of sound symbolic words (the method of<br />

excerption of words is described <strong>in</strong> chapter 3) the follow<strong>in</strong>g categories,<br />

which are differentiated by their mean<strong>in</strong>g, were found:<br />

<strong>Sound</strong><br />

Movement<br />

Light<br />

Surface structure<br />

Consistency (plasticity)<br />

Wetness<br />

Dryness<br />

Attitude<br />

Slang<br />

Jocular<br />

Pejorative<br />

Mental feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Separation<br />

Putt<strong>in</strong>g together (convergence)<br />

Dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

Augmentative<br />

Form<br />

Iterative<br />

60


It is obvious that some of the categories seem to be of similar types, and<br />

they can be structured <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g way (figure 2.5):<br />

61


cognition spatiality repetition<br />

sense attitude mental bodily structure size form iterative<br />

impr. feel<strong>in</strong>g feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sound attitude pejorative separation dim<strong>in</strong>utive augmentative<br />

movement slang putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

light jocular (convergence)<br />

surface<br />

structure<br />

consistency<br />

(plasticity)<br />

wetness<br />

dryness<br />

Figure 2.5 Relations between the recurr<strong>in</strong>g semantic categories <strong>in</strong> Swedish sound symbolism.


Most of the categories can also be described as related to sense impressions<br />

<strong>in</strong> a more or less direct way. The categories are either related to one sense<br />

or to several senses. The simpler cases are 'sound', related to hear<strong>in</strong>g, 'light'<br />

and 'movement' related to see<strong>in</strong>g and 'consistency' related to touch. The<br />

experience of 'surface structure', 'wetness' or 'dryness' is a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of<br />

tactile, auditive and visual experience. 'Form' <strong>in</strong>volves the senses see<strong>in</strong>g<br />

and touch<strong>in</strong>g. 'Separation' and 'putt<strong>in</strong>g together' have a more abstract<br />

relation to the expression. 'Dim<strong>in</strong>utive' and 'augmentative' are also more<br />

abstract (but can be expla<strong>in</strong>ed with reference to the frequency code, cf.<br />

discussion <strong>in</strong> 1.10.5.) as well as 'iterative' (but it is clearly iconic). F<strong>in</strong>ally<br />

there are the expressive categories of mental and bodily emotion and<br />

attitude which are cognitive categories.<br />

The categories are not mutually exclusive. E.g. 'cognition' and 'spatiality'<br />

could be comb<strong>in</strong>ed e.g. <strong>in</strong> prepositions like <strong>in</strong> and other basic relations.<br />

'Mental feel<strong>in</strong>g' and 'bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g' can comb<strong>in</strong>e. However, for the analysis<br />

of significant properties of phonesthemes the above categorisation is<br />

useful.<br />

The categories are identical to or comparable with the semantic categories<br />

listed <strong>in</strong> the end of 1.10.4, which were based on the studies of different<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guists. The only really new classes are 'separation' (e.g. spl- spl<strong>in</strong>ta,<br />

split, splits, splitter, spr- spreta, sprida) and 'putt<strong>in</strong>g together' (e.g. knknipa,<br />

knyta, knyckla, tv- tv<strong>in</strong>g, tv<strong>in</strong>na).<br />

2.6.1 Relations between the categories<br />

The onomatopoeic free morphemes or phonesthemes are <strong>in</strong> a special class<br />

s<strong>in</strong>ce the iconicity concerns language sounds imitat<strong>in</strong>g sounds. Almost all<br />

the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters correspond to an onomatopoeic phonestheme.<br />

This means that this category is co-occurr<strong>in</strong>g with many of the other<br />

categories, e.g. many consonant clusters can be both 'onomatopoeic' and<br />

'pejorative', however not necessarily <strong>in</strong> the same root morpheme (see<br />

further discussion <strong>in</strong> chapter 4).<br />

63


One semantic category, 'light', can be classified as a metaphorical extension<br />

from sound phenomena. The basis for extension is sense analogy - an<br />

iconic phenomenon based on some sort of similarity, e.g. the word warm<br />

can be used not only for temperature but also for colour ('light') and tones<br />

('sound') i. e. us<strong>in</strong>g phonesthemes of one sensory modality for words of<br />

another sensory modality. (cf Abel<strong>in</strong> 1988). (This is not the same as<br />

synesthesia - a neurological, <strong>in</strong>dexical phenomenon, e.g. the experience of<br />

hear<strong>in</strong>g a certa<strong>in</strong> tone because of tast<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g that is salty, cf. 1.11.2).<br />

Appeal<strong>in</strong>g to sense analogy we can expla<strong>in</strong> the category of 'light'. Sense<br />

analogy concerns conventional relations between words. These relations<br />

are there because we see some sort of similarity between a particular type<br />

of sound and a particular type of light. Underly<strong>in</strong>g sense analogy (as well<br />

as synaesthesia and sound symbolism) could be "stable <strong>in</strong>termodal<br />

connections". An example of sense analogy with sound as a po<strong>in</strong>t of<br />

departure is gn-, as <strong>in</strong> gnälla (squeak), which can be seen as extended to<br />

gn- <strong>in</strong> gnistra (glimmer) <strong>in</strong> a fashion similar to the mean<strong>in</strong>g extension of<br />

e.g. the words skrikig (loud), gäll (shrill), dov (dull) which orig<strong>in</strong>ally<br />

described sounds but later also colours.<br />

Many of the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g categories can be expla<strong>in</strong>ed satisfactorily with the<br />

metaphoric and metonymical model of Lakoff and Johnson (1980) and<br />

Lakoff (1987). <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism can be seen as depend<strong>in</strong>g on<br />

1) <strong>in</strong>nate capacities for metaphor (similarity, icon), especially<br />

personification, e.g. gr- 'negative mood' and for metonymy (contiguity,<br />

<strong>in</strong>dex) i. e. closeness <strong>in</strong> time and space, e.g. gl- denotes both 'light' and<br />

'form lett<strong>in</strong>g through light').<br />

and on<br />

2) the result of learn<strong>in</strong>g from one's senses through use of the <strong>in</strong>nate<br />

capacities, (e. g. how someth<strong>in</strong>g sounds when mov<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong> way)<br />

where the <strong>in</strong>nate capacity for metaphor is put to use.<br />

This model seems to <strong>in</strong>corporate the ideas of both Cytowic and Brown, as<br />

described above.<br />

64


Lakoff (1987, p. 8) writes "human categorization is essentially a matter of<br />

both human experience and imag<strong>in</strong>ation - of perception, motor activity and<br />

culture on the one hand, and of metaphor, metonymy and mental imagery<br />

on the other." This would also expla<strong>in</strong> why sound symbolism is partly<br />

universal, partly language specific. Universality from the semantic<br />

perspective depends among other th<strong>in</strong>gs on human capacity for metaphor<br />

and metonymy; experience is shaped by metaphor and metonymy.<br />

Lakoff and Johnsson (1980) write: "the most obvious ontological<br />

metaphors are those where the physical object is further specified as be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

a person." How does this apply to sound symbolism? <strong>Sound</strong>s of language<br />

naturally cannot be seen as persons, but they can be seen (heard) as<br />

belong<strong>in</strong>g to persons of a certa<strong>in</strong> type, mood etc. Do<strong>in</strong>g this is a k<strong>in</strong>d of<br />

personification, which is a basic conceptual strategy. The material this<br />

personification is work<strong>in</strong>g on is based on our experience of how people<br />

sound when of a certa<strong>in</strong> type, <strong>in</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong> mood etc. This personification<br />

could expla<strong>in</strong> the 'negative mood' apply<strong>in</strong>g to persons.<br />

The categories Movement, Form, Consistency, Surface structure, Wetness,<br />

Dryness and Dim<strong>in</strong>utive are simply expla<strong>in</strong>ed through metonymy<br />

(closeness <strong>in</strong> time and space) between sound and manipulation of objects<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g the language learn<strong>in</strong>g process. Already Brown (1958) said that what<br />

was mysteriously called physiognomic (by Werner, 1953) is a result and<br />

consequence of a simple act of learn<strong>in</strong>g. In everyday experience there is a<br />

correlation between certa<strong>in</strong> physical attributes of objects and the noises<br />

produced by these objects. A large object is more likely to produce a deep<br />

sound than a small object when it is pushed or when it falls. The<br />

relationship between size and deep sounds can therefore be learned; equally<br />

a connection between sharpness and high frequency and between roundness<br />

or bluntness and a low frequency, cf. however Ohala (1994).<br />

Werner (1953) <strong>in</strong>troduced the concept of physiognomy. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to him,<br />

<strong>in</strong> the primitive perception of animals, children etc. th<strong>in</strong>gs are not treated<br />

objectively, but physiognomically, i. e. as if they expressed an <strong>in</strong>ner life<br />

and had a m<strong>in</strong>d; a landscape is cheerful, a cup on its side tired etc. Werner,<br />

<strong>in</strong> contrast to Lakoff and Johnsson (1980) treated this as an exception.<br />

65


Werner and Kaplan (1963) developed a genetic theory for symbol<br />

formation accord<strong>in</strong>g to which objects are expressive, e. g. two headlights<br />

look like eyes, music can be threaten<strong>in</strong>g. This transcendence of expressive<br />

qualities does not only form the basis for analogies, metaphors and similes,<br />

it is also the basis for see<strong>in</strong>g similarities <strong>in</strong> such unrelated th<strong>in</strong>gs as rounded<br />

shapes <strong>in</strong> a draw<strong>in</strong>g and the word "maluma".<br />

Lakoff and Johnsson's theory is <strong>in</strong> agreement with both Brown, Werner and<br />

Kaplan, and Cytowic (i. e. sound symbolism is formed by experience of the<br />

world (learn<strong>in</strong>g) (cf. Brown, 1958) but it is also formed by predispositions<br />

of perception (Werner and Kaplan, 1963, Cytowic, 1989). This po<strong>in</strong>ts to<br />

the conclusion that sound symbolism works <strong>in</strong> much the same way as other<br />

concepts except that much more is dependent on iconicity and <strong>in</strong>dexicality.<br />

2.6.2 An explanatory model for sound symbolism<br />

From the considerations discussed above a number of dimensions for a<br />

model of sound symbolism can be extracted. The general background to the<br />

model is the framework presented <strong>in</strong> 1.9, e.g. sound symbolism is central to<br />

language, it can be <strong>in</strong>nate or conventional, it is productive and context<br />

sensitive. The present model tries to expla<strong>in</strong> the nature of the relation<br />

between mean<strong>in</strong>g and expression, "the arrow with a question-mark". Some<br />

dimensions from the model <strong>in</strong> 1.9 then appear aga<strong>in</strong>, e.g. <strong>in</strong>nateness vs.<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g (the latter connected with conventionality). The dimensions are the<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g:<br />

1. <strong>in</strong>nateness<br />

2. learn<strong>in</strong>g<br />

3. icon (metaphor)<br />

4. <strong>in</strong>dex (metonymy)<br />

5. conventionalization<br />

6. the sound symbolic categories:<br />

<strong>Sound</strong><br />

Movement<br />

Light<br />

Surface structure<br />

66


Consistency (plasticity)<br />

Wetness<br />

Dryness<br />

Attitude<br />

Slang<br />

Jocular<br />

Pejorative<br />

Mental feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Separation<br />

Putt<strong>in</strong>g together (convergence)<br />

Dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

Augmentative<br />

Form<br />

Iterative<br />

The sound symbolic categories concern Swedish s<strong>in</strong>ce they build on the<br />

analysis of the Swedish lexical material. In a description of another<br />

language they might be slightly different but they would be expla<strong>in</strong>able <strong>in</strong><br />

terms of icon and <strong>in</strong>dex and mostly relatable to sense impressions or<br />

emotions.<br />

<strong>in</strong>nateness<br />

(icon,<strong>in</strong>dex)<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sound<br />

symbolism<br />

Figure 2.6 An explanatory model for sound symbolism, which shows the<br />

cause of the motivated relation between expression and mean<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

67<br />

conventionalization<br />

phonesthemes<br />

for<br />

onomatopoeia<br />

pejoratives,<br />

surface<br />

structure,<br />

movement,<br />

form, etc.


The model <strong>in</strong> figure 2.6 tries to capture the relations between the different<br />

concepts that are fundamental to sound symbolism. It says that the relation<br />

between expression and content <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism can be caused by<br />

either <strong>in</strong>nateness or learn<strong>in</strong>g. What is predisposed can be affected by<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g, and what is learned is depend<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>in</strong>nate categories for th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

such as <strong>in</strong>dex (metonymy) or icon (metaphor). <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is the<br />

result of both <strong>in</strong>nate capacities and learn<strong>in</strong>g. Some types of sound<br />

symbolism depend more on an <strong>in</strong>dexical relation (connected with learn<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

e.g. phonesthemes for 'movement', 'form' etc. while other types of sound<br />

symbolism depend more on an iconic relation, e.g. phonesthemes for<br />

'onomatopoeia' and perhaps 'pejoratives', but these have nevertheless been<br />

put <strong>in</strong> the same box. The model also shows that sound symbolic expression,<br />

and maybe also conent, is always conventionalized, to a greater or lesser<br />

degree. This expla<strong>in</strong>s the fact that e.g. words for a certa<strong>in</strong> animal call can<br />

differ between languages at the same times as it is motivated. The model<br />

can be seen as a static model where the processes to the left are necessary<br />

prerequisites for the existence of phonesthemes. It could also be seen as a<br />

basis for how sound symbolic expressions have emerged <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>teraction with<strong>in</strong> a language system, with the result of sound symbolic<br />

expressions <strong>in</strong> that particular language.<br />

The concepts iconicity and <strong>in</strong>dexicality can be used for describ<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

nature of the different explanations of the relation between expression and<br />

form <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism described above. The concepts iconicity and<br />

<strong>in</strong>dexicality (superord<strong>in</strong>ate to metaphor and metonymy) can also be used<br />

for describ<strong>in</strong>g the semantic relations between the semantic features<br />

reoccurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism (cf. 2.6) The relations between some of the<br />

semantic categories will be further specified <strong>in</strong> 4.6. Figure 2.6 An<br />

explanatory model for sound symbolism, which shows the cause of the<br />

motivated relation between expression and mean<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The model <strong>in</strong> figure 2.6 tries to capture the relations between the different<br />

concepts that are fundamental to sound symbolism. It says that the relation<br />

between expression and content <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism can be caused by<br />

either <strong>in</strong>nateness or learn<strong>in</strong>g. What is predisposed can be affected by<br />

68


learn<strong>in</strong>g, and what is learned is depend<strong>in</strong>g on <strong>in</strong>nate categories for th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

such as <strong>in</strong>dex (metonymy) or icon (metaphor). <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism is the<br />

result of both <strong>in</strong>nate capacities and learn<strong>in</strong>g. Some types of sound<br />

symbolism depend more on an <strong>in</strong>dexical relation (connected with learn<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

e.g. phonesthemes for 'movement', 'form' etc. while other types of sound<br />

symbolism depend more on an iconic relation, e.g. phonesthemes for<br />

'onomatopoeia' and perhaps 'pejoratives', but these have nevertheless been<br />

put <strong>in</strong> the same box. The model also shows that sound symbolic expression,<br />

and maybe also conent, is always conventionalized, to a greater or lesser<br />

degree. This expla<strong>in</strong>s the fact that e.g. words for a certa<strong>in</strong> animal call can<br />

differ between languages at the same times as it is motivated. The model<br />

can be seen as a static model where the processes to the left are necessary<br />

prerequisites for the existence of phonesthemes. It could also be seen as a<br />

basis for how sound symbolic expressions have emerged <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>teraction with<strong>in</strong> a language system, with the result of sound symbolic<br />

expressions <strong>in</strong> that particular language.<br />

The concepts iconicity and <strong>in</strong>dexicality can be used for describ<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

nature of the different explanations of the relation between expression and<br />

form <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism described above. The concepts iconicity and<br />

<strong>in</strong>dexicality (superord<strong>in</strong>ate to metaphor and metonymy) can also be used<br />

for describ<strong>in</strong>g the semantic relations between the semantic features<br />

reoccurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism (cf. 2.6) The relations between some of the<br />

semantic categories will be further specified <strong>in</strong> 4.6.<br />

69


3 Method<br />

The method used <strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>vestigation is a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of <strong>in</strong>tuition and<br />

empirical studies (experiments). Us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tuition about language is an<br />

<strong>in</strong>dispensable part of all l<strong>in</strong>guistic research, as the researcher cannot avoid<br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g some knowledge of the subject. However, <strong>in</strong>tuition is not<br />

sufficient, and will therefore be supported by empirical studies.<br />

This <strong>in</strong>vestigation of sound symbolic words <strong>in</strong> language is concentrated on<br />

<strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters <strong>in</strong> Swedish. It consists of three ma<strong>in</strong><br />

stages.<br />

1. A number of Swedish dictionaries were excerpted with the purpose of<br />

establish<strong>in</strong>g tentative phonesthemes for Swedish. The material collected<br />

was stored <strong>in</strong> spreadsheats, which could be manually searched for roots,<br />

key words and semantic features.<br />

2. Stage 2, which is a smaller part of the study, consists of partial<br />

excerptions from other languages. The excerptions were done us<strong>in</strong>g<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs, or, <strong>in</strong> some cases, phonesthemes as the po<strong>in</strong>t of departure.<br />

3. A number of tests were performed <strong>in</strong> order to study the tentative<br />

Swedish phonesthemes found <strong>in</strong> stage 1.<br />

Below follows an elaboration of the three stages. In each chapter there is a<br />

more detailed description of the method.<br />

3.1 Stage 1: Collection of lexical material<br />

Initial consonant clusters<br />

S<strong>in</strong>ce the study has its po<strong>in</strong>t of departure <strong>in</strong> the Swedish language it was<br />

<strong>in</strong>itiated by an excerption of sound symbolic words <strong>in</strong> Swedish. The<br />

lexical material consists of excerptions from Svenska Akademiens Ordlista<br />

(SAOL 10, The Wordlist of the Swedish Academy), a word list with some<br />

semantic <strong>in</strong>formation, and Svensk ordbok (SOB, Dictionary of Swedish),<br />

a dictionary with def<strong>in</strong>itions, exemplifications etc.<br />

71


The excerption of words from SOB was done <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g way. To<br />

qualify as a candidate for be<strong>in</strong>g a possibly <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g word, one or two of<br />

the follow<strong>in</strong>g criteria should be met:<br />

(i) The word is clearly onomatopoeic.<br />

(ii) At least two 1 word roots with the same consonant sequence and<br />

similar mean<strong>in</strong>g can be found <strong>in</strong> the lexical material, where the likeness<br />

does not come from trivial morphological relatedness like derivation of<br />

e.g. nouns or adjectives. As an example, fjant, fjanta, fjantig are counted<br />

as forms of the same root. Kladd, kladda och kladdig are another<br />

example. In other words: compounds, derivations or words belong<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

different parts of speech are seen as different <strong>in</strong>stances of one root<br />

morpheme (conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a phonestheme).<br />

Through the method described above there is no absolute guarantee that<br />

all possible phonesthemes for Swedish are found. Those that are found are<br />

chosen with approximately the same criteria. The emphasis has been on an<br />

analysis of all words, rather than a detailed analysis of a few words. The<br />

purpose of this is to get an overview of the ma<strong>in</strong> traits <strong>in</strong> the Swedish<br />

lexical material.<br />

The Swedish lexicon (represented by SOB) was manually excerpted for<br />

root morphemes with onomatopoeic or other sound symbolic mean<strong>in</strong>gs,<br />

(motivated words.) Only words beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with consonant clusters were<br />

considered <strong>in</strong> this excerption.<br />

"Key words" from the SOB were also registered <strong>in</strong> the excerption. A key<br />

word is a formal representative of the sense connected to a root<br />

morpheme and usually denotes e.g. 'sound', 'shape', 'texture' (sense<br />

related categories), 'pejorative' etc.<br />

A key word is either a word <strong>in</strong> the def<strong>in</strong>ition of the actual word, or a<br />

word <strong>in</strong> the def<strong>in</strong>ition of a word which is <strong>in</strong> the def<strong>in</strong>ition of the actual<br />

word, or another word <strong>in</strong> the paraphrase of a more peripheral men<strong>in</strong>g of<br />

the actual word, or a synonym. Ideally the key word should have been <strong>in</strong><br />

the def<strong>in</strong>ition of the actual word, but unfortunately SOB is not completely<br />

1 For lexically <strong>in</strong>frequent clusters, with less than 13 roots, 1 root sufficed if it had one of<br />

the recurr<strong>in</strong>g semantic features.<br />

72


consistent <strong>in</strong> its way of giv<strong>in</strong>g def<strong>in</strong>itions. Sometimes a word is only given<br />

a very short def<strong>in</strong>ition, e.g. a synonym.<br />

The key word is always a word which is written <strong>in</strong>to the def<strong>in</strong>ition of<br />

the lexicon entry of the word under consideration and, <strong>in</strong> a most cases, a<br />

word <strong>in</strong> the def<strong>in</strong>ition of the actual word.<br />

This function of key words is to ensure a certa<strong>in</strong> amount of objectivity,<br />

through consensus, <strong>in</strong> the selection procedure. It depends then on earlier<br />

semantic analyses by several persons. Key words are sound symbolic<br />

categories or hyponyms to these. They can be found <strong>in</strong> Appendix 1.<br />

Examples of key words from SOB are: ljud (sound), rörelse (movement),<br />

spetsig (po<strong>in</strong>ted), ljus (light), äcklig (nasty), vatten (water), tjock (thick),<br />

klibbig, (sticky), ohyfsad (rude), löjlig (ridiculous). Naturally there were<br />

expectations as to what semantic categories would be <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g; the key<br />

words are related to the senses, to 'form', 'mental feel<strong>in</strong>g' etc. Words<br />

with def<strong>in</strong>itions conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g appropriate key words were, however, not<br />

considered if a connection between expression and mean<strong>in</strong>g was not felt,<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>tuition, as e.g. for skam. No part of the expression side<br />

of this word seems to mirror the words oförmåga or förkastlig of the<br />

def<strong>in</strong>ition. The opposite is also the case; a connection seemed to be present<br />

but an appropriate key word was not to be found <strong>in</strong> the def<strong>in</strong>ition. 2<br />

Some words which can be found <strong>in</strong> SAOL 10 are described as colloquial,<br />

while they <strong>in</strong> fact are out of date. Nevertheless they are not difficult to get<br />

a vague understand<strong>in</strong>g of. Examples are words like pjask, pjasker and<br />

pjalt 3 that have a clearly pejorative r<strong>in</strong>g. These and the words of the<br />

previous paragraph could probably easily be (mis)<strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> an<br />

appropriate context!<br />

With an attitude of accept<strong>in</strong>g doubtful words rather than discard<strong>in</strong>g them,<br />

about 1,000 words – <strong>in</strong> the sense of root morphemes – were registered<br />

for further analysis. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Nusvensk frekvensordbok 4 (Allén et<br />

2 There are also words that, from the po<strong>in</strong>t of view of sound, would seem to be<br />

appropriate for mirror<strong>in</strong>g a certa<strong>in</strong> content but do not. Examples are fläns (part of a<br />

mach<strong>in</strong>e), skralna (to beg<strong>in</strong> to head, of w<strong>in</strong>d; sail<strong>in</strong>g term), and slubb (sort of sp<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g<br />

mach<strong>in</strong>e).<br />

3 In SAOL 12 these words are no longer <strong>in</strong>cluded.<br />

73


al, 1980) the number of lexical morphemes <strong>in</strong> Swedish amount to around<br />

8,300 (suffixes add another 562 morphemes).<br />

It is important to keep <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, both what concerns the data collection and<br />

the data analysis, that the borders between the different mean<strong>in</strong>gs are<br />

fuzzy and partly subjective, and the result<strong>in</strong>g diagrams and tables are to<br />

be seen as po<strong>in</strong>ts of departure for different <strong>in</strong>vestigations, experiments<br />

and discussions. The primary aim of the data collection is to get an<br />

overview of the structure of the clusters and to show patterns of forms<br />

and contents.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>al clusters<br />

F<strong>in</strong>al consonant comb<strong>in</strong>ations with recurr<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gs were also<br />

excerpted from Svensk baklängesordbok (1981) (Reverse Order<br />

Dictionary of Swedish) and Nusvensk frekvensordbok (Allén et al, 1980)<br />

(Frequency Dictionary of Present-Day Swedish). They were analyzed <strong>in</strong> a<br />

fashion similar to the one described above.<br />

3.2 Stage 2: Cross-language comparisons<br />

With a focus on both the expression and the content side of phonesthemes,<br />

some comparisons with other languages were done. The emphasis was on<br />

certa<strong>in</strong> easily def<strong>in</strong>ed mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Also, some contrastive experiments of<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation, test<strong>in</strong>g Swedish phonesthemes on native speakers of other<br />

languages, were made.<br />

3.2.1 Cross-language thesaurus studies<br />

The lexical study consists of two studies, one <strong>in</strong> Swedish and one <strong>in</strong><br />

English. It is concentrated on the semantic fields of 'stupidity' and<br />

'surface structure' – 'rough' and 'smooth'. These fields were chosen<br />

because they were different <strong>in</strong> type, they were quite common ('stupidity'<br />

is a subclass of 'pejorative') and also quite uncomplicated semantically.<br />

There were three <strong>in</strong>formants from each language.<br />

3.2.2 Cross-language <strong>in</strong>formant studies<br />

Self-imitative <strong>in</strong>terjections<br />

The Swedish primary <strong>in</strong>terjections (see Ideforss, 1928) have been<br />

translated to 8 different languages with the help of <strong>in</strong>formants. The<br />

languages are Icelandic, English, Polish, Hungarian, F<strong>in</strong>nish, Ososo,<br />

74


Malagasi and Slovenian. The material is presented <strong>in</strong> Appendix 2 and it is<br />

grouped accord<strong>in</strong>g to type of <strong>in</strong>terjection. In some cases it was difficult to<br />

translate word for word. The semantic contents of an <strong>in</strong>terjection <strong>in</strong>stead<br />

had to be described (with semantic features) after which the <strong>in</strong>formant<br />

gave the closest correspondence <strong>in</strong> her own language.<br />

Interjections imitat<strong>in</strong>g animal sounds<br />

Swedish <strong>in</strong>terjections imitat<strong>in</strong>g animal calls were translated, with the help<br />

of <strong>in</strong>formants, <strong>in</strong>to 17 different languages: Icelandic, English, Polish,<br />

Hungarian, F<strong>in</strong>nish, Ososo, Malagasi, Slovenian, Korean, Japanese,<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, Estonian, Urdu, Persian, Kurdish, Arabic, and Spanish.<br />

Interpretation of animal sounds<br />

Sixteen of the <strong>in</strong>terjections imitat<strong>in</strong>g animal calls from different languages<br />

were chosen to test how speakers of other languages would <strong>in</strong>terpret<br />

them. The ones chosen were those whose expression sides were most<br />

different between the languages studied, <strong>in</strong> order to make the task as<br />

difficult as possible. The subjects <strong>in</strong> this test were 15 persons, with the<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g first languages: Swedish (8 subjects) French (2 subjects),<br />

English, Hungarian, Czech, Slovenian, Ososo. All were tested on the same<br />

occasion. They listened to pronunciations of the words for the animal calls<br />

and they saw them transcribed <strong>in</strong> IPA. They were told to guess which<br />

animal had gotten its call conventionalized <strong>in</strong> each way, and to write down<br />

their answers.<br />

Cross-language experiment<br />

Furthermore, one person represent<strong>in</strong>g each of the languages: Arabic,<br />

Spanish, German, Dutch, Ibo and English, took part <strong>in</strong> an experimental<br />

study of persons who did not have Swedish as a first language. The study<br />

was conducted <strong>in</strong> much the same way as one of the experiments described<br />

below and <strong>in</strong> 7.1, (test 2.a) – a free choice test from expression to<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g. The ma<strong>in</strong> difference was that the subjects were confronted<br />

mostly with real words. There were, however, also a few neologisms<br />

based on phonesthemes.<br />

75


3.3 Stage 3: Experiments<br />

3.3.1 Experiments with neologisms<br />

To further penetrate the status and productivity of the tentative Swedish<br />

phonesthemes of language users of today, a number of tests were<br />

performed (see Appendix 3).<br />

The tests <strong>in</strong>vestigated both production and understand<strong>in</strong>g of written<br />

neologisms, which were, <strong>in</strong> three of the tests, modelled on the tentative<br />

phonesthemes. The tests consisted of free and forced choice tasks. There<br />

was was an additional match<strong>in</strong>g test where two neologisms were to be<br />

matched with two mean<strong>in</strong>gs. The subjects were 14–15 native Swedish<br />

speakers. In one test they were <strong>in</strong>structed to freely <strong>in</strong>terpret neologisms<br />

and <strong>in</strong> another they were supposed to produce neologisms out of different<br />

concepts written on the test sheet. In the next set of tests the subjects<br />

should, <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g neologisms, chose from three alternative mean<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

written on the test sheet, or, <strong>in</strong> produc<strong>in</strong>g neologisms, choose among three<br />

alternative neologisms, also written on the test sheet. The design of the<br />

experiments is described <strong>in</strong> detail <strong>in</strong> 7.1.<br />

3.4 Further method<br />

Explanatory models for sound symbolism are also constructed, see 1.9,<br />

2.6.2. These models aim at expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the nature of the motivated<br />

connection between expression and mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism.<br />

Comparisons between the results of the lexical studies, the experiments –<br />

also the cross-language experiments – some of the cross language<br />

comparisons 4 ,and the models are made <strong>in</strong> chapter 8.<br />

4 Interjections and animal sounds of the cross language comparisons belong to the classes<br />

expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections and onomatopoeia which are not <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the models of 1.9<br />

and 2.6.2.<br />

76


4 Analysis of the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters<br />

4.1. Data analysis, a short overview<br />

All the entries of the lexicon Svensk Ordbok (1986) were exam<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

(around 65, 000 lexemes, which corresponds to approximately 8, 300 1<br />

morphemes, which is the number of morphemes <strong>in</strong> Swedish accord<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

Allén et al, 1980). Around 1,250 words (root morphemes, cf. 3.1) were<br />

judged to be possibly motivated.<br />

Of all the 37 <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters of Swedish all but one are used for<br />

sound symbolism. The 37 clusters are: bj-, bl-, br-, dr-, dv-, fj-, fl-,<br />

fn-, fr-, gl-, gn-, gr-, kl-, kn-, kr-, kv-, mj-, nj-, pj-, pl-, pr-,<br />

sk-, skr-, skv-, sl-, sm-, sn-, sp-, spj-, spl-, spr-, st-, str-, sv-,<br />

tr-, tv-, vr-. The unused one (dv- ) has a lexical frequency of 3<br />

morphemes. Thus, almost all clusters seem to be used, but they are used<br />

to vary<strong>in</strong>g extents and for different purposes.<br />

(Now and then one hears authentic examples of e.g. onomatopoeic<br />

expressions which use non-standard clusters (cf. Garlén (1988) like<br />

sklofsa ("walk <strong>in</strong> mud"). It is also the case that a cluster like spl-, which<br />

normally is not onomatopoeic <strong>in</strong> Swedish, is often used for imitat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sounds, for example <strong>in</strong> comic strips – perhaps <strong>in</strong>fluenced by English. In<br />

comic strips expressions like splofs and splafs are common.)<br />

All <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters were analyzed closely, both those that appear<br />

to have a greater and those that seem to have a smaller amount of<br />

motivated root morphemes (cf. 3.1).<br />

For each cluster, each motivated root morpheme was classified accord<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to its motivated semantic features. (cf. 2.6). A root morpheme can have<br />

one or more motivated semantic features, e.g. skvalpa (lap, ripple, splash,<br />

spill) has 'sound', 'wetness'. Even though part of the mean<strong>in</strong>g might also<br />

be due to -alpa and not only to skv-, such possibilities were not<br />

1This number of morphemes is the result of an <strong>in</strong>vestigation of a one million word<br />

newspaper corpus (NFO 4). It is most probable that newspaper language conta<strong>in</strong>s fewer<br />

roots than spoken language, but these figures are not known at present. The estimation of<br />

65, 000 lexemes is exclud<strong>in</strong>g transparent compounds.<br />

77


considered at this stage. 2 The semantic features 'wetness' and 'sound' of<br />

skvalpa were, <strong>in</strong> this analysis, attributed to the cluster skv- because there<br />

are two or more root morphemes beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with skv- that have the same<br />

semantic features. The SOB-def<strong>in</strong>ition of skvalpa is: "vara i rörelse och<br />

därvid ge ifrån sig ett kluckande och plaskande ljud – om vatten o.d."(my<br />

italics) ("to be mov<strong>in</strong>g and thereby emit a gurgl<strong>in</strong>g and splash<strong>in</strong>g sound –<br />

of water, etc")<br />

The SOB-def<strong>in</strong>itions of other skv- lexemes are:<br />

skvimpa (splash to and fro) "skvalpa med små rörelser – ofta så att vätska<br />

spills ut" (The semantic features are the same as for skvalpa (lap with<br />

small movements – often so that liquid is spilled out), with the addition of<br />

'dim<strong>in</strong>utive' – which is probably due to the i, cf. Ultan, 1978.)<br />

skvätta (splash, squirt) "fara i väg i skvättar" (go off <strong>in</strong> squirts), skvätt:<br />

"liten mängd vätska" (small amount of liquid) The semantic features are<br />

'movement', 'dim<strong>in</strong>utive', 'wetness'.<br />

skvala (pour, gush, rush) "r<strong>in</strong>na rikligt och ljudligt (flow abundantly and<br />

noisily). R<strong>in</strong>na (flow) is def<strong>in</strong>ed as "förflytta sig nedåt (längs viss yta) i<br />

sammanhängande formlös mängd – om vätska e.d." (move downwards<br />

(along a certa<strong>in</strong> surface) <strong>in</strong> a cont<strong>in</strong>uous shapeless quantity – about liquid,<br />

etc) and riklig (abundant) is def<strong>in</strong>ed as "förekommande i stor mängd eller<br />

omfattn<strong>in</strong>g" (occurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> large quantity or range) and so the semantic<br />

features of skvala are 'movement', 'wetness', 'sound' and 'augmentative'.<br />

As we see the features 'movement', 'wetness' and 'sound' are all present<br />

<strong>in</strong> three of these four words. There are around eight more words<br />

(consider<strong>in</strong>g the root morphemes) beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with skv-.<br />

(The cluster -Vlpa ends the follow<strong>in</strong>g words (consider<strong>in</strong>g the root<br />

morphemes): valpa (whelp), skvalpa (lap), stolpa (walk with long paces),<br />

pulpa (pulp), hjälpa (help), stjälpa (tip over), skölpa (hollow out with a<br />

special tool) – no other root morphemes with the features 'wetness' or<br />

'sound', but 4 root morphemes with the feature 'movement'.)<br />

2 For an analysis of comb<strong>in</strong>ed effects of <strong>in</strong>itial cluster, f<strong>in</strong>al clusters and, to some extent,<br />

of vowels, see 5.6.<br />

78


Diagram 4.1 shows the number of motivated root morphemes for every<br />

cluster. The number of all root morphemes and of the motivated root<br />

morphemes for each cluster can be studied <strong>in</strong> diagrams 4.2 and 4.3. Also<br />

the ratios between the number of sound symbolic root morphemes on the<br />

one hand (irrespective of exact mean<strong>in</strong>g) and total number of root<br />

morphemes beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with a certa<strong>in</strong> cluster was calculated, see diagram<br />

4.4.<br />

In addition to this, the various semantic features (cf. 4.2.3) for every<br />

cluster and the ratios for the occurrence of different semantic features per<br />

total number of root morphemes (for every cluster) were put <strong>in</strong>to the<br />

chart. These figures are used for the follow<strong>in</strong>g diagrams. 3<br />

Diagram 4.5 shows the absolute numbers of all <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters<br />

for all semantic features.<br />

In diagrams 4.6 to 4.15 the distributions of separate semantic features<br />

over different clusters can be seen.<br />

Diagrams 4.16 to 4.23 show the sound symbolic profiles of different<br />

clusters, i.e. how the semantic features are differently distributed.<br />

In addition to this, the relations between the different types of mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

with<strong>in</strong> a cluster were analyzed synchronically.<br />

4.2 Results<br />

In this section some of the results from the lexical analysis are presented.<br />

The same material will be studied from different angles and <strong>in</strong> greater or<br />

lesser detail. The focus will be either on the different consonant clusters,<br />

or on the different mean<strong>in</strong>gs. There will be general patterns and patterns<br />

for certa<strong>in</strong> clusters or mean<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

3 If all dialectal, archaic and slang words had been counted the ratios of motivated root<br />

morphemes would have been higher for most consonant clusters. (This reflects the<br />

phenomenon of slang words float<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> and out of language at another rate, cf. 1.3 - 1.4).<br />

As an example the follow<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes, found <strong>in</strong> Svenska Akademiens ordlista<br />

över svenska språket (SAOL 10), are not <strong>in</strong>cluded: skvattra "snattra"(quack, gabble,<br />

chatter), skrålla (coll.) "löjlig damhatt" (silly lady's hat), skryp (dial.) "slösaktig"<br />

(wasteful). The ma<strong>in</strong> reason for not count<strong>in</strong>g these is that they are not covered <strong>in</strong> SOB;<br />

special studies would have to be made of dialectal and slang dictionaries. There seem to<br />

be more dialectal and archaic words <strong>in</strong> SAOL than <strong>in</strong> SOB.<br />

79


4.2.1 More and less sound symbolic clusters<br />

The 36 <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters are very different with respect to how<br />

many root morphemes they conta<strong>in</strong>. They also differ considerably <strong>in</strong> how<br />

many sound symbolic root morphemes there are for each cluster.<br />

Diagram 4.1 shows the number of motivated root morphemes for all 36<br />

clusters.<br />

Diagram 4.1 shows e.g. that sl- has the highest number of sound symbolic<br />

clusters, namely 83, while nj- has the fewest, namely only 1. In absolute<br />

numbers, the 6 clusters that have most motivated root morphemes are (<strong>in</strong><br />

order of descend<strong>in</strong>g frequency): sl-, sn-, kn-, kr-, kl-, sp-. Of these 6<br />

clusters 3 beg<strong>in</strong> with an s and 3 beg<strong>in</strong> with a k. There is reason to wonder<br />

if nj- should be counted as a sound symbolic cluster at all, but nj-, as well<br />

as all other clusters, must be seen <strong>in</strong> the light of how many root<br />

morphemes there are <strong>in</strong> the total vocabulary for each cluster. This<br />

comparison is done <strong>in</strong> diagram 4.2.<br />

80


sl<br />

sn<br />

kn<br />

kr<br />

kl<br />

sp<br />

st<br />

gl<br />

tr<br />

fl<br />

skr<br />

pl<br />

bl<br />

sm<br />

str<br />

gr<br />

sk<br />

sv<br />

spr<br />

pr<br />

gn<br />

kv<br />

dr<br />

vr<br />

br<br />

fr<br />

fn<br />

skv<br />

mj<br />

fj<br />

pj<br />

spl<br />

bj<br />

spj<br />

tv<br />

nj<br />

0 20 40 60 80 100<br />

Diagram. 4.1. More and less sound symbolic <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters. Number of<br />

motivated root morphemes per cluster.<br />

81


st<br />

pr<br />

tr<br />

kr<br />

sl<br />

sp<br />

br<br />

sn<br />

gr<br />

sk<br />

kl<br />

pl<br />

fl<br />

fr<br />

str<br />

kn<br />

dr<br />

gl<br />

sv<br />

bl<br />

skr<br />

kv<br />

sm<br />

spr<br />

fj<br />

gn<br />

tv<br />

vr<br />

bj<br />

mj<br />

skv<br />

fn<br />

spl<br />

pj<br />

spj<br />

nj<br />

0 50 100 150 200 250 300<br />

82<br />

no of motiv roots<br />

all roots<br />

Diagram. 4.2 More and less sound symbolic consonant clusters.<br />

Number of root morphemes and number of motivated root<br />

morphemes per cluster.


Diagram 4.2 shows the proportions between total number of root<br />

morphemes and the number of motivated root morphemes. The diagram<br />

shows for example that the cluster pj- conta<strong>in</strong>s both few root morphemes<br />

and few motivated root morphemes. So there is a high proportion of<br />

motivated ones. It also shows that tr- has many root morphemes, quite a<br />

few motivated root morphemes, but a lower proportion of motivated root<br />

morphemes. For percentages of the proportion motivated root<br />

morphemes/all root morphemes, see diagram 4.4 (It should be noted that<br />

one motivated root morpheme may conta<strong>in</strong> one or more motivated<br />

semantic feature.)<br />

Diagram. 4.3 shows the same <strong>in</strong>formation as diagram 4.2. but here the<br />

clusters are ranked from the most sound symbolic to the least sound<br />

symbolic, as <strong>in</strong> 4.1. The diagram shows that lexically frequent clusters do<br />

not necessarily conta<strong>in</strong> a large proportion of sound symbolic morphemes,<br />

e.g. pr-. (A large proportion of the non-motivated morphemes beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g<br />

with pr- are due to loan words 4 and it is also quite difficult to count the<br />

morphemes.)<br />

Table 4.1 (related to diagrams 4.1, 4.2 and 4.3) shows the number of root<br />

morphemes and the number of motivated root morphemes for the 36<br />

clusters. The table is ordered alphabetically. The clusters are ranked from<br />

the most sound symbolic (<strong>in</strong> absolute numbers) to the least sound<br />

symbolic.<br />

4 It would be <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> further research, to study the <strong>in</strong>fluence of loan words on the<br />

frequencies of sound symbolic root morphemes per cluster. Words older than a 100<br />

years, etc. could easily be found with the aid of the mach<strong>in</strong>e readable version of SAOB<br />

(Ordbok över svenska språket, utgiven av Svenska Akademien). It could be hypothesized<br />

that there would be a higher frequency of sound symbolic root morphemes among the<br />

older root morphemes. On the other hand, root morphemes that confirm with the native<br />

pattern could be preferred <strong>in</strong> loans, cf. discussion <strong>in</strong> 1.5.)<br />

83


sl<br />

sn<br />

kn<br />

kr<br />

kl<br />

sp<br />

gl<br />

st<br />

tr<br />

fl<br />

skr<br />

pl<br />

bl<br />

sm<br />

str<br />

gr<br />

sk<br />

spr<br />

sv<br />

pr<br />

gn<br />

kv<br />

dr<br />

vr<br />

br<br />

fn<br />

fr<br />

skv<br />

mj<br />

fj<br />

pj<br />

spl<br />

bj<br />

spj<br />

tv<br />

nj<br />

0 50 100 150 200 250 300<br />

84<br />

no of motiv roots<br />

all roots<br />

Diagram 4.3. Number of root morphemes and number of motivated root<br />

morphemes per cluster, which are ordered from highest number of sound<br />

symbolic root morphemes to lowest number of sound symbolic root<br />

morphemes.


Table 4.1 Number of root morphemes and motivated root morphemes.<br />

total number of motivated root Rank<br />

root morphemes morphemes<br />

bj- 13 5 32<br />

bl- 64 33 13<br />

br- 130 11 25<br />

dr- 74 16 23<br />

fj- 24 7 29<br />

fl- 96 36 10<br />

fn- 10 10 26<br />

fr- 87 10 26<br />

gl- 69 45 7<br />

gn- 22 20 21<br />

gr- 126 26 16<br />

kl- 115 51 5<br />

kn- 77 59 3<br />

kr- 169 53 4<br />

kv- 56 17 22<br />

mj- 11 7 29<br />

nj- 3 1 36<br />

pj- 7 6 31<br />

pl- 100 34 12<br />

pr- 200 21 20<br />

sk- 116 25 17<br />

skr- 63 35 11<br />

skv- 10 9 28<br />

sl- 150 83 1<br />

sm- 55 31 14<br />

sn- 126 62 2<br />

sp- 137 47 6<br />

spj- 6 4 34<br />

spl- 9 5 32<br />

spr- 34 23 18<br />

st- 279 45 7<br />

str- 77 30 15<br />

sv- 65 23 18<br />

tr- 169 44 9<br />

tv- 20 3 35<br />

vr- 19 12 23<br />

85


Table 4.1 shows, for example, that 5 out of 13 root morphemes beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g<br />

with bj- have a sound symbolic mean<strong>in</strong>g. The rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 8 (13-5)<br />

unmotivated root morphemes beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with bj- are:<br />

bjuda (<strong>in</strong>vite, offer, etc)<br />

bjugg (barley)<br />

bjussa (slang for bjuda)<br />

bjälke (balk)<br />

björk (birch)<br />

björkna (a fish)<br />

björn (bear)<br />

björna (demand repayment)<br />

and the 5 motivated root morphemes are:<br />

Key word<br />

bjäbba uppnosigt prat (cheeky talk)<br />

bjäfs överdriven (exaggerated)<br />

bjällra kl<strong>in</strong>gande (chim<strong>in</strong>g bell)<br />

bjärt lysande (sh<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g)<br />

bjässe mycket stor (very big)<br />

(Only key words, not the complete def<strong>in</strong>itions, are given above.)<br />

4.2.2 Proportions of motivated root morphemes.<br />

Summary<br />

To sum up part of the forego<strong>in</strong>g comparisons, diagram 4.4 shows the<br />

proportions between number of sound symbolic morphemes and total<br />

number of morphemes <strong>in</strong> percentages.<br />

86


fn<br />

gn<br />

skv<br />

pj<br />

kn<br />

spr<br />

spj<br />

gl<br />

mj<br />

vr<br />

spl<br />

sm<br />

skr<br />

sl<br />

bl<br />

sn<br />

kl<br />

str<br />

fl<br />

bj<br />

sv<br />

sp<br />

pl<br />

nj<br />

kr<br />

kv<br />

fj<br />

tr<br />

sk<br />

dr<br />

gr<br />

st<br />

tv<br />

pr<br />

fr<br />

br<br />

0 20 40 60 80 100<br />

Diagram 4.4. Percentage of motivated root morphemes per cluster.<br />

87<br />

%


Diagram 4.4. shows the proportion of motivated root morphemes <strong>in</strong><br />

percentage of the total number of root morphemes per <strong>in</strong>dividual cluster.<br />

The root morphemes are the same as <strong>in</strong> diagrams 4.1–4.3. We can see that<br />

the clusters fn-, gn-, skv-, pj-, kn-, spr-, spj-, gl- mj- and vr- have a<br />

greater ratio of motivated root morphemes, well over 60%. The cluster<br />

fn- is the most sound symbolic, at 100 %. These clusters are all lexically<br />

<strong>in</strong>frequent clusters with the exception for kn- and gl- which are<br />

comparatively larger, (cf. diagrams 4.2 and 4.3). It is clear that lexically<br />

<strong>in</strong>frequent clusters are exploited sound symbolically to a higher degree<br />

than lexically frequent clusters are (cf. the discussion <strong>in</strong> 8.3). The clusters<br />

br-, pr-, and fr- have the lowest proportion of sound symbolic root<br />

morphemes. Of these clusters pr- is very frequent lexically and br- is<br />

quite frequent too (cf. diagrams 4.2, 4.3). This phenomenon is however<br />

not symmetrical; there are lexically very frequent clusters like sl- which<br />

are also sound symbolic to quite a high degree (55%). An <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g<br />

result is that the two most sound symbolic clusters, fn- and gn-, end with<br />

n while the three least sound symbolic clusters, pr-, fr- and br-, all end<br />

with an r.<br />

4.2.3 Types of mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

After calculat<strong>in</strong>g the ratios of motivated root morphemes per cluster, an<br />

analysis was made of the semantic features appear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> each cluster. The<br />

analysis can be done <strong>in</strong> detail or more abstractly. The analysis presented<br />

<strong>in</strong> this chapter is quite detailed, with more specific semantic features. For<br />

example there are the categories 'narrow form', round form', 'th<strong>in</strong><br />

form', hollow form', 'short wide form', 'crooked form', 'long th<strong>in</strong><br />

form', 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form' and 'small end form'. On a more abstract level<br />

these can of course be classified as 'form'. The same goes for e.g. 'surface<br />

structure' which is further analyzed <strong>in</strong>to 'rough surface structure',<br />

'smooth surface structure', 'hardness'. Some categories are broader than<br />

their names imply. This should be noted for 'pejorative', which <strong>in</strong>cludes<br />

someth<strong>in</strong>g more generally negative and 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' which <strong>in</strong>cludes all sounds<br />

made by humans.<br />

The categories of diagram. 4.5 are related to those presented <strong>in</strong> chapter 2<br />

<strong>in</strong> the manner listed below. They are not mutually exclusive, s<strong>in</strong>ce it was<br />

judged to be <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to count certa<strong>in</strong> frequent special cases of<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g. Therefore e.g. 'mental feel<strong>in</strong>g' is subdivided <strong>in</strong>to 'bad mood'<br />

and 'other mental feel<strong>in</strong>g'. However, <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g discussions and<br />

88


diagrams 'other mental feel<strong>in</strong>g', etc is simply called 'mental feel<strong>in</strong>g' 5. The<br />

feature 'slang' is a stylistic feature and <strong>in</strong> fact adds another dimension.<br />

<strong>Sound</strong>: <strong>Sound</strong><br />

Talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Beat<br />

Movement: Movement<br />

Quick or strong movement<br />

Walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Fall<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Potential movement<br />

Quickness<br />

Light: Light<br />

Gaze<br />

Surface structure: Rough surface structure<br />

Smooth surface structure<br />

Consistency: Soft consistency<br />

(Plasticity) Hardness<br />

Slackness<br />

Stiffness<br />

Wetness: Wetness<br />

Adhesion<br />

Dryness: Dryness<br />

Attitude: Attitude<br />

Secrecy<br />

Slang: Slang<br />

Jocular: Jocular<br />

Pejorative: Pejorative<br />

Destruction<br />

Mental feel<strong>in</strong>g: Mental feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Bad mood<br />

Bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g: Bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Suffocation<br />

Separation: Separation<br />

5 Other such cases are 'other movement' which is called 'movement', 'light emission'<br />

which is called 'light', 'other wetness' which is called 'wetness', 'other attitude' which is<br />

called 'attitude', 'other mental feel<strong>in</strong>g' which is called 'mental feel<strong>in</strong>g', 'other bodily<br />

feel<strong>in</strong>g' which is called 'bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g', 'other form' which is called 'form', and 'other<br />

iterative' which is called 'iterative'.<br />

89


Putt<strong>in</strong>g together Putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

Dim<strong>in</strong>utive: Dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

Augmentative: Augmentative<br />

Form: Form<br />

Round form<br />

Short-wide form<br />

Th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

Hollow form<br />

W<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

Long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

Narrow form<br />

Small end form<br />

Bent form<br />

Iterative: Iterative<br />

F<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong><br />

The most frequent mean<strong>in</strong>gs (shown <strong>in</strong> table 4.2) are, <strong>in</strong> descend<strong>in</strong>g<br />

order:<br />

Table 4.2 The most frequent semantic features, <strong>in</strong> descend<strong>in</strong>g order.<br />

semantic freq semantic freq semantic freq<br />

feature<br />

feature<br />

feature<br />

'pejorative' 163 'gaze' 15 'attitude' 5<br />

'sound' 107 'th<strong>in</strong> form' 14 'jocular' 5<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> form' 97 'smooth surface' 13 'fall<strong>in</strong>g' 5<br />

'quick or strong 67 'slang' 12 'soft<br />

4<br />

movement'<br />

consistency'<br />

'wetness' 63 'beat' 12 'small end<br />

form'<br />

4<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g' 55 'slackness' 11 'secrecy' 4<br />

'light' 32 'rough surface<br />

structure'<br />

10 'iterative' 3<br />

'dim<strong>in</strong>utive' 31 'separation' 9 'bent form' 3<br />

'round form' 23 'putt<strong>in</strong>g<br />

together'<br />

9 'narrow form' 3<br />

'way of walk<strong>in</strong>g' 22 'hardness' 8 'stiffness' 2<br />

90


'destruction' 21 'bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g' 8 'potential<br />

movement'<br />

2<br />

'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form' 20 'f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong>' 8 'light tactile<br />

sensation'<br />

2<br />

'short-wide<br />

form'<br />

18 'hollow form' 8 'augmentative' 1<br />

'bad mood' 18 'quickness' 6 'dryness' 1<br />

'form' 16 'adhesion' 6<br />

91


pejorative<br />

sound<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

quick or strong movem<br />

wetness<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

light<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

round form<br />

way of walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

destruction<br />

w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

bad mood<br />

shortwide form<br />

form<br />

gaze<br />

th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

smooth surface<br />

beat<br />

slang<br />

slackness<br />

rough surf. str<br />

separation<br />

putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

hardness<br />

bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong><br />

hollow form<br />

quickness<br />

adhesion<br />

attitude<br />

jocular<br />

fall<strong>in</strong>g<br />

soft cons<br />

small end form<br />

secrecy<br />

bent form<br />

narrow form<br />

stiffness<br />

pot. movem.<br />

light tactile sens.<br />

augmentative<br />

dryness<br />

0 20 40 60 80 100 120 140 160 180<br />

Diagram, 4.5 The diagram shows the extent to which different semantic<br />

features are exploited by all <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters, <strong>in</strong> absolute<br />

numbers. It shows e.g. that 'pejorative' is the most frequent semantic<br />

feature.<br />

92


Comb<strong>in</strong>ations of features<br />

The high frequency of some of the features is probably due to their<br />

ability to comb<strong>in</strong>e with other features. 'Pejorative' and 'sound' often<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>e with other features <strong>in</strong> a root morpheme, e.g. 'walk<strong>in</strong>g', 'talk<strong>in</strong>g'<br />

('way of walk<strong>in</strong>g' and 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' are very often comb<strong>in</strong>ed with<br />

'pejorative'). A feature like 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form', on the other hand, is very<br />

low frequent (only found <strong>in</strong> two clusters, kr- and sn-) and this low<br />

frequency might be partly a consequence of lower ability to comb<strong>in</strong>e with<br />

other features. Likewise 'bad mood' is conf<strong>in</strong>ed to the clusters gr-, vrand<br />

tr-. The frequencies of different semantic features for each cluster<br />

and how different features comb<strong>in</strong>e can be studied <strong>in</strong> Appendix 1.<br />

4.3 Frequent semantic features<br />

A detailed account for how semantic features exploit the five most<br />

frequent clusters (cf. diagram. 4.5) is shown below. These features are<br />

'pejorative', 'wetness', 'sound', 'long th<strong>in</strong> form' and 'quick or strong<br />

movement'.<br />

Diagram 4.6 shows the feature 'pejorative' and how it is distributed <strong>in</strong><br />

terms of percentage over 28 clusters. (The percentage is calculated on<br />

number of features per total number of root morphemes, for every<br />

cluster.)<br />

4.3.1 Pejorative<br />

Diagram 4.6 shows the percentage of morphemes with a pejorative<br />

feature. For example, 71 % of all morphemes beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with pj- and<br />

44% of all morphemes beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with fn- have a pejorative mean<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Almost all clusters have morphemes with pejorative features. As can be<br />

seen, pj- is the cluster with the highest percentage of pejorative root<br />

morphemes. fn- comes second and then two more cluster end<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> j: nj-<br />

(which is however to small to be considered <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g) and fj-.<br />

93


80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

sp<br />

sk<br />

pr<br />

str<br />

tr<br />

sv<br />

sm<br />

kl<br />

kr<br />

gr<br />

pl<br />

fl<br />

gl<br />

mj<br />

skv<br />

sn<br />

dr<br />

bl<br />

kn<br />

skr<br />

bj<br />

vr<br />

sl<br />

gn<br />

fj<br />

nj<br />

fn<br />

pj<br />

Diagram 4.6. Percent root morphemes with the feature 'pejorative' for different clusters.


It is also <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to study the absolute numbers of morphemes with<br />

pejorative features, s<strong>in</strong>ce some clusters are lexically very frequent and<br />

others are very <strong>in</strong>frequent. The morphemes with a pejorative feature for<br />

the clusters are shown <strong>in</strong> table 4.3 (the absolute number of root<br />

morphemes for each cluster is placed after every cluster). The clusters<br />

are presented <strong>in</strong> order of descend<strong>in</strong>g relative frequency (cf. diagram 4.6).<br />

Table 4.3 The morphemes with the feature pejorative, the <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

consonant clusters <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

pj-: pjatt (squirt), pjoller (babble,<br />

twaddle), pjosk (coddl<strong>in</strong>g), pjunk (coddl<strong>in</strong>g;<br />

pul<strong>in</strong>g), pjåkig (mawkish, bad) (SAOL also<br />

lists pjalt (stackare), pjunk ("gnäll, pjosk"),<br />

pjåk ("pjunk")) 5<br />

fn-: fnask (prostitute), fnatta (run about),<br />

fnoskig (dotty), fnurra (cf. grumpy) 4<br />

nj-: njugg (niggardly) 1<br />

fj-: fjant (busybody), fjollig (foolish), fjompig<br />

(foolish), fjuttig (<strong>in</strong>significant), fjäsa<br />

(show off), fjäska (fawn on) (SAOL also lists<br />

fjoskig (fnoskig) (dotty) 6<br />

gn-: gnat (nagg<strong>in</strong>g), gneta (st<strong>in</strong>t), gnidare<br />

(miser) 3<br />

sl-: slabba (splash), slabbertacka<br />

(gossipmonger), sladdra (gossip), slafs<br />

(slopp<strong>in</strong>ess), slampa (slut), slams<br />

(slovenl<strong>in</strong>ess), slarvig (slipshod), slas<br />

(slugish and slipshod person), slasa<br />

(walk lazily), slask (slush), slatt (heeltap),<br />

slattrig (gossip<strong>in</strong>g), slidder (gossip), sl<strong>in</strong>ka (wench),<br />

slisk (sweet stuff), slok (bloke),<br />

sludder (slurred speech), slum (slum),<br />

slusk (shabby-look<strong>in</strong>g fellow), slyna (bitch),<br />

slyngel (young rascal), slö (<strong>in</strong>dolent),<br />

slödder (riff-raff), slösa (squander) 24<br />

vr-: vräkig (flashy), vränga<br />

(turn <strong>in</strong>side out), vrövel (silly talk) 3<br />

bj-: bjäbba (squabble, bicker), bjäfs<br />

95


(gewgaws) 2<br />

skr-: skral (poor, bad), skrodera (brag),<br />

skrubb (cubby-hole), skrutt (rubbish),<br />

skrymt (hypocrisy), skräppa (brag),<br />

skrävla (brag), skröna (tall story) 8<br />

kn-: knackig (quite bad), knal<br />

(economically weak), knasig (crazy),<br />

kneg (job), kneken (vara på kneken:<br />

be down one's luck), knodd (bounder),<br />

knorva (make creased<br />

and wr<strong>in</strong>kled), knutte (person who is<br />

one-sidedly focused on a certa<strong>in</strong> activity),<br />

knöl (bastard), knös (rich fellow) 10<br />

bl-: bladdra (babble), blaffa (big ugly<br />

colour patch), blaj (silly talk), blarr (silly<br />

talk), blaska (splash), bliga (stare stupidly),<br />

bluddra (talk nonsense), blunder (<br />

blunder) (SAOL also lists (blaha<br />

(nonsense talk)) 8<br />

dr-: drasut (lanky fellow), dratta (fall),<br />

dravel (twaddle), dregel (dribble), drulle<br />

(clumsy fool), drummel (lout), drägg<br />

(dregs), drälla (spill), dröse (unstructured<br />

amount) 9<br />

sn-: snafs (dirt), snaskig (smutty), snatta<br />

(p<strong>in</strong>ch), snicksnack (chit-chat), sniken<br />

(greedy), snobb (dandy), snorkig (snooty),<br />

snusk (smutt<strong>in</strong>ess), snylta (sponge),<br />

snål (st<strong>in</strong>gy), snärta (young th<strong>in</strong>g),<br />

snöd (sordid), snöplig (<strong>in</strong>glorious) 13<br />

skv-: skvallra (gossip) 1<br />

mj-: mjäkig (namby-pamby) 1<br />

gl-: glop (whipper-snapper), glufsa<br />

(scoff), glunkas (det glunkas: there is a<br />

rumour), glupande (ravenous), glupsk<br />

(greedy), glåpord (taunt) 6<br />

fl-: flacka (rove), flamsig (silly, giggly),<br />

96


fl<strong>in</strong>a (sneer, cackle), flitter (?), floskel 6<br />

(empty phrase), flabba (cackle), flopp<br />

(ignom<strong>in</strong>ious failure), flummig (<strong>in</strong>tellectually<br />

unclear). SAOL also lists flepig ("mjäkig",<br />

namby-pamby) 8<br />

pl-: pladuska (irregular spot), plottra<br />

(potter about), plufsig (bloated), plump<br />

(rude), plump (blot), plundra (plunder),<br />

plussig (bloated) 7<br />

gr-: grumlig (muddy), grummel<br />

(dregs), grums (dregs), grumsa<br />

(grumble), gräll (glar<strong>in</strong>g), gräma (grieve),<br />

gräslig (terrible) 7<br />

kr-: krafs (trash), krake (wretch), kram<br />

(trash), krams (trash), kratta (funk), kreta<br />

(badly whittle), krimskrams (knick-knacks),<br />

kruserlig (ceremonious), krångel (fuss), kräk<br />

(wretch) 10<br />

tr-: tradig (bor<strong>in</strong>g), traggla (plod through),<br />

trams (rubbish, drivel), traska (trot,<br />

jog), trassel (tangle, muddle), troll<br />

(troll), truls (unordered collection) 7<br />

kl-: klanka (grouse, grumble), klantig<br />

(clumsily stupid), klotter (scribble, doodle),<br />

kludda (daub, smudge), klyscha 7 (cliché;<br />

lump of spittle), klåpare (bungler, botcher) 6<br />

sm-: smicker (blarney), smolk<br />

(particle of dirt), smuts (filth) 3<br />

sv-: svamla (drivel), svassa (strut,<br />

swagger), svulstig (bombastic) 3<br />

str-: strul (fuss), strunt (rubbish),<br />

(stuck-up blighter) 2<br />

pr-: pracka (fob), pryl (awl), prångla (utter -<br />

counterfeit co<strong>in</strong>) 3<br />

6 The word floskel comes from Lat<strong>in</strong> flosculus which means "little flower; showy<br />

decoration <strong>in</strong> speech" but it also fits <strong>in</strong>to the Swedish pattern (cf. discussion <strong>in</strong> 1.5)<br />

7 Another <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g word, which can be said to come from cliché, but which fits <strong>in</strong>to the<br />

pejorative group (and also with all the root morphemes denot<strong>in</strong>g way of talk<strong>in</strong>g, etc.)<br />

especially <strong>in</strong> connection with the dialectal mean<strong>in</strong>g of klyscha (lump of spittle).<br />

97


sk-: skavank (flaw), skorv (old tub) 2<br />

sp-: spill (waste), spol<strong>in</strong>g (whipper-snapper) 2<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g diagram (4.7) shows the absolute number of features for<br />

the clusters. 8 This diagram shows that sl- has 24 root morphemes with a<br />

pejorative feature, sn- has 13, etc. It is clear that a large number of all<br />

pejorative morphemes beg<strong>in</strong> with an s. Sl-, sn- and skr- beg<strong>in</strong> a large<br />

number of morphemes that have a pejorative component (but these<br />

clusters are not the most dom<strong>in</strong>ated by sound symbolic root morphemes,<br />

cf. diagram 4.4).<br />

Note aga<strong>in</strong> that one root morpheme can have more than one motivated<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g component. For example 'sound' and 'pejorative' are often<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ed as <strong>in</strong> bluddra, or 'sound', 'wetness' and 'movement' as <strong>in</strong><br />

skvimpa. 'Walk<strong>in</strong>g' is often comb<strong>in</strong>ed with an additional slightly<br />

pejorative mean<strong>in</strong>g component (e. g. svassa has 'pretentious manner').<br />

'Talk<strong>in</strong>g' is also often comb<strong>in</strong>ed with an additional pejorative mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

component (e.g. pladdra implies 'nonsense').<br />

8 The reason why there are 30 clusters here <strong>in</strong>stead of 28 as <strong>in</strong> diagram 4.6 is that the<br />

frequency of 1 morpheme is below 1% <strong>in</strong> a large cluster like st-.<br />

98


25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

fl<br />

spr<br />

nj<br />

mj<br />

st<br />

sp<br />

sk<br />

str<br />

bj<br />

sv<br />

sm<br />

vr<br />

pr<br />

gn<br />

fn<br />

pj<br />

gl<br />

kl<br />

fj<br />

pl<br />

gr<br />

tr<br />

bl<br />

s kr<br />

dr<br />

kr<br />

kn<br />

sn<br />

sl<br />

k<br />

Diagram 4.7 Number of root morphemes with the 'pejorative' feature.


Comments on the feature 'pejorative'<br />

As stated above, pj- is the cluster with the highest percentage of<br />

pejorative root morphemes. All two-consonant clusters conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a jphoneme<br />

are pejorative and to quite a high degree.<br />

Look<strong>in</strong>g at absolute figures, clusters beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with s – especially sl- snand<br />

skr- – are dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g among the pejorative morphemes. Sl- is by far<br />

connected with the greatest number of pejorative morphemes.<br />

4.3.2 <strong>Sound</strong><br />

The semantic feature 'sound' ranks as number 2 <strong>in</strong> sound symbolic<br />

frequency (cf. diagram 4.5). Diagram 4.8 shows that fn- is the cluster<br />

with the highest proportion of morphemes hav<strong>in</strong>g the feature 'sound'. Fnis<br />

a lexically <strong>in</strong>frequent cluster and thus the tendency from 'pejorative'<br />

and is repeated: lexically <strong>in</strong>frequent clusters tend to be highly sound<br />

symbolic. Fn- is also the most sound symbolic cluster of all (100%)<br />

count<strong>in</strong>g the different semantic features.<br />

100


35<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

st<br />

pr<br />

tr<br />

fl<br />

fr<br />

pl<br />

gl<br />

fj<br />

sk<br />

kr<br />

br<br />

skr<br />

sm<br />

sn<br />

mj<br />

bj<br />

kl<br />

kv<br />

gn<br />

kn<br />

skv<br />

fn<br />

Diagram 4.8 Percent root morphemes with the feature 'sound' for different clusters.


The morphemes with the feature 'sound' for the clusters are shown <strong>in</strong><br />

table 4.4 (the absolute number of root morphemes for each cluster is<br />

placed after every cluster). The clusters are presented after descend<strong>in</strong>g<br />

relative frequency (cf. diagram 4.8):<br />

Table 4.4 The morphemes with the feature 'sound', the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant<br />

clusters <strong>in</strong>volved and their absolute frequencies.<br />

fn-: fnissa (giggle), fnittra (giggle), fnysa<br />

(snort) 3<br />

skv-: skval (pour<strong>in</strong>g), skvalp (lapp<strong>in</strong>g) 2<br />

kn-: knacka (knock), knaka (creak), knall<br />

(bang), knapra (nibble), knarr (creak<strong>in</strong>g),<br />

knastra (crackle), knatter (rattle), knirka<br />

(creak), knirra (creak), knistra (crackle),<br />

knittra (crackle), knorr (grumbl<strong>in</strong>g), knot<br />

(murmur<strong>in</strong>g), knyst (the least sound),<br />

knäppa (click) 15<br />

gn-: gnissla (squeak), gnod<strong>in</strong>g (a fish with a<br />

grumbl<strong>in</strong>g sound), gnägga (wh<strong>in</strong>ny), gnälla<br />

(squeak) 4<br />

kv-: kvacka (quack), kvarka (strangles),<br />

kvida (whimper), kvillra (twitter), kvirra<br />

(grumble), kvitter (chirp), kväda (s<strong>in</strong>g),<br />

kväka (croak) 8<br />

kl-: klafs (squelch), klamp (tramp), klang<br />

(clang), klappa (knock), klappra (klatter),<br />

klatsch (crack), klick (lump), klifs (squelch),<br />

kl<strong>in</strong>g (r<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g), kl<strong>in</strong>k (t<strong>in</strong>kl<strong>in</strong>g), klirra (j<strong>in</strong>gle),<br />

klocka (bell), klucka (cluck), klämta (toll),<br />

kläpp (clapper) 15<br />

bj-: bjällra (bell) 1<br />

mj-: mjau (meow) 1<br />

sn-: snarka (snore), snarpa (creak), snarra<br />

(burr), snattra (quack), snusa (sniff),<br />

snyfta (sob), snyta (blow one's nose),<br />

snäppa (click), snärp (duck's sound),<br />

snörvla (snuffle) 10<br />

102


sm-: smack (smack), smaska (guzzle),<br />

smatter clatter), smätta (flick) 4<br />

skr-: skramla (rattle), skrap (scrap<strong>in</strong>g),<br />

skrälla (blare) 3<br />

br-: braka (crash), brassa (fire away),<br />

brum (grumble), brus (buzz), bräka (bleat),<br />

bröl (growl) 6<br />

kr-: krafsa (scratch), krakel (row), kras<br />

(crack), krasch (crash), kraxa (croak),<br />

krysta (bear down), kråka (crow), krämta<br />

(hawk) 8<br />

sk-: skall (bark<strong>in</strong>g), skalla (resound), skallra<br />

(rattle), skorr (burr), skott (shot), skångra<br />

(vibrate) 6<br />

fj-: fjärta (fart) 1<br />

gl-: glam (laugh<strong>in</strong>g and talk<strong>in</strong>g), glissando,<br />

gläfs (yelp) 3<br />

pl-: pladask (flop), plask (splash), pl<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(t<strong>in</strong>g-a-l<strong>in</strong>g), plums (plop) 4<br />

fr-: frasa (rustle), frusta (snort), fräsa (hiss) 3<br />

fl-: flabb (cackle), flöjt (flute) 2<br />

tr-: trumma (drum), trumpet<br />

(trumpet) 2<br />

pr-: prassel (rustle), prutt (fart) 2<br />

st-: stampa (stamp), stepp (tap-dance),<br />

stön (groan) 3<br />

The list (as well as diagrams 4.8 and 4.9) shows that kn- is frequent both<br />

percentally and absolutely.<br />

Comments on the feature 'sound'<br />

Diagram 4.9 shows how many root morphemes have the feature 'sound',<br />

<strong>in</strong> absolute figures. Four of the five largest ones beg<strong>in</strong> with k: kl-, kn-,<br />

kv- and kr-. Kl-, kn- and kv- are also percentally (cf. diagram 4.8)<br />

among the clusters more dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g for the feature 'sound'.<br />

103


16<br />

14<br />

12<br />

10<br />

8<br />

6<br />

4<br />

2<br />

0<br />

mj<br />

fj<br />

bj<br />

skv<br />

fl<br />

pr<br />

tr<br />

st<br />

gl<br />

fr<br />

fn<br />

skr<br />

sm<br />

pl<br />

gn<br />

sk<br />

br<br />

kr<br />

kv<br />

sn<br />

kn<br />

kl<br />

Diagram 4.9 Number of root morphemes with the feature 'sound' for different clusters.


4.3.3 Long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

The feature 'long th<strong>in</strong> form' ranks as number 3 <strong>in</strong> sound symbolic<br />

frequency accord<strong>in</strong>g to diagram 4.5. This is one of the many 'form'features.<br />

35<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

spj<br />

s tr<br />

spr<br />

sp<br />

sl<br />

sn<br />

Diagram 4.10 Percent of the feature 'long th<strong>in</strong> form' for different<br />

clusters.<br />

Diagram 4.10 shows that the semantic feature 'long th<strong>in</strong> form' is<br />

dom<strong>in</strong>ated by three three-consonant clusters: spj-, str-, and spr-. All<br />

these beg<strong>in</strong> with s (a fact of all Swedish <strong>in</strong>itial three consonant clusters)<br />

just as all but two of the other clusters, namely sp-, sl-, sn-, st-, sk- and<br />

sv-. The only ones that do not beg<strong>in</strong> with s are tr- and tv-. ( However, t<br />

is a dental like s.) Aga<strong>in</strong> the highest ranked cluster spj- is a lexically very<br />

<strong>in</strong>frequent cluster.<br />

The morphemes with the feature 'long th<strong>in</strong> form' for the clusters are<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> table 4.5 (the absolute number of root morphemes for each<br />

cluster are placed after every cluster). The clusters are presented after<br />

descend<strong>in</strong>g relative frequency (cf. diagram 4.10).<br />

tr<br />

105<br />

tv<br />

st<br />

sk<br />

sv


Table 4.5 The morphemes with the feature 'long th<strong>in</strong> form', the <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

consonant clusters <strong>in</strong>volved and their absolute frequencies.<br />

spj-: spjut (spear), spjäla (lath) 2<br />

str-: streamer (streamer), streck (stroke),<br />

stretch (stretch), strigel (strop), stril<br />

(nozzle), strimla (strip), strimma (streak),<br />

stripa (wisp), strut (cone), strå (straw),<br />

stråk (band), stråle (ray), stråt (way),<br />

sträcka (stretch), sträng (str<strong>in</strong>g), ström<br />

(stream), strössel (spr<strong>in</strong>kles) 17<br />

spr-: spricka (crack), spr<strong>in</strong>ga (narrow<br />

open<strong>in</strong>g), spr<strong>in</strong>t (p<strong>in</strong>), sprits (squirt),<br />

sprund (slit), spröjs (w<strong>in</strong>dow-bar),<br />

spröt (rib) 7<br />

sp-: spaljé (trellis), spalt (column),<br />

spant (rib), sparre (rafter), sparris<br />

(asparagus), spatel (spatula), spene (dug),<br />

spenslig (slender), spetig (sk<strong>in</strong>ny), spets<br />

(po<strong>in</strong>t), spett (spit), spigg (stickleback), spik<br />

(nail), spila (spike l<strong>in</strong>g), sp<strong>in</strong>del (spider),<br />

sp<strong>in</strong>kig (th<strong>in</strong>), sp<strong>in</strong>na (sp<strong>in</strong>), spira (spire),<br />

spole (bobb<strong>in</strong>), spont (tongue), spång<br />

(foot-bridge), spänta (split wood), spö (twig) 23<br />

sl-: sladd (cord), slamsa (rag), slana (scaffold<br />

pole), slang (tube), slank (slender), slant<br />

(a fish<strong>in</strong>g rod), slejf (strap), slimmad (fitted),<br />

sl<strong>in</strong>ga (coil), slips (tie), slits (slit), släde (sleigh) 12<br />

sn-: snabel (trunk), snigel (snail), snilja<br />

(a thread), snitsel (paperstrip), sno (tw<strong>in</strong>e),<br />

snodd (cord), snok (grass snake), snorkel<br />

(snorkel), snugga (cutty), snöre (str<strong>in</strong>g) 10<br />

tr-: tratt (funnel), tross (hawser), truta<br />

(pout), tryne (snout), tråd (thread), tråg<br />

(trough), trål (trawl), träns (braid) 8<br />

tv-: tv<strong>in</strong>na (tw<strong>in</strong>e) 1<br />

st-: stake (stake), stav (staff), sticka<br />

(spl<strong>in</strong>ter), stift (p<strong>in</strong>), stig (path), stilett<br />

(stiletto), stock (log), stolpe (pole), stylta<br />

106


(stilt), stång (pole), stör (stake), stötta<br />

(prop) 12<br />

sk-: skakel (shaft), skalm (shaft), skalpell<br />

(scalpel) 3<br />

sv-: svabba (swab), svans (tail) 2<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

sp<br />

s tr<br />

sl<br />

st<br />

sn<br />

tr<br />

Diagram 4.11 Number of morphemes with the feature 'long th<strong>in</strong> form'<br />

for different clusters.<br />

Comments on the feature 'long th<strong>in</strong> form'.<br />

Diagram 4.11 shows that sp- is the largest cluster, <strong>in</strong> absolute figures, for<br />

the semantic feature 'long th<strong>in</strong> form'.<br />

4.3.4 Quick or strong movement 9<br />

Diagram 4.12 shows that fl- has the highest percent of morphemes hav<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the feature 'quick or strong movement'. Fl- is a quite large cluster. In<br />

second place comes spr- which is a lexically <strong>in</strong>frequent cluster even if it<br />

is not extremely small. Vr- <strong>in</strong> third place is also lexically <strong>in</strong>frequent.<br />

The morphemes with the feature 'quick or strong movement' for the<br />

clusters are shown <strong>in</strong> table 4.6 (the absolute number of root morphemes<br />

9 The reason for collaps<strong>in</strong>g quick movement and strong movement is that <strong>in</strong> some root<br />

morphemes both these mean<strong>in</strong>gs are present (although <strong>in</strong> others only one is present).<br />

However, it seems cumbersome to make three categories <strong>in</strong>stead of one.<br />

spr<br />

107<br />

sk<br />

spj<br />

sv<br />

tv


for each cluster are placed after every cluster). The clusters are presented<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to descend<strong>in</strong>g relative frequency (cf. diagram 4.12).<br />

20<br />

18<br />

16<br />

14<br />

12<br />

10<br />

8<br />

6<br />

4<br />

2<br />

0<br />

fl<br />

spr<br />

vr<br />

sv<br />

gn<br />

fn<br />

skv<br />

Diagram 4.12 Percentage 'quick or strong movement' for different<br />

clusters.<br />

Table 4.6 The morphemes with the feature 'quick or strong movement',<br />

the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

fl-: flacka (rove), fladdra (flutter), flagga<br />

(flag) flamma (flame), flanera (be out for a<br />

stroll), flaxa (flutter), flimra (flicker), fluga<br />

(fly), fluktuation (fluctuation), flyga (fly), fly<br />

(flee), flyta (float), flytta (move), flåsa<br />

(puff) fläkta (fan), flämta (pant), flänga<br />

(be dash<strong>in</strong>g about), flöda (flow) 18<br />

spr-: sprallig (frolicsome), sprattla<br />

(flounder), spritta (jump), sprudla (bubble),<br />

spruta (spurt), sprutt (speed) 6<br />

vr-: vricka (scull), vrida (twist), vräka<br />

(heave) 3<br />

sv-: svaja (sw<strong>in</strong>g), svalla (surge), svepa<br />

(sweep), sv<strong>in</strong>g (sw<strong>in</strong>g), svirvel (swivel),<br />

svämma (overflow), sväng (sweep),<br />

svärm (swarm), sväva (float) 9<br />

sl<br />

108<br />

kn<br />

sk<br />

tr<br />

fr<br />

kr<br />

sp<br />

st


gn-: gnida, (rub) gno (rub), gnugga (rub) 3<br />

fn-: fnatta (run about)) 1<br />

skv-: skvimpa (splash to and fro) 1<br />

sl-: sladda (skid), sl<strong>in</strong>ka (slip), sl<strong>in</strong>ta (slide),<br />

slipprig (slippery), slira (skid), slita (pull),<br />

slugga (beat), slunga (fl<strong>in</strong>g), slänga (throw) 9<br />

kn-: knixa (bob), knyck (jerk) 2<br />

sk-: skaka (shake), skalv (quake), skippa<br />

(skip) 3<br />

tr-: tromb (tornado), tromla (rotat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

cyl<strong>in</strong>drical sieve), trumla (treat<br />

someth<strong>in</strong>g by enclos<strong>in</strong>g it <strong>in</strong> a rotat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

drum) 3<br />

fr-: frossa (the shivers), frusa (gush) 2<br />

kr-: kratsa (scrape), kränga (heave over) 2<br />

sp-: spasm (spasm), spurt (spurt) 2<br />

st-: studsa (bounce), stöppla (give pa<strong>in</strong>ted<br />

surface a certa<strong>in</strong> look by strik<strong>in</strong>g a brush<br />

aga<strong>in</strong>st it), stöt (thrust) 3<br />

18<br />

16<br />

14<br />

12<br />

10<br />

8<br />

6<br />

4<br />

2<br />

0<br />

fl<br />

sv<br />

sl<br />

spr<br />

vr<br />

tr<br />

st<br />

gn<br />

Diagram 4.13 Number of morphemes with the feature 'quick and strong<br />

movement' for different clusters.<br />

109<br />

sk<br />

fr<br />

kn<br />

kr<br />

sp<br />

skv<br />

fn


40<br />

35<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

kr<br />

str<br />

dr<br />

sn<br />

pl<br />

sp<br />

sv<br />

bl<br />

sl<br />

kl<br />

sm<br />

spr<br />

skv<br />

Diagram 4.14 Percent root morphemes with the feature 'wetness' for different clusters.


Comments on the feature 'quick or strong<br />

movement'.<br />

Diagram 4.13 shows that fl- has by far the greatest number of<br />

morphemes with the feature 'quick and strong movement'. Fl- is then<br />

most frequent both absolutely and percentally.<br />

4.3.5 Wetness<br />

The last semantic feature to be exam<strong>in</strong>ed is 'wetness', which ranks as<br />

number 5 <strong>in</strong> sound symbolic frequency accord<strong>in</strong>g to diagram 4.5.<br />

Diagram 4.14 shows that skv- is the cluster with the highest proportion of<br />

morphemes hav<strong>in</strong>g the feature wetness. (Almost half of the morphemes<br />

beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with skv- have the feature 'wetness', cf. table 4.16) The next to<br />

largest cluster, sm-, has only 13% morphemes with this feature. The<br />

cluster skv- is small and conta<strong>in</strong>s a total of ten root morphemes. The<br />

morphemes with the 'wetness' feature for the clusters are the follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(the absolute number of root morphemes for each cluster are placed after<br />

every cluster). The clusters are presented accord<strong>in</strong>g to descend<strong>in</strong>g relative<br />

frequency (cf. diagram 4.14).<br />

Table 4.7 The morphemes with the feature 'wetness', the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant<br />

clusters <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

skv-: skval (gush), skvalta (ripple), skvimpa<br />

(splash), skvätt (drop) 4<br />

spr-: sprej (spray), spr<strong>in</strong>kler (spr<strong>in</strong>kler),<br />

sprudla (bubbla), spruta (spurt) 4<br />

sm-: smegma (secretion), smet (sludge),<br />

smetana (a sour thick cream), sm<strong>in</strong>k<br />

(make-up), smälta (melt), smörja (grease) 6<br />

kl-: klabb (stick), kladd (daub), klafs<br />

(squelch), klena (daub), kleta (daub), klibba<br />

(stick), klick (daub), klifs (squelch), klucka<br />

(gurgle), klunk (gulp) 10<br />

sl-: slabba (splash), slafsa (squelch),<br />

slam (ooze), slask (slush), slatt (drop), slem<br />

(slime), slicka (lick), slipprig (slippery), slisk<br />

(sweet stuff), slur<strong>in</strong>g (soup), slurk (swig),<br />

111


sluss (sluice) 12<br />

bl-: blaska (splash), blod (blood), bläck (<strong>in</strong>k),<br />

blöt (wet) 4<br />

sv-: svabba (swab), svett (sweat), svämma<br />

(overflow) 3<br />

sp-: spackel (putty), spad (liquid), sperma<br />

(sperm), spilla (spill), spola (flush), spott<br />

(spittle), sputum (phlegm) 7<br />

pl-: plask (splash), plums (plop), plurr<br />

(water) 3<br />

sn-: snaskig (smutty), snigel (snail), snor<br />

(snot), snuva (cold) 4<br />

dr-: dregel (dribble), droppa (drip) 2<br />

str-: strila (spr<strong>in</strong>kle), ström (stream) 2<br />

kr-: kram (cloggy), kräm (cream) 2<br />

Comments on the feature 'wetness'<br />

Diagram. 4.15. shows <strong>in</strong> absolute numbers how many morphemes have<br />

the feature 'wetness' for each cluster. The largest cluster is sl- (twelve<br />

<strong>in</strong>stances) followed by kl- (ten <strong>in</strong>stances). 8 of the 13 'wetness' clusters<br />

beg<strong>in</strong> with s. Three beg<strong>in</strong> with an unvoiced stop and two beg<strong>in</strong> with a<br />

voiced stop.<br />

Summary of and discussion of the five most<br />

frequent features<br />

As stated above, skv- is the cluster with the highest percentage of<br />

'wetness' root morphemes. This cluster only conta<strong>in</strong>s ten morphemes so –<br />

as well as for the pejorative root morphemes – small clusters are<br />

proportionally more utilized for sound symbolism. The clusters sl-, kl-,<br />

sp-, sm-, skv-, spr-, sn-, bl-, pl-, and sv- are most utilized for the<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g 'wetness', i.e. s or <strong>in</strong>itial unvoiced plosives are preferred. Sland<br />

kl- (both end<strong>in</strong>g with l) have the highest number of root morphemes<br />

with the feature 'wetness'.<br />

112


12<br />

10<br />

8<br />

6<br />

4<br />

2<br />

0<br />

sl kl sp sm skv sn bl spr sv pl kr dr str<br />

Diagram 4.15 Number of root morphemes with the feature 'wetness' for different clusters.


The cluster sl- is the most frequent sound symbolic cluster for both<br />

'pejorative' and 'wetness'.<br />

Look<strong>in</strong>g at absolute figures, clusters beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with s, especially sl- sn-,<br />

and skr-, are dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g among the 'pejorative' morphemes. Sl- has by<br />

far the greatest number of pejorative morphemes.<br />

Similar results for cluster types were obta<strong>in</strong>ed for other clusters : 'light'<br />

is dom<strong>in</strong>ated by voiced plosives + l or n: bl-, gl-, gn- (dl-, bn-, dn- are<br />

not allowed <strong>in</strong> Swedish) 10, 'bad (negative) mood' is constra<strong>in</strong>ed to some<br />

plosives + r: gr-, tr-, vr- 11 and 'wetness' is dom<strong>in</strong>ated by: sl-, kl-, sp-,<br />

sm-, skv-, spr-, sn-, bl-, pl-, sv-, i.e. s and <strong>in</strong>itial unvoiced plosives are<br />

preferred (the only exception is bl-). The phoneme l is also quite<br />

common.<br />

10 There are root morphemes beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with other clusters that have been classified as<br />

‘light’ but these ma<strong>in</strong>ly concern quality of colour: bjärt (gaudy), gräll (glar<strong>in</strong>g),<br />

prunkande (dazzl<strong>in</strong>g), prålig (garish).<br />

11 There is only one exception: knarrig (creaky).<br />

114


4.3.6 The most sound symbolic clusters<br />

Table 4.8 shows which the most frequent clusters are, for every mean<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Table 4.8. The most sound symbolic clusters <strong>in</strong> absolute numbers and<br />

proportionally, for the five most frequent mean<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

freq % examples <strong>in</strong> % % freq examples<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

absolutenumbers<br />

'pejorative' sl- 24 16 sladder<br />

(gossip), slok<br />

(bloke),<br />

slödder (riffraff)<br />

'sound' kl-,<br />

kn-<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong><br />

form'<br />

'quick or<br />

strong<br />

movement'<br />

15 13,<br />

19<br />

klang (clang),<br />

klirra (j<strong>in</strong>gle)<br />

knarra<br />

(creak),<br />

knittra<br />

(crackle)<br />

sp- 23 17 spant (rib),<br />

spett (spit)<br />

spö (twig)<br />

fl- 18 19 fladdra<br />

(flutter),<br />

flamma<br />

(flame),<br />

flimra<br />

(flicker).<br />

fläkta (fan)<br />

'wetness' sl- 12 8 slask (slush)<br />

slem (slime)<br />

slipprig<br />

(slippery)<br />

115<br />

pj- 71 5 pjoller<br />

(babble),<br />

pjosk<br />

(coddl<strong>in</strong>g),<br />

pjunk<br />

fn- 33 3<br />

(coddl<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

fnissa<br />

(giggle),<br />

fnysa (snort)<br />

spj- 33 2 spjut (spear),<br />

spjäla (lath)<br />

fl- 19 18 fladdra<br />

(flutter),<br />

flamma<br />

(flame),<br />

flimra<br />

(flicker)<br />

fläkta (fan)<br />

skv- 40 4 skval (gush),<br />

skvalta<br />

(ripple),<br />

skvimpa<br />

(splash)


The <strong>in</strong>formation given <strong>in</strong> table 4.8, for the five most frequent mean<strong>in</strong>gs,<br />

will now be expla<strong>in</strong>ed. For the feature 'pejorative' (which is the most<br />

frequent mean<strong>in</strong>g, see diagram 4.5) the most high frequent cluster (<strong>in</strong> the<br />

sense of the greatest number of roots) is sl-, and it has a frequency of 24<br />

root morphemes. Sl- can therefore be said to be a very pejorative cluster.<br />

However, many words beg<strong>in</strong> with sl- and only 16% of these are <strong>in</strong> fact<br />

pejorative. It is also <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to look at which cluster is dom<strong>in</strong>ated to<br />

the highest degree by the feature 'pejorative'. This turns out to be pjwhere<br />

5 root morphemes (71% of the root morphemes beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with<br />

pj-) are pejorative. So, <strong>in</strong> the sense of be<strong>in</strong>g dom<strong>in</strong>ated by a certa<strong>in</strong><br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g, pj- can be said to be the most pejorative cluster. The same<br />

comparisons are made for the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 4 semantic features.<br />

The clusters that are most frequent, <strong>in</strong> absolute numbers, show a strong<br />

tendency to end with l. The clusters that are most frequent proportionally<br />

show a weak tendency to end with j. In both groups the clusters beg<strong>in</strong><br />

with a voiceless obstruent.<br />

4.4 Frequent clusters<br />

So far we have studied how clusters are distributed over different<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs. We will now look at what mean<strong>in</strong>gs different clusters hold.<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g section shows how different mean<strong>in</strong>gs are distributed over<br />

different clusters. The clusters exam<strong>in</strong>ed are the most frequent <strong>in</strong> absolute<br />

numbers: sl-, sn-, kn-, kr- (cf. diagram 4.3) and some of the most<br />

frequent percentally: fn-, kn-, gn-, spr-, pj- (cf. diagram 4.4).<br />

Also those clusters that are almost unique for a certa<strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g will be<br />

discussed, e.g. 'pejorative' has the unique cluster fj-, i.e., fj- is almost<br />

only 'pejorative'. Fl- is to a high degree 'quick or strong movement', fn-<br />

'pejorative', gl- 'light', bl- 'light' or 'gaze', kr- 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form', kn-<br />

'round form', skr- 'pejorative' and 'destruction', sl- 'pejorative', str-<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> form', br- 'sound' and 'destruction', gr- 'bad mood' or 'hollow<br />

form', (vr- 'bad mood', tr- 'bad mood'), kl- is 'wetness', 'short-wide<br />

form' or 'adhesion', kv- is 'suffocation', mj- 'softness', sk- 'hardness',<br />

skv- 'wetness', sl- 'slackness' and sp-, st- and str- are 'long th<strong>in</strong> form'<br />

(cf. table 4.16).<br />

116


25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

% m.comp/all<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

pot. movem.<br />

smooth surf.<br />

beat<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

strong movem<br />

quick or<br />

slackness<br />

long th<strong>in</strong><br />

wetness<br />

pejorative<br />

Diagram 4.16 Semantic features of the cluster sl-. Percentages of semantic features for all root morphemes.


4.4.1 The cluster sl-<br />

The first cluster to be studied is sl-, the largest cluster <strong>in</strong> absolute<br />

numbers. Diagram 4.16 shows the semantic features of the consonant<br />

cluster sl-. The words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g<br />

features are shown <strong>in</strong> table 4.8. The absolute frequencies (not the<br />

percentages) of the semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

Table 4.8 The sound symbolic morphemes of the sl- cluster, the semantic<br />

features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'pejorative': slabba (splash),<br />

slabbertacka (gossipmonger),<br />

sladdra (chatter), slafs (slopp<strong>in</strong>ess)<br />

slampa (slut), slams (slovenl<strong>in</strong>ess),<br />

slarvig (slipshod), slas (lazy and<br />

careless person), slasa (walk<br />

heavily and shuffl<strong>in</strong>g), slask (slush),<br />

slatt (drop), slattrig (slack), slidder<br />

(gossip), sl<strong>in</strong>ka (wench), slisk<br />

(sweet stuff), sloka (droop), sludder<br />

(slurred speech), slum (slum), slusk<br />

(shabby look<strong>in</strong>g fellow), slyna<br />

(bitch), slyngel (young rascal), slö<br />

(sluggish), slödder (riff-raff),<br />

slösa (waste) 24<br />

'wetness': slabba (splash), slafs<br />

(slopp<strong>in</strong>ess), slam (ooze), slask<br />

(slush), slatt (drop), slem (slime),<br />

slicka (lick), slipprig (slippery), slisk<br />

(sweet stuff), slur<strong>in</strong>g (a soup), slurk<br />

(swig), sluss (sluice)<br />

(cf. discussion <strong>in</strong> 1.5) 12<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> form': sladd (cord),<br />

slamsa (rag), slana (scaffold pole),<br />

slang (tube), slank (slender), slant<br />

(a fish<strong>in</strong>g rod), slejf (strap), slimmad<br />

(fitted), sl<strong>in</strong>ga (coil), slips (tie), slits (slit),<br />

släde (sleigh). 12<br />

118


'slackness': slack (slack), sladdrig<br />

(flabby), slak (slack), slamsa (rag),<br />

slana (scaffold pole), slang (tube),<br />

slankig (flaccid), slapp (slack), sliddrig<br />

(flabby), sl<strong>in</strong>kig (slabby), sloka (droop) 11<br />

'quick or strong movement': sladda<br />

(skid), sl<strong>in</strong>ka (slip), sl<strong>in</strong>ta (slip),<br />

slipprig (slippery), slira (skid), slita<br />

(tear), slugga (slug), slunga (fl<strong>in</strong>g), slänga<br />

(fl<strong>in</strong>g) 9<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g': slabbertacka (gossipmonger),<br />

sladdra (chatter), slattrig (gossip<strong>in</strong>g),<br />

slidder (gossip), sludder (slurred<br />

speech) 5<br />

'beat': slag (beat), slå (beat), slägga<br />

(sledge hammer) 3<br />

'smooth surface': slipa (polish), slät<br />

(smooth), slätt (pla<strong>in</strong>) 3<br />

'potential movement': slutta (slant),<br />

slänt (slope) 2<br />

'walk<strong>in</strong>g': slasa (walk heavily and<br />

shuffl<strong>in</strong>g), släntra (saunter) 2<br />

The semantic feature 'slackness' is unique for this cluster.<br />

The relations between some of the different mean<strong>in</strong>gs of sl- are discussed<br />

below <strong>in</strong> 4.6.<br />

Sl- has a different mean<strong>in</strong>g profile than sn- (cf. diagram 4.17). However,<br />

they are both dom<strong>in</strong>ated by the feature 'pejorative'.<br />

4.4.2 The cluster sn-<br />

The second most sound symbolic cluster, <strong>in</strong> absolute numbers, is sn-.<br />

Diagram 4.17 shows the semantic features of this consonant cluster.<br />

119


10<br />

9<br />

8<br />

7<br />

6<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

pejorative<br />

sound<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

quickness<br />

wetness<br />

Diagram 4.17 Semantic features of the cluster sn-. Percentages of<br />

semantic features for all root morphemes.<br />

In table 4.9 the words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g<br />

features are shown. The absolute frequencies (not the percentages) of the<br />

semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

Table 4.9 The sound symbolic morphemes of the sn- cluster, the semantic<br />

features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'pejorative': snafs (dirt), snaskig (smutty),<br />

snatta (p<strong>in</strong>ch), snicksnack (chit-chat),<br />

sniken (greedy), snobb (dandy), snorkig<br />

(snooty), snusk (smutt<strong>in</strong>ess), snylta<br />

(sponge), snål (greedy), snärta<br />

(young th<strong>in</strong>g), snöd (sordid), snöplig<br />

(<strong>in</strong>glorious) 13<br />

'sound': snarka (snore), snarpa (creak),<br />

snarra (burr), snattra (quack), snusa<br />

120<br />

small end form<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

way of walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

slang


(sniff), snyfta (sob), snyta (blow nose),<br />

snäppa (click) , snärp (duck´s sound),<br />

snörvla (snuffle) 10<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> form': snabel (trunk), snigel<br />

(snail), snilja (a thread) , snitsel (paper<br />

strip), sno (tw<strong>in</strong>e), snodd (cord), snok (grass<br />

snake), snorkel (snorkel), snultra (a long<br />

th<strong>in</strong> fish), snöre (str<strong>in</strong>g) 10<br />

'quickness': snabb (quick), snappa (snatch),<br />

snar (who reacts without delay), sno<br />

(tw<strong>in</strong>e), snudig (quick), snärj (hurry) 6<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g': snacka (chat), snicksnack<br />

(chit-chat), snubba (rebuke), snäsa (snub) 4<br />

'wetness': snaskig (smutty), snigel (snail),<br />

snor (snot), snuva (head cold) 4<br />

'small end form': snagga (clip short), snibb<br />

(tip), snip (lip), snopp (tip) 4<br />

'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form': snirkel (scroll), snurra<br />

(twist), snäcka (shell) 3<br />

'dim<strong>in</strong>utive': snitt (cut), snugga (cutty),<br />

snutt (small piece) 3<br />

'slang': snofsa upp (make elegant), snubbe<br />

(person), snut (cop) 3<br />

'walk<strong>in</strong>g': snava (trip), snubbla (trip) 2<br />

4.4.3 The cluster kn-<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g diagram (4.18) shows the semantic features of the cluster<br />

kn-, the third largest cluster <strong>in</strong> absolute numbers.<br />

The words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the semantic features of knare<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> table 4.10. The absolute frequency (not the percentages) of<br />

the semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

121


20<br />

18<br />

16<br />

14<br />

12<br />

10<br />

8<br />

6<br />

4<br />

2<br />

0<br />

round form<br />

sound<br />

pejorative<br />

together<br />

Diagram 4.18 Semantic features of the cluster kn-. Percentages of<br />

semantic features for all root morphemes.<br />

Table 4.10 The sound symbolic morphemes of the kn- cluster, the<br />

semantic features <strong>in</strong>volved and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'sound': knacka (knock), knaka (creak)<br />

, knall (bang), knapra (nibble), knarr<br />

(creak<strong>in</strong>g) , knastra (crackle), knatter (rattle)<br />

, knirka (creak), knirra (creak), knistra<br />

(crackle), knittra (crackle), knorr<br />

(grumbl<strong>in</strong>g), knot (murmur<strong>in</strong>g), knyst<br />

(the least sound), knäppa (click) 15<br />

'round form': knagg (wooden stick), knalle<br />

(hillock), knapp (button), knast (twig <strong>in</strong><br />

122<br />

way of walk<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

beat<br />

quick or<br />

strong movem


wood), knick (sharp curve on pipe), knoge<br />

(fist), knollrig (frizzy), knop (knot), knopp<br />

(bud), knorr (curl), knota (bone), knottra<br />

(goose-pimples), knubbig (chubby), knödel<br />

(cooked bun of patatoes), knöl (bump) 15<br />

'pejorative': knackig (quite bad), knal<br />

(economically weak), knasig (crazy), kneg<br />

(job), kneken (vara på kneken: be down<br />

one's luck), knodd (bounder), knorva (make<br />

creased and wr<strong>in</strong>kled), knutte (person who<br />

is onesidedly focused on a certa<strong>in</strong> activity),<br />

knöl (bastard), knös (rich fellow) 10<br />

'putt<strong>in</strong>g together': knipa (p<strong>in</strong>ch), knippa<br />

(bunch), knipsa (staple), knut (knot),<br />

knyckla (crumple up), knyppla (make lace),<br />

knyta (tie) 7<br />

'walk<strong>in</strong>g': knaggla (move forward slowly<br />

and with difficulty), knalla (trot), knata<br />

(run), knoga (walk laboriously) 4<br />

'dim<strong>in</strong>utive': knatte (little fellow), knott<br />

(gnat), knåp (f<strong>in</strong>icky job) 3<br />

'beat': knocka (knock out), knuff (push),<br />

knäck (crack) 3<br />

'quick or strong movement': knixa (bob),<br />

knyck (jerk) 2<br />

Kn- conta<strong>in</strong>s many words with the mean<strong>in</strong>g 'round form', but the cluster<br />

is not unique for this mean<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Kn- also belongs to the clusters <strong>in</strong> the next section, s<strong>in</strong>ce it is also one of<br />

the 4 most sound symbolic cluster percentally.<br />

123


10<br />

9<br />

8<br />

7<br />

6<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

pejorative<br />

sound<br />

way of walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

124<br />

th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

rough surf<br />

destruction<br />

quick or<br />

strong movem<br />

Diagram 4.19 Semantic features of the cluster kr-. Percentages of<br />

semantic features for all root morphemes.<br />

4.4.4 The cluster kr-<br />

The last of the four largest clusters (<strong>in</strong> absolute numbers) is kr-, shown <strong>in</strong><br />

diagram 4.19. In table 4.11 the words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of<br />

the semantic features of kr- are shown. The absolute frequency (not the<br />

percentages) of the semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

Table 4.11 The sound symbolic morphemes of the kr- cluster, the<br />

semantic features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form': krans (wreath),<br />

kr<strong>in</strong>gelikrokar (lots of bends <strong>in</strong> different<br />

wetness


directions), kr<strong>in</strong>gla (pretzel), krok (hook),<br />

krokan (croquembouche), krulla (curl), krum<br />

(crooked), krumbukt (w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs), krumelur<br />

(flourish), krumpen (crooked and bent from<br />

age or disease), krusa (curl), krusiduller<br />

(ornaments), krycka (crutch), kråma (prance<br />

about), kräkla (crosier), kräla (crawl),<br />

krök (bend) 17<br />

'pejorative': krafs (trash), krake (wretch),<br />

kram (trash), krams (trash), kratta (funk),<br />

kreta (badly whittle), krimskrams (knick-knacks),<br />

kruserlig (ceremonious), krångel (fuss), kräk<br />

(wretch) 10<br />

'sound': krafsa (scratch), krakel (row), kras<br />

(crack), krasch (crash), kraxa (croak), krysta<br />

(bear down), kråka (crow), krämta (hawk) 8<br />

'th<strong>in</strong> form': krakmandel (soft-shell<br />

almond), krokett (croquette), krusta (crust),<br />

krustad (croustade) 4<br />

'walk<strong>in</strong>g': kravla (crawl), krylla (crawl with),<br />

krypa (crawl), kräla (crawl) 4<br />

'rough surface structure': kratta (rake),<br />

krås (ruffle), kräpp (crepe) 3<br />

'destruction': kracka (decompose molecules<br />

through heat<strong>in</strong>g), krackelera (crackled), krossa<br />

(crush) 3<br />

'wetness': kram (cloggy), kräm (cream) 2<br />

'quick or strong movement': kratsa<br />

(scrape), kränga (heave over) 2<br />

Kr- is dom<strong>in</strong>ated by the unusual semantic feature 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form' and the<br />

cluster and mean<strong>in</strong>g are almost uniquely connected. (However 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g<br />

form' also occurs <strong>in</strong> some root morpheme with sn-.)<br />

The clusters described above are the four largest ones. Taken together<br />

they are dom<strong>in</strong>ated by the semantic features 'pejorative' and 'sound'.<br />

125


The largest clusters, percentally<br />

The next group of clusters that are to be studied more closely are the ones<br />

that are the percentally most sound symbolic, cf. diagram 4.4.<br />

4.4.5 The cluster fn-<br />

Fn- is the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant cluster with the largest percentage of sound<br />

symbolic root morphemes. The percentages of the semantic features are<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> diagram 4.20.<br />

45<br />

40<br />

35<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

pejorative<br />

sound<br />

quick or strong<br />

movem<br />

Diagram 4.20 Semantic features of the cluster fn-. Percentages of<br />

semantic features for all root morphemes.<br />

The words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the semantic features of fnare<br />

presented <strong>in</strong> table 4.12. The absolute number of words (not the<br />

percentages) of the semantic features are shown to the right. For all<br />

lexically <strong>in</strong>frequent clusters, i.e. clusters conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g less than 13 root<br />

126<br />

th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

dryness


morphemes, also root morphemes with the frequency 1 are counted (cf.<br />

chapter 3).<br />

Table 4.12 The sound symbolic morphemes of the fn- cluster, the<br />

semantic features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'pejorative': fnask (prostitute), fnatta (run<br />

about), fnoskig (dotty), fnurra (cf. grumpy) 4<br />

'sound': fnissa (giggle), fnittra (giggle),<br />

fnysa (snort) 3<br />

'quick or strong movement': fnatta (run about) 1<br />

'th<strong>in</strong> form': fnasig (chapped) 1<br />

'dryness': fnöske (t<strong>in</strong>der) 1<br />

The cluster fn- is dom<strong>in</strong>ated by 'pejorative'. All fn- root morphemes are<br />

sound symbolic. (However, as shown <strong>in</strong> diagrams 4.6 and 4.7, the feature<br />

'pejorative' is spread over many clusters.)<br />

4.4.6 The cluster kn-<br />

Kn- is the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant cluster with the fourth largest percentage of<br />

sound symbolic root morphemes. It is also the third largest cluster <strong>in</strong><br />

absolute numbers. The diagrams and the list of root morphemes can be<br />

studied above <strong>in</strong> 4.4.3.<br />

4.4.7 The cluster gn-<br />

Gn- is the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant cluster with the second largest percentage of<br />

sound symbolic root morphemes. The percentages of the semantic features<br />

are shown <strong>in</strong> diagram 4.21.<br />

The words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the semantic features of gnare<br />

presented <strong>in</strong> table 4.13. The absolute number of words (not the<br />

percentages) of the semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

127


25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sound<br />

pejorative<br />

quick or<br />

strong movem<br />

Diagram 4.21 Semantic features of the cluster gn-. Percentages of<br />

semantic features for all root morphemes.<br />

Table 4.13 The sound symbolic morphemes of the gn- cluster, the<br />

semantic features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g': gnabb (bicker<strong>in</strong>g), gnat (nagg<strong>in</strong>g),<br />

gnola (hum), gny (d<strong>in</strong>), gnöla (klaga) 5<br />

'sound': gnissla (squeak), gnod<strong>in</strong>g (a fish<br />

with a grumbl<strong>in</strong>g sound), gnägga (wh<strong>in</strong>ny),<br />

gnälla (squeak) 4<br />

'pejorative': gnat (nagg<strong>in</strong>g), gneta (st<strong>in</strong>t),<br />

gnidare (miser) 3<br />

'smooth surface structure': gnida (rub),<br />

gno (rub), gnugga (rub) 3<br />

'quick or strong movement': gnida,(rub),<br />

gno (rub), gnugga (rub) 3<br />

'light': gnista (spark), gnistra (sparkle) 2<br />

128<br />

smooth surf<br />

light


The semantic features of gn- are primarily 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' and 'sound', which<br />

semantically are very close. An analysis of relations between some of the<br />

semantic features of gn- are presented <strong>in</strong> 4.6.<br />

4.4.8 The cluster spr-<br />

The <strong>in</strong>itial consonant cluster with the fifth largest percentage of sound<br />

symbolic root morphemes is spr-. The percentages of the semantic<br />

features are shown <strong>in</strong> diagram 4.22:<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

Diagram 4.22 Semantic features of the cluster spr-. Percentages of<br />

semantic features for all root morphemes.<br />

129<br />

separation<br />

wetness


Table 4.14 shows the words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the<br />

semantic features of spr-. The absolute number of words (not the<br />

percentages) of the semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

Table 4.14 The sound symbolic morphemes of the spr- cluster, the<br />

semantic features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> form': spricka (crack), spr<strong>in</strong>ga<br />

(narrow open<strong>in</strong>g), spr<strong>in</strong>t (p<strong>in</strong>), sprits (squirt),<br />

sprund (slit), spröjs (w<strong>in</strong>dow-bar), spröt (rib) 7<br />

'quick or strong movement': sprallig<br />

(frolicsome), sprattla (flounder), spritta<br />

(jump), sprudla (bubble), spruta (spurt),<br />

sprutt (speed) 6<br />

'separation': sprej (spray), spreta (sprawl),<br />

sprida (spread), spr<strong>in</strong>kler (spr<strong>in</strong>kler),<br />

spränga (burst), sprätta (flick) 6<br />

'wetness': sprej (spray), spr<strong>in</strong>kler (spr<strong>in</strong>kler),<br />

sprudla (bubbla), spruta (spurt) 4<br />

Spr- is quite unique for the mean<strong>in</strong>g 'separation'. (The mean<strong>in</strong>g does,<br />

however, occur <strong>in</strong> sp- and spl-).<br />

4.4.9 The cluster pj-<br />

Pj- is the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant cluster with the third largest percentage of<br />

sound symbolic root morphemes. The percentages of the semantic<br />

features are shown <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g diagram (4.23):<br />

The words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the semantic features of pjare<br />

presented <strong>in</strong> table 4.15. The absolute number of words (not the<br />

percentages) of the semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

130


80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

pejorative way of<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Diagram 4.23 Semantic features of the cluster pj-. Percentages of<br />

semantic features for both root morphemes.<br />

Table 4.15 The sound symbolic morphemes of the pj- cluster, the<br />

semantic features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'pejorative': pjatt (squirt), pjoller (babble,<br />

twaddle), pjosk (coddl<strong>in</strong>g), pjunk (coddl<strong>in</strong>g;<br />

pul<strong>in</strong>g), pjåkig (mawkish, bad) 5<br />

'sound': pjoller (babble, twaddle) 1<br />

The cluster is clearly dom<strong>in</strong>ated by the feature 'pejorative'.<br />

Summary for percentages<br />

Also for the four percentally most sound symbolic clusters the semantic<br />

features 'pejorative' and 'sound' are dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g, together with 'talk<strong>in</strong>g'.<br />

Only the cluster spr- is deviat<strong>in</strong>g from this tendency. The cluster kn- is<br />

among the most sound symbolic clusters, both <strong>in</strong> absolute numbers and<br />

percentally.<br />

4.5 Typical or unique mean<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

In Table 4.16 all the clusters with typical or unique mean<strong>in</strong>gs are<br />

presented. Clusters that are dom<strong>in</strong>ated by a certa<strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g (e.g. 'quick<br />

or strong movement' <strong>in</strong> fl-), or have a mean<strong>in</strong>g which is almost unique<br />

for that cluster (e.g. 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form': kr-), have those mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> italics.<br />

For small clusters, i.e. those with less than 13 root morphemes, also<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs that have the frequency 1 are counted.<br />

131


Table 4.16 Typical<br />

or unique mean<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

of <strong>in</strong>itial clusters.<br />

bl- 33<br />

pejorative 8<br />

light 9<br />

gaze 5<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 4<br />

wetness 4<br />

round form 3<br />

br- 11<br />

sound 6<br />

destruction 5<br />

fj- 7<br />

pejorative 6<br />

sound 1<br />

fl- 36<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

18<br />

pejorative 8<br />

th<strong>in</strong> form 6<br />

sound 2<br />

light 2<br />

fn- 10<br />

pejorative 4<br />

sound 3<br />

th<strong>in</strong> form 1<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

1<br />

dryness 1<br />

gl- 45<br />

light 14<br />

smooth surface<br />

structure<br />

7<br />

pejorative 6<br />

gaze 6<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive 5<br />

form 4<br />

sound 3<br />

gn- 20<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 5<br />

sound 4<br />

pejorative 3<br />

smooth surface<br />

structure<br />

3<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

3<br />

light 2<br />

gr- 21<br />

hollow form 8<br />

bad mood 6<br />

pejorative 7<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 5<br />

kl- 51<br />

sound 15<br />

short wide form 11<br />

wetness 10<br />

adhesion 6<br />

pejorative 6<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 3<br />

kn- 59<br />

round form 15<br />

sound 15<br />

pejorative 10<br />

putt<strong>in</strong>g together 7<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g 4<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive 3<br />

beat 3<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

2<br />

kr- 53<br />

w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form 17<br />

132<br />

pejorative 10<br />

sound 8<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g 4<br />

th<strong>in</strong> form 4<br />

rough surface<br />

structure<br />

3<br />

destruction 3<br />

wetness 2<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

2<br />

kv- 17<br />

sound 8<br />

suffocation 5<br />

destruction 2<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive 2<br />

mj- 7<br />

f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong> 3<br />

soft consistency 2<br />

sound 1<br />

pejorative 1<br />

pj- 6<br />

pejorative 5<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 1<br />

sk- 25<br />

hardness 8<br />

sound 6<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 4<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

3<br />

pejorative 2<br />

round form 2<br />

skr- 35<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 12<br />

pejorative 8<br />

destruction 7<br />

rough surface<br />

structure<br />

5


sound 3<br />

skv- 9<br />

wetness 4<br />

sound 2<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 1<br />

pejorative 1<br />

movement 1<br />

sl- 83<br />

pejorative 24<br />

wetness 12<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 12<br />

slackness 11<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

9<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 5<br />

beat 3<br />

smooth surface str 3<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g 2<br />

potential<br />

movement<br />

2<br />

sn- 62<br />

pejorative 13<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 10<br />

sound 10<br />

quickness 6<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 4<br />

small end form 4<br />

wetness 4<br />

w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form 3<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive 3<br />

slang 3<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g 2<br />

sp- 47<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 23<br />

wetness 7<br />

jocular 4<br />

th<strong>in</strong> form 3<br />

gaze 2<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

2<br />

attitude 2<br />

pejorative 2<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive 2<br />

spr- 23<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 7<br />

separation 6<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

6<br />

wetness 4<br />

str- 30<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 17<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g 3<br />

stiffness 2<br />

wetness 2<br />

light 2<br />

pejorative 2<br />

f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong> 2<br />

tr- 44<br />

bad mood 8<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 8<br />

pejorative 7<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g 5<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive 4<br />

round form 3<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

3<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 2<br />

short wide form 2<br />

sound 2<br />

133


4.6 Patterns of semantic features<br />

The different semantic features of a certa<strong>in</strong> consonant cluster often do not<br />

only constitute a list of haphazard features, but <strong>in</strong>stead they are related.<br />

These k<strong>in</strong>ds of relations between phonesthemes (e.g. sl-:'smooth surface<br />

structure' (slät) -> 'quick or strong movement (slira, sl<strong>in</strong>gra)) can also be<br />

found with<strong>in</strong> the same phonestheme (e.g. bl-: 'light' (blixt) –>'reflect<strong>in</strong>g<br />

surface structure potentially giv<strong>in</strong>g light' (blank), at a more detailed level<br />

of analysis. In other words, there are many clusters which conta<strong>in</strong> root<br />

morphemes that have similar, but not exactly the same, semantic features.<br />

These features are related <strong>in</strong>dexically or iconically and can be seen as<br />

belong<strong>in</strong>g to the same mean<strong>in</strong>g potential and activated <strong>in</strong> different<br />

contexts. This is especially strik<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the case of root morphemes<br />

connected to 'light', beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with the clusters gl-, bl- and gn-. Most of<br />

these words have been classified as 'light' or 'gaze' <strong>in</strong> the analysis above,<br />

but as shown here they could be further analyzed. Some root morphemes<br />

can denote light, others perception of light, yet others potentiality for<br />

perception of light, etc. The different relations to 'light' are paraphrased<br />

below. Almost all relations between phonesthemic mean<strong>in</strong>gs can be<br />

described as some k<strong>in</strong>d of potentiality. Potentiality is not a semantic<br />

feature but it describes the relation between different mean<strong>in</strong>gs. By<br />

potentiality I mean a relation between mean<strong>in</strong>gs where one mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

describes a possible prerequisite for another mean<strong>in</strong>g, e.g. form can be<br />

seen from the perspective of be<strong>in</strong>g able to let through a bit of light, or a<br />

reflect<strong>in</strong>g (glossy) surface can send out light, cf. the paraphrases below.<br />

The emergence of these relations probably come from simultaneity <strong>in</strong><br />

perception, i.e. a k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>in</strong>dexical relation. In some cases the relation is<br />

iconic.<br />

The structures are similar but not exactly parallel, so the clusters will be<br />

presented one at a time:<br />

gl-<br />

'form through which light<br />

potentially can be perceived'<br />

glipa (narrow space), glugg<br />

(aperture), glänt (slightly open),<br />

glänta (glade)<br />

134


'perception of light'<br />

glana (stare), glo (stare), glutta<br />

(take a glance), glimt (glimpse)<br />

'production or source of light'<br />

glans (lustre), glimma (gleam), glimra (gleam), gl<strong>in</strong>dra<br />

(gleam), glisa (sh<strong>in</strong>e), glittra (glitter), glänsa (sh<strong>in</strong>e), glöd<br />

(glow), gloria (halo), glåmig (pale)<br />

'smooth surface that potentially<br />

reflects light'<br />

glas (glass), glasyr (glaz<strong>in</strong>g), glatt<br />

(smooth), glimmer (gleam<strong>in</strong>g),<br />

gl<strong>in</strong>der (a sh<strong>in</strong>y troll<strong>in</strong>g-spoon),<br />

glaciär (glacier)<br />

'movement on such a surface'<br />

glida (glide)<br />

135


l-<br />

gn-<br />

'perception of light'<br />

blick (gaze), bliga (stare), blänga<br />

(glare), blända (bl<strong>in</strong>d), bl<strong>in</strong>d (bl<strong>in</strong>d),<br />

bl<strong>in</strong>ka (bl<strong>in</strong>k)<br />

'light'<br />

blixt (lightn<strong>in</strong>g), bl<strong>in</strong>ka (tw<strong>in</strong>kle), blänka (sh<strong>in</strong>e), blossa<br />

(flare), blek (pale)<br />

'reflect<strong>in</strong>g surface potentially giv<strong>in</strong>g light'<br />

blank (sh<strong>in</strong>y)<br />

136<br />

(iconic)<br />

'light surface'<br />

bläs (blaze), black (drab)<br />

'momentary light'<br />

gnista (spark), gnistra (sparkle)<br />

'action which produces smooth surface'<br />

gnida (rub), gno (rub), gnugga (rub)


'sound aris<strong>in</strong>g from, and simultaneous<br />

with, such an action'<br />

gnida (rub), gno (rub), gnugga (rub)<br />

'similar sound aris<strong>in</strong>g from similar action'<br />

gnissla (squeak)<br />

'similar sounds aris<strong>in</strong>g from humans or<br />

creatures'<br />

gnabb (bicker<strong>in</strong>g), gnat (nagg<strong>in</strong>g),<br />

gnod<strong>in</strong>g (a murmur<strong>in</strong>g fish), gnola<br />

(hum), gny (d<strong>in</strong>), gnägga (neigh), gnälla<br />

(wh<strong>in</strong>e), gnöl (grumble)<br />

In the last cluster we can see <strong>in</strong>dexical and iconic relations between such<br />

basic categories as 'sound', 'movement' and 'light', e.g. the relation<br />

between the sound and the action is often simultaneous (<strong>in</strong>dexical) or the<br />

relation between this sound and another sound (like <strong>in</strong> gnissla) is likeness<br />

(iconic). Gn- can, <strong>in</strong> other words, be used for other sounds than those<br />

aris<strong>in</strong>g simultaneously with a polish<strong>in</strong>g movement. These are synchronic<br />

relations but one can imag<strong>in</strong>e the historical changes through mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

extension. (The etymological relations, as described <strong>in</strong> e.g. Hellquist<br />

(1966), are not clear.)<br />

Common for the three clusters is the mean<strong>in</strong>g 'light', i. e. the process<br />

which is the source for visual perception. It is of course central. Different<br />

extensions of mean<strong>in</strong>g from 'light' are possible. As can be seen, there are<br />

"empty slots" <strong>in</strong> the mean<strong>in</strong>g patterns for the clusters. For example, gndoes<br />

not have adjectives hav<strong>in</strong>g to do with 'reflect<strong>in</strong>g surface', only verbs<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g 'produc<strong>in</strong>g reflect<strong>in</strong>g surface'. And it does not have root<br />

morphemes for 'perception of light'. These can be accidental gaps. A<br />

speculative explanation of why gn- does not have words for 'perception<br />

137


of light' is that these words often are verbs and that the verbs beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g<br />

with gn- are used for the feature 'sound'.<br />

Similar relations can be found for other semantic fields and for other<br />

clusters, sometimes as different root morphemes beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with a certa<strong>in</strong><br />

cluster, sometimes as semantic features of a root morpheme. Mean<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

often comb<strong>in</strong>ed are e.g. 'sound' - 'movement' - 'destruction' (e.g. braka),<br />

'destruction' - 'pejorative' (e.g bråte), 'wetness' - 'pejorative' (e.g.<br />

slemmig), 'sound' - 'surface structure' (e.g skrovlig), etc, and the relation<br />

between the mean<strong>in</strong>gs (except for 'wetness' - 'pejorative') is nearness <strong>in</strong><br />

space and time (<strong>in</strong>dexical).<br />

The semantic pattern of sl- is similar:<br />

pejorative (slabba (splash), slafs<br />

(slopp<strong>in</strong>ess), slampa (slut), slarvig<br />

(slipshod), slasa (walk lazily),<br />

sladdrig (flabby))<br />

wetness (slafsa (slop), slipprig (slipprig), slem (phlegm))<br />

smooth surface structure (slipa<br />

(polish), slät (smooth))<br />

quick or strong movement (sl<strong>in</strong>ka<br />

(slide), sl<strong>in</strong>ta (glide), slira (skid),<br />

sl<strong>in</strong>gra (w<strong>in</strong>d))<br />

138<br />

potential<br />

movement


139<br />

(slutta (slope),<br />

slänt (slope))<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form (slang (tube),<br />

slimmad (slimmed), slana (scaffold<br />

pole), slejf (strap), sl<strong>in</strong>ga (coil), slips<br />

(tie))<br />

slackness (slamsa (rag), slankig<br />

(limp), slana (scaffold pole), slejf<br />

(strap), sl<strong>in</strong>ga (wreath), slips (tie))<br />

The relations are the follow<strong>in</strong>g:<br />

Wetness causes smooth surface structure (<strong>in</strong>dex).<br />

Smooth surface causes quick or strong movement (<strong>in</strong>dex).<br />

That which moves quickly often has a long th<strong>in</strong> form (<strong>in</strong>dex).<br />

That which has same form as long th<strong>in</strong> form is often slack (<strong>in</strong>dex).<br />

Mean<strong>in</strong>g hierarchies?<br />

One aim of the analysis was to f<strong>in</strong>d mean<strong>in</strong>g hierarchies with<strong>in</strong> clusters,<br />

e.g. that sound symbolism presupposes sound imitation, that movement<br />

presupposes sound imitation, that wetness always implies pejorative, but<br />

such strong claims can not be made. However, there are strong<br />

tendencies:<br />

For all 36 clusters 'pejorative' is the largest category, followed by<br />

'sound'. As mentioned earlier, clusters are often both 'sound' (or<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g') and 'pejorative'. Exceptions are br-, fr-, kv- which are<br />

'sound' but not 'pejorative'. Spj-, spl-, spr- are neither 'sound' nor<br />

'pejorative'.<br />

Clusters with 'quick or strong movement' also have the semantic feature<br />

'sound' (th<strong>in</strong>gs that move quickly often sound) except for one cluster:<br />

spr-. 'Wetness' is almost always co-occurr<strong>in</strong>g with 'sound imitation', also<br />

probably due to the fact that wet th<strong>in</strong>gs often sound.


Some clusters are used for a variety of mean<strong>in</strong>gs (sl-, kl-) while others<br />

are more specialized on one mean<strong>in</strong>g (fj-, pj-).<br />

4.7 Discussion and conclusions<br />

This chapter has shown that certa<strong>in</strong> semantic features are connected with<br />

certa<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters, <strong>in</strong> different comb<strong>in</strong>ations and to<br />

different extents. From the analysis of sound symbolism of the <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

consonant clusters of Swedish some general conclusions can be drawn:<br />

* There is a difference between clusters <strong>in</strong> the number of root<br />

morphemes that are motivated. This is partly due to how lexically<br />

frequent the cluster is. There is also a difference between clusters <strong>in</strong> how<br />

big a proportion of the root morphemes are motivated. Some clusters are<br />

simply more used for motivated root morphemes. The variation is<br />

between 8% (br-) and 100% (fn-). It seems clear that lexically <strong>in</strong>frequent<br />

clusters are exploited for sound symbolism to a higher degree.<br />

(One might ponder about the importance of total or percental number for<br />

the language user's feel<strong>in</strong>g of degree of motivation of a cluster.<br />

Nevertheless this analysis has mostly dealt with percentages. Another<br />

related issue is the issue of textual frequency of motivated root<br />

morphemes, which has not been dealt with here.)<br />

* The mean<strong>in</strong>g profiles for most clusters differ. Different clusters (and<br />

different cluster types) seem to be fitt<strong>in</strong>g for different (types of)<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

* There are often, on a more detailed level of analysis, <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g<br />

relations between the different root morphemes classified as belong<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

the same phonestheme, and sometimes there is a connection between the<br />

different mean<strong>in</strong>gs of a cluster profile, see the analysis of gl-, bl- gnand<br />

sl- above. The mean<strong>in</strong>gs are related <strong>in</strong>dexically or iconically and can<br />

be seen as belong<strong>in</strong>g to the same mean<strong>in</strong>g potential and activated <strong>in</strong><br />

different contexts. It is a question of detail of analysis how many<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs are described.<br />

140


* There is also a frequency difference between the mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Some<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs are quite frequent, e.g. 'sound' and 'pejorative', other mean<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

are less frequent, e.g. 'adhesion' and 'round form'.<br />

* Some mean<strong>in</strong>gs seem to comb<strong>in</strong>e more often with other mean<strong>in</strong>gs, e.g.<br />

pejorative + walk<strong>in</strong>g or talk<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The next chapter will present an analysis of sound symbolism of f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

consonant clusters and of vowels where phonesthemes connected with the<br />

semantic features found <strong>in</strong> the analysis of <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters will be<br />

searched for. The results of the analysis of <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters (as<br />

well as that of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters) have been the base for the experiments with<br />

neologisms presented <strong>in</strong> chapter 7. It was also the base for part of the<br />

cross l<strong>in</strong>guistic studies presented <strong>in</strong> chapter 6.<br />

141


5. Analysis of f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters and<br />

vowels and of comb<strong>in</strong>ations<br />

It seems clear that a greater or lesser part of the sound symbolic<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g of root morphemes can be attributed not only to an <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

consonant sequence but also to the f<strong>in</strong>al consonants, sometimes <strong>in</strong><br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ation with vowels (<strong>in</strong> different positions). It could be possible<br />

that the semantic feature 'quick or strong movement' of e. g. fladdra<br />

(flutter) is attributable not only to fl- (cf. 4.3.4) but also to -dr-. For all<br />

words end<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> -dr- the dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g semantic feature is 'talk<strong>in</strong>g'<br />

(bladdra (talk nonsense), pladdra (babble), sladdra (chatter), bluddra<br />

(talk nonsense), sluddra (slur one's words) but also 'quick or strong<br />

movement' (fladdra and bläddra (turn over the pages). So, <strong>in</strong> the case<br />

of fladdra, fl- is most strongly tied to 'quick and strong movement', but<br />

-dr- also adds to the mean<strong>in</strong>g of the word. In addition it is possible that<br />

the vowels add to the sound symbolic flavor of a word, and that a<br />

neologism like fliddra would depict a yet quicker movement.<br />

To facilitate understand<strong>in</strong>g of the study, the reader is rem<strong>in</strong>ded of the<br />

procedure of decid<strong>in</strong>g which f<strong>in</strong>al clusters should be counted as tentative<br />

phonesthemes (cf. 3.1). When there are at least two 1 root morphemes<br />

end<strong>in</strong>g with the same cluster (which is sometimes followed by an<br />

obligatory vowel, i. e. strictly speak<strong>in</strong>g they are semif<strong>in</strong>al) and hav<strong>in</strong>g<br />

similar mean<strong>in</strong>g (ascerta<strong>in</strong>ed by key words), one phonestheme is<br />

established. If there are roots (normally at least two of each) with<br />

different, sound symbolic, mean<strong>in</strong>gs, different phonesthemes are<br />

established, e.g. -mla 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' (svamla, mumla) and 'quick or strong<br />

movement' (famla, vimla, tromla, fumla, rumla, drumla, tumla). It can<br />

also be the case that words have clearly different mean<strong>in</strong>gs, e.g. blaska.<br />

There is one mean<strong>in</strong>g 'splash' and one pejorative mean<strong>in</strong>g for<br />

'newspaper'. This word is then counted as two roots, and the analysis<br />

proceeds as above. In many cases a word has more than one mean<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

but only one of them is sound symbolic.<br />

1 Or, for lexically <strong>in</strong>frequent clusters, 1 root<br />

143


5.1 F<strong>in</strong>al clusters<br />

It is not as easy to enumerate the f<strong>in</strong>al sequences as it is to list the <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

sequences. In f<strong>in</strong>al sequences, morphological structure has to be taken<br />

<strong>in</strong>to account s<strong>in</strong>ce some sequences only occur <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>flected or derived<br />

forms, e.g. -ndsk <strong>in</strong> bondsk. Many forms are difficult to evaluate,<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to the discussion <strong>in</strong> Sigurd (1965, pp. 67–69), who adopts a<br />

set of mechanical rules which exclude certa<strong>in</strong> forms (secondary forms)<br />

which can be assumed to break the natural phonotactic pattern.<br />

Thus, for practical reasons, f<strong>in</strong>al clusters were studied <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

ways. Primarily they were studied with the aid of 'Svensk<br />

Baklängesordbok' (1981), which is a list of most Swedish lemmas 2<br />

(ordered alphabetically after the end<strong>in</strong>gs of the words only). The f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters that are followed by a have been analyzed, partly s<strong>in</strong>ce the<br />

clusters were easy to f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> that way, but ma<strong>in</strong>ly because some clusters<br />

cannot have word f<strong>in</strong>al position (e.g. C+r- and C+l-sequences). These<br />

are ma<strong>in</strong>ly verbs. (In addition, roots from Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 4<br />

(NFO4) were analyzed, see 5.3 and 5.5). The roots that were excerpted<br />

were those that have one or more of the semantic features discussed<br />

earlier. For this reason also s<strong>in</strong>gle occurrences of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters with<br />

these semantic features were registered. The aim has been to use<br />

different materials <strong>in</strong> order to study as many roots as possible. Not all<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al clusters are sound symbolic 3, as almost all <strong>in</strong>itial clusters are.<br />

The result<strong>in</strong>g 27 sound symbolic f<strong>in</strong>al clusters before a (semi f<strong>in</strong>al) are<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> table 5.1 below. There, gem<strong>in</strong>ate consonants, and not only<br />

consonant clusters, are <strong>in</strong>cluded. The semantic features recurr<strong>in</strong>g more<br />

than once for each f<strong>in</strong>al cluster are with<strong>in</strong> brackets.<br />

Table 5.1 The 27 sound symbolic f<strong>in</strong>al clusters (before a and preceded<br />

by a short vowel), from Svensk Baklängesordbok. 100% <strong>in</strong>dicates that<br />

there is only one root. This root is <strong>in</strong> all <strong>in</strong>dicated cases sound symbolic.<br />

All other clusters have at least two roots with similar mean<strong>in</strong>g (or, for<br />

lexically <strong>in</strong>frequent clusters, 1 root) accord<strong>in</strong>g to the method described<br />

<strong>in</strong> 3.1.<br />

2Svensk Baklängesordbok conta<strong>in</strong>s lemmas from SAOL and from newspaper articles,<br />

and does not exactly match Svensk Ordbok.<br />

3 Approximately 22% of the f<strong>in</strong>al clusters from NFO4 are sound symbolic, cf. table<br />

5.17.<br />

144


%<br />

/tSa/ (sound, quick or strong movement)<br />

/f:a/ (slang4, quick or strong movement)<br />

/ska/ (wetness, pejorative)<br />

/bla/ (talk<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

/fla/ (pejorative, talk<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

/Nla/ (quick or strong movement)<br />

/l:a/ (pejorative, round form, walk<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

/mla/ (quick or strong movement, talk<strong>in</strong>g, sound)<br />

/pla/ (quick or strong movement) 1/15 100%<br />

/rpla/ (sound) 1/1 100%<br />

/spla/ (quick or strong movement) 1/1 100%<br />

/rla/ (sound)<br />

/sla/ (sound, talk<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

/ampa/ (walk<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

/mpa/ (walk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative, quick or strong<br />

movement, short-wide form)<br />

/p:a/ (quick or strong movement)<br />

/bra/ (talk<strong>in</strong>g) 1/1 100%<br />

/dra/ (talk<strong>in</strong>g, quick or strong movement)<br />

/<strong>in</strong>dra/ (light)<br />

/lra/ (quick or strong movement, sound, talk<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

/imra/ (light)<br />

/r:a/ (quick or strong movement, sound, talk<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

/tra/ (quick or strong movement, sound, talk<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

pejorative)<br />

/fsa/ (pejorative, quick or strong movement,<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g, sound)<br />

/msa/ (long th<strong>in</strong> form, pejorative, talk<strong>in</strong>g, quick or<br />

strong movement)<br />

/lta/ (walk<strong>in</strong>g, quick or strong movement)<br />

4 'Slang' is a stylistic feature and thus belongs to another dimension, cf. 4.2.3.<br />

5 1/1 means that there is one root morpheme which is sound symbolic.<br />

145


sta/ (talk<strong>in</strong>g, quick or strong movement)<br />

Examples of sound symbolic root morphemes end<strong>in</strong>g with these f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters from Svensk Baklängesordbok are found <strong>in</strong> table 5.2 below.<br />

Some f<strong>in</strong>al clusters are rare and occur <strong>in</strong> only one or a few words. These<br />

are (translated <strong>in</strong> the table below): klatscha, ratscha, rutscha (three out of<br />

four root morphemes), knaggla, raggla, traggla (three out of four root<br />

morphemes), porla, sorla (two out of four root morphemes) and stöppla,<br />

sörpla, haspla, slabbra (there is only one root morpheme for each of these<br />

last four f<strong>in</strong>al clusters).<br />

146


Table 5.2 <strong>Sound</strong> symbolic roots,<br />

translation and categorization.<br />

klatscha crack sound<br />

ratscha rip sound<br />

rutscha slide quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

haffa nab slang<br />

blaffa large (ugly) color slang<br />

patch<br />

klaffa tally slang<br />

fiffa smarten (up) slang<br />

sniffa sniff slang<br />

piffa smarten (up) slang<br />

skoffa shovel slang<br />

roffa rob slang<br />

buffa nudge slang<br />

skuffa push slang<br />

luffa tramp slang<br />

fluffa fluff (up) slang<br />

knuffa push quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

puffa push quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

ruffa foul slang<br />

gruffa squabble slang<br />

tuffa puff slang<br />

töffa puff slang<br />

blaska splash wetness<br />

plaska splash wetness<br />

sjaska soil pejorative<br />

slaska splash wetness<br />

smaska slurp wetness<br />

snaska munch wetness<br />

vaska wash wetness<br />

babbla babble talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

rabbla rattle talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

gaffla gabble talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

taffla muck th<strong>in</strong>gs up pejorative<br />

fiffla fiddle pejorative<br />

ruffla fiddle pejorative<br />

knaggla plod along movement<br />

traggla go on about talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

dangla dangle quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

rangla be lanky quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

skrangla be rickety quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

d<strong>in</strong>gla dangle quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

147<br />

p<strong>in</strong>gla t<strong>in</strong>kle sound<br />

r<strong>in</strong>gla coil quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

kr<strong>in</strong>gla pretzel w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

v<strong>in</strong>gla stagger quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

fjolla foolish woman pejorative<br />

lolla scatter-bra<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

woman<br />

pejorative<br />

rolla pa<strong>in</strong>t with a round form<br />

roller<br />

skrolla scroll round form<br />

stolla fool of a woman pejorative<br />

bulla bun round form<br />

lulla reel walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

rulla roll round<br />

drulla blunder walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

krulla curl round form<br />

famla grope quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

ramla tumble fall<strong>in</strong>g<br />

skramla rattle sound<br />

svamla drivel talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

strimla strip long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

vimla swarm quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

tromla rotat<strong>in</strong>g quick or strong<br />

cyl<strong>in</strong>drical sieve movement<br />

fumla fumble quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

humla bumble-bee sound<br />

mumla mumble talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

rumla be on the spree quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

drumla blunder quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

tumla tumble quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

stöppla give pa<strong>in</strong>ted quick or strong<br />

surface a certa<strong>in</strong><br />

look by strik<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

brush aga<strong>in</strong>st it<br />

movement<br />

sörpla dr<strong>in</strong>k noisily sound<br />

haspla reel quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

porla ripple sound<br />

sorla ripple sound<br />

rassla rattle sound<br />

prassla rustle sound<br />

tassla whisper talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

gnissla squeak sound<br />

tissla whisper talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

vissla whistle sound<br />

rossla wheeze talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

klampa tramp walk<strong>in</strong>g


slampa slut pejorative<br />

trampa tramp walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

stampa stamp walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

dimpa tumble quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

fimpa stub short-wide form<br />

limpa loaf short-wide form<br />

klimpa get lumpy short-wide form<br />

skvimpa splash to and fro quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

fjompa be silly pejorative<br />

dumpa dump slang<br />

gumpa jog quick or strong<br />

jumpa jump from one<br />

piece of float<strong>in</strong>g<br />

ice to another<br />

movem.<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

skumpa jog quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

klumpa form lumps short-wide form<br />

plumpa make blots form<br />

pumpa pump quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

rumpa rump slang<br />

sumpa blow a th<strong>in</strong>g slang<br />

stumpa t<strong>in</strong>y tot dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

klappa pat quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

snappa snatch quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

rappa strike quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

tappa drop quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

greppa grip quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

steppa tap-dance quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

kippa flop about quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

skippa skip quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

trippa trip along walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tippa tip over quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

vippa sw<strong>in</strong>g up and quick or strong<br />

down<br />

movem.<br />

hoppa jump quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

loppa flea dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

glopp sleet wetness<br />

moppa mop quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

noppa pluck quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

snoppa top and tail quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

148<br />

poppa pop up quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

droppa drip quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

proppa cram quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

stropp sl<strong>in</strong>g round form<br />

guppa jolt quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

knäppa flick quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

snäppa snap one's f<strong>in</strong>gers quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

slabbra chatter talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

bladdra talk nonsense talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

fladdra flutter quick or strong<br />

movem<br />

pladdra babble talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sladdra chatter talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

bluddra talk nonsense talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sluddra slur one's words talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

bläddra turn over the quick or strong<br />

pages<br />

movem.<br />

gl<strong>in</strong>dra gleam light<br />

t<strong>in</strong>dra tw<strong>in</strong>kle light<br />

dallra wobble quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

skallra rattle sound<br />

pillra potter at quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

tillra trickle quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

kvillra ripple, twitter sound<br />

jollra babble talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

pjollra babble talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

knollra curl round form<br />

bullra rumble sound<br />

mullra rumble sound<br />

myllra swarm quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

bjällra bell sound<br />

skimra shimmer light<br />

flimra flicker quick or strong<br />

movem., light<br />

glimra gleam light<br />

darra tremble quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

blarra talk nonsense talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

knarra creak sound<br />

snarra burr talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

irra wander about quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

dirra tremble quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

klirra j<strong>in</strong>gle sound


knirra creak sound<br />

pirra t<strong>in</strong>gle bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

stirra stare gaze<br />

virra wander about quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

kvirra make a fuss talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

svirra whirl quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

skorra burr sound<br />

morra growl talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

knorra grouse talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

burra ruffle up quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

hurra hurrah talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

kurra rumble sound<br />

plurra fall <strong>in</strong>to the water quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

murra growl talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

snurra sp<strong>in</strong> quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

surra hum sound<br />

tjattra jabber talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

klattra be awkward pejorative<br />

smattra clatter sound<br />

knattra rattle sound<br />

snattra quack sound<br />

skvattra quack sound<br />

glittra glitter quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

splittra spl<strong>in</strong>ter quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

fnittra giggle talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

knittra sound high<br />

pitched and<br />

sound<br />

iterated<br />

kvittra chirp sound<br />

klottra scrawl quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

plottra potter about pejorative<br />

knottra get goose- rough surface<br />

pimples structure<br />

huttra shiver quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

kuttra coo sound<br />

muttra mutter talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

puttra chug sound<br />

hafsa scamp a th<strong>in</strong>g pej. quick or<br />

strong movement<br />

sjafsa shuffle quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

tjafsa talk drivel pej., talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

lafsa shuffle pej., walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

klafsa squelch sound, wetness,<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

slafsa slop sound, pejorative<br />

149<br />

nafsa snap quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

snafsa snap quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

rafsa rummage quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

krafsa scratch quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

tafsa fiddle about with quick or strong<br />

a th<strong>in</strong>g movem.<br />

lufsa lumber walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

glufsa gobble down pejorative<br />

plufsa plop quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

rufsa ruffle quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

tufsa tousle quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

gläfsa yelp sound<br />

räfsa rake quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

jamsa talk nonsense talk<strong>in</strong>g, pej.<br />

flamsa fool about pejorative<br />

slamsa rag slackness, long<br />

th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

ramsa str<strong>in</strong>g long (th<strong>in</strong>) form<br />

tramsa talk rubbish talk<strong>in</strong>g, pej.<br />

remsa strip long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

slimsa rag long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

plumsa plop quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

mumsa munch quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

grumsa grumble talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

dalta coddle pejorative<br />

halta limp walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

palta muffle up quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

skralta be rickety quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

skvalta ripple wetness, quick or<br />

strong movem.<br />

bulta beat beat<br />

rulta waddle walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tulta toddle about walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

stulta toddle walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

bylta muffle up quick or strong<br />

movem<br />

stylta walk stiff-legged walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

gasta yell talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

hasta hasten quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

kasta throw quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

rista cut quick or strong<br />

movem.


ista burst destruction<br />

hosta cough talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

pusta pant talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

frusta snort talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

hysta throw quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

150<br />

nysta w<strong>in</strong>d quick or strong<br />

movem.<br />

knysta breath<strong>in</strong>g a word talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

krysta bear down talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

5.2. Summary of the analysis of semantic features<br />

for f<strong>in</strong>al clusters<br />

The most common semantic feature of the f<strong>in</strong>al clusters is 'quick or<br />

strong movement', which is present <strong>in</strong> 15 f<strong>in</strong>al clusters. Table 5.3<br />

compresses all the features and f<strong>in</strong>al clusters of table 5.2. The table can<br />

also be compared with table 5.1, where <strong>in</strong>stead the semantic features for<br />

each f<strong>in</strong>al cluster are shown. The semantic features are shown, <strong>in</strong><br />

descend<strong>in</strong>g order, <strong>in</strong> table 5.3.<br />

Table 5.3 The semantic features of roots from Svensk Baklängesordbok.<br />

Listed are also the clusters correspond<strong>in</strong>g to each feature and the total<br />

frequencies of clusters.<br />

Semantic feature Frequency of<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al clusters<br />

F<strong>in</strong>al clusters6 quick or strong movement 85 ffa, Nla, pla, spla,<br />

mpa, mla, ppa, dra,<br />

lra, rra, tra, fsa, msa,<br />

lta, sta<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 37 bla, fla, mla, sla, bra,<br />

lra, rra, msa, dra,<br />

tra, sta, ska<br />

sound 34 tSa, mla, rpla, rla,<br />

sla, lra, rra, tra, fsa<br />

pejorative 20 fla, lla, mpa, tra, fsa,<br />

msa<br />

slang 19 ffa, mpa<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g 15 lla, ampa, fsa, lta<br />

wetness 9 ska<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 5 msa<br />

round form 7 lla<br />

light 5 <strong>in</strong>dra, imra<br />

short-wide form 4 mpa<br />

6 Double consonant grapheme stands for a phonologically long consonant.


In table 5.4 the rank<strong>in</strong>g of table 5.3 is compared with diagram 4.5, which<br />

shows the most frequent mean<strong>in</strong>gs for i n i t i a l consonant clusters.<br />

Table 5.4 The most frequent semantic features for <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters.<br />

rank <strong>in</strong>itial clusters f<strong>in</strong>al clusters<br />

1 pejorative quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

2 sound talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

3 long th<strong>in</strong> form sound<br />

4 quick or strong pejorative<br />

movement<br />

5 wetness slang<br />

6 talk<strong>in</strong>g walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

7 light wetness<br />

8 dim<strong>in</strong>utive long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

9 round form round form<br />

10 walk<strong>in</strong>g light<br />

11 destruction short-wide form<br />

The semantic features <strong>in</strong> bold type are those n<strong>in</strong>e that are among the<br />

eleven most common features for both groups. '<strong>Sound</strong>' is ranked second<br />

for <strong>in</strong>itial clusters and third for f<strong>in</strong>al clusters. 'Quick or strong<br />

movement' is ranked first for f<strong>in</strong>al clusters and fourth for <strong>in</strong>itial clusters.<br />

'Pejorative' is ranked fourth for f<strong>in</strong>al clusters but first for <strong>in</strong>itial clusters.<br />

The semantic features of the six most frequent f<strong>in</strong>al clusters are among<br />

the semantic features of the ten most frequent <strong>in</strong>itial clusters. In other<br />

words, many of the most common semantic features are the same for<br />

<strong>in</strong>itial clusters and f<strong>in</strong>al clusters. However, 'pejorative' is not as common<br />

<strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al clusters as <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial.<br />

Some clusters can occur both <strong>in</strong>itially and f<strong>in</strong>ally (before a), namely sk,<br />

bl, fl, spl, sl, br, dr, tr and st. Four of these can have the same<br />

semantic feature. These are bl (talk<strong>in</strong>g), fl (pejorative), sl (talk<strong>in</strong>g), tr<br />

(quick or strong movement, sound, talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative). However, there<br />

are no conventional words that both beg<strong>in</strong> and end with these<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ations.<br />

151


5.3 Properties of consonant clusters of Nusvensk<br />

Frekvensordbok<br />

Us<strong>in</strong>g the root analysis with paraphrases of Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 4<br />

(NFO4), an analysis of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters that resembles the analysis of <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

clusters was made, i.e. sound symbolic roots were excerpted from a<br />

larger set of roots with the aid of key words (cf. Appendix 1). NFO4 is a<br />

frequency dictionary of Swedish morphemes, and the material consists of<br />

1 million words from newspaper articles. List 4.3 of NFO4 conta<strong>in</strong>s the<br />

roots ordered <strong>in</strong> reverse. The reason not to analyze only the NFO4<br />

material is that there are many more words <strong>in</strong> Svensk Baklängesordbok.<br />

The roots from the NFO4 material are chosen <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g way: they<br />

shall conta<strong>in</strong> at least one root end<strong>in</strong>g of a cluster, if this root belongs to<br />

one of the semantic categories listed <strong>in</strong> chapter 4.<br />

The most common clusters, absolutely and percentally, are shown <strong>in</strong><br />

diagrams 5.1 and 5.2, and <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g diagrams (5.3 to 5.11) the<br />

semantic profiles of some of the most common clusters are shown.<br />

The first diagram (5.1) shows how the clusters rank when the total<br />

number of sound symbolic roots are counted. -Nk and -sk are the clusters<br />

with the largest number of sound symbolic roots. Thereafter follow - fs, -<br />

nd and -tr. -Nk, -sk, -fs and -tr are described <strong>in</strong> detail below. Diagram<br />

5.1 corresponds to diagram 4.1 of <strong>in</strong>itial clusters.<br />

The next diagram (5.2) shows the clusters that have the highest degree of<br />

sound symbolic roots. The first five have a percentage of 100% because<br />

there is only one – sound symbolic – root, for every cluster. These are<br />

not studied <strong>in</strong> further detail. The follow<strong>in</strong>g ones with quite a high<br />

percentage are -fs, -dr and -lr, followed by -ml and -Nl, These are<br />

described <strong>in</strong> detail below. Also -bl is described. Diagram 5.2 corresponds<br />

to diagram 4.4 of <strong>in</strong>itial clusters.<br />

152


20<br />

18<br />

16<br />

14<br />

12<br />

10<br />

8<br />

6<br />

4<br />

2<br />

0<br />

ksn<br />

ltn<br />

spl<br />

mj<br />

rS<br />

jf<br />

lb<br />

mt<br />

lt<br />

ps<br />

rl<br />

Sk<br />

rj<br />

ld<br />

ls<br />

lm<br />

rv<br />

nt<br />

ns<br />

ms<br />

sl<br />

Nl<br />

bl<br />

lk<br />

lr<br />

dr<br />

st<br />

ml<br />

mp<br />

tr<br />

nd<br />

fs<br />

sk<br />

Nk<br />

Diagram 5.1 7 More and less sound symbolic f<strong>in</strong>al clusters. Number of motivated root morphemes per cluster.<br />

7 In this and the follow<strong>in</strong>g diagrams "N" stands for [N] and "S" stands for [S].


100<br />

90<br />

80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

nt<br />

lt<br />

st<br />

ns<br />

rj<br />

ld<br />

nd<br />

rv<br />

ps<br />

rl<br />

mt<br />

ls<br />

lk<br />

lm<br />

mp<br />

sk<br />

bl<br />

ms<br />

sl<br />

tr<br />

Nk<br />

Sk<br />

Nl<br />

ml<br />

lr<br />

dr<br />

ksn<br />

fs<br />

ltn<br />

spl<br />

mj<br />

rS<br />

jf<br />

lb<br />

Diagram 5.2 Percent of motivated root morphemes per cluster, for f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters.


The follow<strong>in</strong>g diagrams, 5.3 to 5.11 show the semantic profiles for<br />

different f<strong>in</strong>al clusters. These diagrams can be compared with the<br />

diagrams 4.16 – 4.23, which show the semantic profiles for different<br />

<strong>in</strong>itial clusters. Diagram 5.3 shows the semantic profile of -fs.<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

pejorative sound quickness<br />

Diagram 5.3 Semantic features of the cluster -fs. Percentages concern the<br />

number of semantic features <strong>in</strong> proportion to all roots of the cluster.<br />

Diagram 5.3 shows the semantic features of the consonant cluster -fs. The<br />

words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g features are<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> table 5.5. The absolute frequencies (not the percentages) of the<br />

semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

Table 5.5 The sound symbolic morphemes of the -fs cluster, the semantic<br />

features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'pejorative': hafs (slovenl<strong>in</strong>ess), tjafs<br />

(drivel), slafs (slopp<strong>in</strong>ess), rafs (trash),<br />

krafs (knick-knacks), lufsa (shamble),<br />

kalufs (forelock), rufsa (ruffle),<br />

tufsa (tousle), bjäfs (gewgaws) 10<br />

'sound': klafs (squelch), gläfs (yelp) 2<br />

'quickness': nafs (snap), rafs (trash) 2<br />

155<br />

%


10<br />

9<br />

8<br />

7<br />

6<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

sound<br />

Diagram 5.4 The semantic profile of -Nk. Percentages concern the<br />

number of semantic features <strong>in</strong> proportion to all roots of the cluster.<br />

Diagram 5.4 shows the semantic features of the consonant cluster -Nk. In<br />

table 5.6. the words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g<br />

features are shown. The absolute frequencies (not the percentages) of the<br />

semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

Table 5.6 The sound symbolic morphemes of the -Nk cluster, the semantic<br />

features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> form': bank (bar), dank<br />

(th<strong>in</strong> candle), hank (a band), planka<br />

(plank), rank (tall and slender), sp<strong>in</strong>kig<br />

(slender), rynka (furrow) 7<br />

'quick or strong movement': fl<strong>in</strong>k<br />

(quick), sl<strong>in</strong>k (slip), v<strong>in</strong>k (wave) 3<br />

beat<br />

156<br />

slang<br />

wetness<br />

%


'sound': banka (knock), dunk (thump<strong>in</strong>g),<br />

stånka (puff and blow) 3<br />

'beat': banka (knock), dunka (thump) 2<br />

'slang': pank (broke), grunka (gadget) 2<br />

'wetness': klunk (gulp), stänk (splash) 2<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g diagram (5.5) shows the semantic profile of -sk.<br />

8<br />

7<br />

6<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

wetness pejorative beat sound<br />

Diagram 5.5 The semantic profile of -sk. Percentages concern the number<br />

of semantic features <strong>in</strong> proportion to all roots of the cluster.<br />

Diagram 5.5 shows the semantic features of the consonant cluster -sk. The<br />

words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g features are<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> table 5.7. The absolute frequencies (not the percentages) of the<br />

semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

Table 5.7 The sound symbolic morphemes of the -sk cluster, the semantic<br />

features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'wetness': blask (slops), plask (splash),<br />

slask (slush), smaska (guzzle), vaska<br />

(wash), loska (spit), mäsk (mash),<br />

träsk (swamp) 8<br />

157<br />

%


'pejorative': sjask (cad), fnask<br />

(prostitute), snaska (be messy), fnoskig<br />

(dotty), fjäsk (fawn<strong>in</strong>g) 5<br />

'beat': daska (slap), plask (splash), piska (whip) 3<br />

'sound': pladask (flop), smaska (guzzle) 2<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g diagram (5.6) shows the semantic profile of -tr.<br />

14<br />

12<br />

10<br />

8<br />

6<br />

4<br />

2<br />

0<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g sound pejorative<br />

Diagram 5.6 The semantic profile of -tr. Percentages concern the number<br />

of semantic features <strong>in</strong> proportion to all roots of the cluster.<br />

The diagram 5.6 shows the semantic features of the consonant cluster -tr.<br />

In table 5.8 the words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g<br />

features can be studied. The absolute frequencies (not the percentages) of<br />

the semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

Table 5.8 The sound symbolic morphemes of the -tr cluster, the semantic<br />

features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g': tjattra (jabber), snattra (quack),<br />

fnittra (giggle), muttra (mutter),<br />

yttra (utter) 5<br />

158<br />

%


'sound': smattra (clatter), knattra (rattle),<br />

kvittra (chirp), puttra (simmer) 4<br />

'pejorative': tjattra (jabber), plottra (fritter) 2<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g diagram (5.7) shows the semantic profile of -Nl.<br />

40<br />

35<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

quick or strong movement<br />

Diagram 5.7 The semantic profile of -Nl. The percentage is of the<br />

semantic feature, for all roots of the cluster.<br />

Diagram 5.7 shows the semantic feature of the consonant cluster -NNl. The<br />

words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the only feature is shown <strong>in</strong><br />

table 5.9. The absolute frequency (not the percentage) of the semantic<br />

feature is shown to the right.<br />

159<br />

%


Table 5.9 The sound symbolic morphemes of the -Nl cluster, the semantic<br />

feature <strong>in</strong>volved, and its absolute frequency.<br />

'quick or strong movement': d<strong>in</strong>gla<br />

(dangle), s<strong>in</strong>gla (dance), v<strong>in</strong>gla (wobble),<br />

jonglera (juggle) 4<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g diagram (5.8) shows the semantic profile of -bl. The<br />

percentage is of the semantic feature for all roots of the cluster.<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Diagram 5.8 The semantic profile of -bl. The percentage is of semantic<br />

features for all roots of the cluster.<br />

Diagram 5.8 shows the semantic feature of the consonant cluster -bl. The<br />

words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the only feature is shown <strong>in</strong><br />

table 5.10. The absolute frequency (not the percentage) of the semantic<br />

feature is shown to the right.<br />

Table 5.10 The sound symbolic morphemes of the -bl cluster, the<br />

semantic features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g': babbla (babble), rabbla (rattle),<br />

käbbla (bicker), jubla (shout with joy) 4<br />

160<br />

%


The follow<strong>in</strong>g diagram (5.9) shows the semantic profile of -dr.<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

quick or<br />

strong<br />

movement<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g pejorative<br />

Diagram 5.9 The semantic profile of -dr. Percentages concern the<br />

number of semantic features <strong>in</strong> proportion to all roots of the cluster.<br />

This diagram shows the semantic features of the consonant cluster -dr. In<br />

table 5.11 the words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g<br />

features are shown. The absolute frequencies (not the percentages) of the<br />

semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

Table 5.11 The sound symbolic morphemes of the -dr cluster, the<br />

semantic features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'quick or strong movement': fladdra (flutter), 2<br />

bläddra (turn over the pages)<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g': pladdra (babble), sluddra (slur) 2<br />

'pejorative': pladdra (babble), sluddra (slur) 2<br />

The next diagram (5.10) shows the semantic profile of -lr. Percentages<br />

are of semantic features for all roots of the cluster.<br />

161<br />

%


45<br />

40<br />

35<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

sound quick or<br />

strong<br />

movement<br />

Diagram 5.10 The semantic profile of -lr. Percentages are of semantic<br />

features for all roots of the cluster.<br />

Diagram 5.10 shows the semantic features of the consonant cluster -lr.<br />

The words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g features are<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> table 5.12. The absolute frequencies (not the percentages) of the<br />

semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

Table 5.12 The sound symbolic morphemes of the -lr cluster, the<br />

semantic features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'sound': skallra (rattle), bullra (make a noise),<br />

mullra (rumble), bjällra (j<strong>in</strong>gle) 4<br />

'quick or strong movement': dallra (tremble),<br />

myllra (swarm) 2<br />

The last diagram (5.11) shows the semantic profile of -ml.<br />

162<br />

%


20<br />

18<br />

16<br />

14<br />

12<br />

10<br />

8<br />

6<br />

4<br />

2<br />

0<br />

quick or<br />

strong<br />

movement<br />

movement sound<br />

Diagram 5.11 The semantic profile of -ml. Percentages concern the<br />

number of semantic features <strong>in</strong> proportion to all roots of the cluster.<br />

Diagram 5.11 shows the semantic features of the consonant cluster -ml.<br />

The words (represent<strong>in</strong>g root morphemes) of the dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g features are<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> table 5.13. The absolute frequencies (not the percentages) of the<br />

semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

Table 5.13 The sound symbolic morphemes of the -ml cluster, the<br />

semantic features <strong>in</strong>volved, and their absolute frequencies.<br />

'quick or strong movement': vimla (swarm), 3<br />

fumla (fumble), tumla (tumble)<br />

'movement': famla (grope), ramla (fall down) 2<br />

'sound': skramla (rattle), humla (bumble-bee) 2<br />

163<br />

%


5.3.1 Summary of the analysis of properties of<br />

different f<strong>in</strong>al clusters<br />

As well as for the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters, the mean<strong>in</strong>g profiles for the sound<br />

symbolic f<strong>in</strong>al clusters vary. Different clusters are connected with<br />

different mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Also the f<strong>in</strong>al clusters have different semantic<br />

profiles. Some f<strong>in</strong>al clusters have several semantic features, others have<br />

only one.<br />

Of these absolutely and percentally most common clusters (-Nk, -sk,-fs, -<br />

nd, -tr, -dr, -lr, -ml, -Nl), 5 out of 9 end with a liquid and are ma<strong>in</strong>ly<br />

connected with 'quick or strong movement', 'talk<strong>in</strong>g', 'sound', and<br />

'pejorative'. -sk is dom<strong>in</strong>ated by 'wetness' and -fs is clearly 'pejorative'<br />

5.4. Vowels<br />

As mentioned earlier, vowels may also have sound symbolic mean<strong>in</strong>g. To<br />

delimit the analysis of vowels, the contrastive effect of different vowels<br />

was studied.<br />

5.4.1 Vowel pairs and triplets<br />

In order to <strong>in</strong>vestigate the semantic effect of different vowels, word pairs<br />

were searched for. However, there are not so many cases of word pairs<br />

with contrast<strong>in</strong>g vowels. Those that were found contrast the vowels [i]-[a],<br />

[i]-[a]-[u], [i]-[o], [i]-[P]. Most of them imitate sound (8 out of 19). The<br />

examples (taken from Svensk Baklängesordbok) are: klibba-klabba<br />

(adhesion 8), slibba-slabba (wetness), slidder-sladder 9 (talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative),<br />

klifsa-klafsa (wetness, sound), klitsch-klatsch (sound), ritsch-ratsch<br />

(sound), snick-snack (talk<strong>in</strong>g), r<strong>in</strong>gla-rangla (movement), tissla-tassla<br />

(talk<strong>in</strong>g), rispa-raspa (rough surface structure), dirr and darr<br />

(movement), knirra-knarra (sound), knistra-knastra (sound), knittraknattra<br />

(sound), slimsa-slamsa (form) snipp-snapp-snorum (<strong>in</strong> nonsense<br />

verse), dripp-dropp (sound), tick-tock (sound), klimp-klump (form).<br />

From the def<strong>in</strong>itions <strong>in</strong> Svensk Ordbok it is not evident that there is a<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g dist<strong>in</strong>ction (size or pitch) between [i] and [a] except for some<br />

cases, but clearly [i] never stands for anyth<strong>in</strong>g large or low-pitched. (For<br />

an <strong>in</strong>vestigation of English reduplicatives see Thun, 1963.)<br />

8Shown with<strong>in</strong> parenthesis is the semantic feature for both words <strong>in</strong> the pair. Some<br />

words share two semantic features.<br />

9 In fact, many of these pairs occur as semi-lexicalized compounds, e.g. <strong>in</strong> poetry and<br />

verse.<br />

164


However, there are a number of words found <strong>in</strong> Svensk Baklängesordbok<br />

which show that root morphemes with end<strong>in</strong>gs conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g [i] often have<br />

the mean<strong>in</strong>gs 'smallness', 'quickness' or 'high pitch', while [P] has the<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs of darker sounds or bigger movements – while [a] seems quite<br />

neutral. Compar<strong>in</strong>g the end<strong>in</strong>gs [<strong>in</strong>ka] and [Pnka] <strong>in</strong> table 5.14, there is, <strong>in</strong><br />

bl<strong>in</strong>ka (tw<strong>in</strong>kle) and lunka (jog along), a contrast <strong>in</strong> small, quick<br />

movement versus big, slow movement. In kl<strong>in</strong>ka (t<strong>in</strong>kle) and dunka<br />

(thump) there is a contrast between high pitched and low pitched sound.<br />

Table 5.14 The vowels i, P, a before the end<strong>in</strong>g -NNka<br />

k<strong>in</strong>ka wh<strong>in</strong>e<br />

l<strong>in</strong>ka limp<br />

bl<strong>in</strong>ka tw<strong>in</strong>kle<br />

kl<strong>in</strong>ka t<strong>in</strong>kle<br />

sl<strong>in</strong>ka slip<br />

sp<strong>in</strong>ka split (wood)<br />

v<strong>in</strong>ka wave (hand)<br />

dunka thump<br />

pjunka coddle<br />

lunka jog along<br />

klunka gulp down<br />

runka shake<br />

banka bang<br />

klanka grouse<br />

planka plank<br />

slanka make slim<br />

ranka creeper<br />

vanka saunter<br />

svanka be sway-backed<br />

The vowels with the end<strong>in</strong>g -lra can also be compared. Most of the -lra<br />

words imitate sounds or movements where i clearly stands for smallness<br />

or high pitch and [P] for low pitch, e.g. tillra (trickle) and mullra<br />

(rumble).<br />

165


Table 5.15 The vowels a, i, O, P before the end<strong>in</strong>g -lra.<br />

dallra quiver<br />

skallra rattle<br />

pillra potter at<br />

tillra trickle<br />

kvillra ripple, twitter<br />

jollra prattle<br />

pjollra twaddle<br />

knollra frizz<br />

bullra rumble<br />

mullra rumble<br />

The i and [P] vowels <strong>in</strong> words end<strong>in</strong>g with -ttra show similar tendencies,<br />

cf. e.g. kvittra (twitter) and muttra (mutter).<br />

Table 5.16 The vowels a, i, O, P before the end<strong>in</strong>g -tra.<br />

tjattra chatter<br />

klattra make a fuss<br />

smattra clatter<br />

knattra rattle<br />

snattra quack<br />

skvattra quack<br />

glittra glitter<br />

splittra spl<strong>in</strong>ter<br />

fnittra giggle<br />

knittra sound high<br />

pitched and<br />

iterated<br />

kvittra twitter<br />

knottra get goosepimples<br />

huttra shiver<br />

kuttra coo<br />

muttra mutter<br />

puttra chug<br />

5.4.2 Vowels <strong>in</strong> light/gaze-words<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g list conta<strong>in</strong>s most light or gaze root morphemes:<br />

blek (pale), blick (gaze), bliga (stare), bl<strong>in</strong>d (bl<strong>in</strong>d), bl<strong>in</strong>ka (bl<strong>in</strong>k), blixt<br />

(lightn<strong>in</strong>g), blossa (flare), blända (bl<strong>in</strong>d), blänga (glare), blänka (sh<strong>in</strong>e),<br />

flimra (flicker), glana (stare), glans (luster), glatt (glossy), glimma<br />

(gleam), glimra (gleam), glimta (gleam), gl<strong>in</strong>dra (gleam), glisa (sh<strong>in</strong>e),<br />

glittra (glitter), glo (stare), gloria (halo), glutta (take a glance), glåmig<br />

166


(pale), glänsa (sh<strong>in</strong>e), glöd (glow), gnista (spark), gnistra (sparkle), t<strong>in</strong>dra<br />

(tw<strong>in</strong>kle), skimra (shimmer).<br />

The stressed vowels of these 30 root morphemes are <strong>in</strong> 16 cases i, <strong>in</strong> 1<br />

case e, <strong>in</strong> 4 cases E, <strong>in</strong> 1 case Ø, <strong>in</strong> 3 cases a (2 [a] and 1 [A:]), <strong>in</strong> 2 cases o,<br />

<strong>in</strong> 2 cases u, and <strong>in</strong> 1 case [P]. The dom<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g vowel is clearly i.<br />

Furthermore, 24 of the vowels are front vowels; only 5 are back vowels<br />

and 1 is central. It seems thus that the feature 'light' is connected with<br />

front vowels (high F2).<br />

5.4.3 The vowel [P]<br />

S<strong>in</strong>ce there seemed to be a tendency for words with [P] (a short, medial,<br />

half closed, rounded vowel) to have a pejorative mean<strong>in</strong>g (see table 5.2),<br />

this vowel was studied more closely. In the whole material (Appendix 1)<br />

the number of pejorative features is 163. Of these features 33 belong to<br />

roots with [P], i.e. 20%. The number of non-pejorative features is 789 and<br />

70 of these belong to roots with a [P], i.e. 8%. A Chi square test showed<br />

that there is a significant correlation between 'pejorative' and [P] (p. <<br />

0.0001).<br />

5.4.4 Summary of vowels<br />

From the examples <strong>in</strong> this material it seems quite clear that stressed<br />

vowels are connected with the semantic dimensions 'size' and 'high-low<br />

pitch' (which are connected through the frequency code); i tends to have<br />

the mean<strong>in</strong>gs 'smallness', 'quickness', 'high pitch', while [P] seems to have<br />

the mean<strong>in</strong>gs of 'low pitch' and 'largeness'. a seems quite neutral. The<br />

special study of [P] showed that this sound is over-represented among<br />

roots with the pejorative feature.<br />

5.5. Comparisons of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters of roots from<br />

different sources<br />

5.5.1 NFO4 and Sigurd (1965)<br />

S<strong>in</strong>ce the question of which f<strong>in</strong>al comb<strong>in</strong>ations should be taken <strong>in</strong>to<br />

account is somewhat open, a comparison is made between NFO4 and<br />

Sigurd (1965). Table 5.17 shows the f<strong>in</strong>al clusters which are found <strong>in</strong> the<br />

two different sources. The comparison with Sigurd (1965) is <strong>in</strong>cluded to<br />

rem<strong>in</strong>d the reader that the f<strong>in</strong>al clusters of NFO4 are not exactly the same<br />

167


as those of other analyses and to show which clusters are found <strong>in</strong> both<br />

sources.<br />

168


Table 5.17 The f<strong>in</strong>al clusters of<br />

NFO4, those of NFO4 that are<br />

sound symbolic, and, as a<br />

comparison, the primary f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters accord<strong>in</strong>g to Sigurd<br />

(1965).<br />

Clusters<br />

for all<br />

reverse<br />

order<br />

sorted<br />

roots of<br />

NFO4<br />

Clusters<br />

for sound<br />

symbolic<br />

roots<br />

from<br />

NFO4<br />

lb + +<br />

mb +<br />

rb<br />

gd +<br />

jd +<br />

ld + +<br />

md +<br />

nd + +<br />

vd +<br />

jf + +<br />

lf +<br />

mf +<br />

rf +<br />

sf<br />

rg +<br />

dg<br />

rj + +<br />

ntg<br />

nS +<br />

rk<br />

nSS<br />

rS + +<br />

tS +<br />

dj<br />

lj +<br />

mj + +<br />

nj +<br />

rj +<br />

lC<br />

tj<br />

vj<br />

jk +<br />

lk + +<br />

mk<br />

Nk + +<br />

rk +<br />

sk + +<br />

lsk +<br />

msk +<br />

Primary<br />

clusters<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to Sigurd<br />

169<br />

nsk +<br />

psk +<br />

Sk + +<br />

bl +<br />

mbl<br />

ndl<br />

fl<br />

gl<br />

Nl +<br />

rjl<br />

gl<br />

jl<br />

kl<br />

NNkl<br />

rkl<br />

ml +<br />

pl<br />

mpl<br />

spl +<br />

rl +<br />

Nsl<br />

jsl<br />

nsl<br />

psl<br />

sl +<br />

ntl<br />

stl<br />

vl<br />

rvl<br />

ksl<br />

gm<br />

hm<br />

km<br />

lm + +<br />

rm +<br />

sm<br />

stm<br />

tm<br />

Nn +<br />

jn<br />

kn<br />

ln +<br />

mn +<br />

pn<br />

rn<br />

sn<br />

tn<br />

ltn +<br />

ksn + +<br />

jp<br />

lp<br />

mp + +<br />

rp +


sp +<br />

br<br />

rbr<br />

dr +<br />

ldr<br />

ndr<br />

dr +<br />

rdr<br />

fr<br />

gr<br />

kr<br />

Nkr<br />

lr +<br />

mr<br />

nr<br />

pr<br />

tr +<br />

rtr<br />

str<br />

kstr<br />

vr<br />

lvr<br />

bs<br />

ds<br />

fs + +<br />

js +<br />

ks +<br />

ls + +<br />

ms + +<br />

ns + +<br />

Ns +<br />

ps + +<br />

mps<br />

rps<br />

ts +<br />

ft +<br />

rft +<br />

kt<br />

Nt<br />

jt +<br />

kt +<br />

Nkt<br />

rkt<br />

lt + +<br />

mt +<br />

nt + +<br />

pt +<br />

st + +<br />

fst +<br />

Nst +<br />

jst +<br />

kst +<br />

mst +<br />

170<br />

nst +<br />

St +<br />

kv<br />

lv +<br />

rv + +<br />

nts


Table 5.17 shows the follow<strong>in</strong>g: There are, <strong>in</strong> the NFO-material, 151<br />

different end<strong>in</strong>gs of roots. Of these, 33 are used systematically for sound<br />

symbolism.<br />

The rightmost column shows which of these clusters are primary f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ations accord<strong>in</strong>g to Sigurd (1965). Quite a few root f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ations <strong>in</strong> NFO4 are not the same as Sigurd’s word f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ations. Also some of Sigurd’s f<strong>in</strong>al comb<strong>in</strong>ations do not occur <strong>in</strong><br />

the NFO-material: tsk, pst, lft, lst, NNd, NNt.<br />

5.5.2 The most frequent f<strong>in</strong>al clusters <strong>in</strong> NFO4<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g tables (5.18 and 5.19) show which f<strong>in</strong>al clusters are the<br />

most frequent for different semantic features, both absolutely and<br />

percentally. They show that fs is the most sound symbolic f<strong>in</strong>al cluster<br />

(pejorative) both <strong>in</strong> absolute numbers and percentally.<br />

Table 5.18 The most frequent f<strong>in</strong>al clusters <strong>in</strong> absolute numbers. Their<br />

semantic features are shown to the right.<br />

Cluster Cluster<br />

frequency<br />

Semantic feature<br />

fs 10 pejorative<br />

sk 8 wetness<br />

Nk<br />

sk<br />

tr<br />

st<br />

7<br />

5<br />

5<br />

5<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

pejorative<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sound<br />

lk<br />

bl<br />

Nl<br />

sl<br />

4<br />

4<br />

4<br />

4<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

sound<br />

lr 4 sound<br />

tr<br />

ms<br />

4<br />

4<br />

sound<br />

pejorative<br />

171


Table 5.19 The most frequent f<strong>in</strong>al clusters <strong>in</strong> percent 10 of the total<br />

number of roots for clusters. Their semantic features are shown to the<br />

right.<br />

Cluster % Semantic feature<br />

fs 59 pejorative<br />

lr 44 sound<br />

Nl 40 quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

Sk 29 attitude<br />

sl 27 sound<br />

ms 27 pejorative<br />

dr 25 quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

dr 25 talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

dr 25 pejorative<br />

bl 24 talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

lr 22 quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

The accumulated frequencies of table 5.18 show that the most common<br />

semantic feature is 'pejorative' with 19 roots (distributed over 3 clusters)<br />

followed by 'sound' with 17 roots (distributed over 4 clusters), 'long th<strong>in</strong><br />

form' with 11 roots distributed over 2 clusters, and 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' with 9 roots<br />

distributed over 2 clusters.<br />

5.5.3 Discussion of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters <strong>in</strong> Svensk<br />

Baklängesordbok and <strong>in</strong> Nusvensk Frekvensordbok<br />

As po<strong>in</strong>ted out <strong>in</strong> the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of this chapter, it is not self-evident which<br />

are the f<strong>in</strong>al clusters <strong>in</strong> Swedish. The semif<strong>in</strong>al clusters (before a) of<br />

Svensk Baklängesordbok (the Reverse Order Dictionary), the f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters of Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 4, and of Sigurd (1965) are<br />

partially overlapp<strong>in</strong>g sets. For practical reasons, percentages are<br />

calculated only on Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 4.<br />

Likewise, the roots of the two dictionaries also constitute partially<br />

overlapp<strong>in</strong>g sets. The lemmas (from which roots were extracted) of<br />

Svensk Baklängesordbok come from the Wordlist of the Swedish<br />

Academy (Svenska Akademiens ordlista över svenska språket, 1974) and<br />

10 The follow<strong>in</strong>g clusters have a 100%-frequency because they have only one root, which<br />

is sound symbolic: lb (round form), jf (long th<strong>in</strong> form), rS (walk<strong>in</strong>g), mj (bodily<br />

feel<strong>in</strong>g), spl (quick or strong movement), ltn (destruction) and ksn (mental feel<strong>in</strong>g).<br />

172


from the same newspaper material as the material of Nusvensk<br />

Frekvensordbok. Consequently Svensk Baklängesordbok conta<strong>in</strong>s more<br />

roots. The similarities and the differences <strong>in</strong> the analyses of the two<br />

dictionaries are as follows (cf. also tables 5.4 and 5.24).<br />

The most common semantic features<br />

The most common semantic features are, <strong>in</strong> order:<br />

Svensk Baklängesordbok: 'quick or strong movement', 'talk<strong>in</strong>g', 'sound',<br />

'pejorative', 'slang', 'walk<strong>in</strong>g', 'wetness', 'long th<strong>in</strong> form', 'round form',<br />

'light', 'short-wide form'.<br />

Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 4: 'pejorative', 'sound', 'long th<strong>in</strong> form',<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g', 'wetness', 'quick or strong movement'.<br />

For both dictionaries the follow<strong>in</strong>g features are the same: 'quick or strong<br />

movement', 'talk<strong>in</strong>g', 'sound', 'pejorative', 'wetness' and 'long th<strong>in</strong> form'.<br />

(These six features are also the six most common features of <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

clusters, however <strong>in</strong> a different order, cf. table 5.4).<br />

The most common f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g all sound symbolic<br />

semantic features) are, <strong>in</strong> descend<strong>in</strong>g order (tables 5.20 and 5.21).<br />

Table 5.20 The most common f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters (for all sound<br />

symbolic semantic features) <strong>in</strong> Svensk Baklängesordbok. Cf. table 5.2.<br />

mp 20<br />

fs 18<br />

tr 18<br />

ml 13<br />

st 12<br />

lr 12<br />

lt 11<br />

ms 10<br />

Nl 8<br />

sl 7<br />

dr 7<br />

sk 6<br />

fl 4<br />

tS 3<br />

173


In Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 4 the most common f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters,<br />

<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g all sound symbolic semantic features, are (cf. diagram 5.1):<br />

Table 5.21 The most common f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters (for all sound<br />

symbolic semantic features) <strong>in</strong> Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 4.<br />

Nk 19<br />

sk 18<br />

fs 14<br />

nd 12<br />

tr 11<br />

mp 8<br />

ml 7<br />

st 7<br />

dr 6<br />

lr 6<br />

As can be seen <strong>in</strong> the two lists above, many, but not all, of the most<br />

common clusters are the same, namely mp, fs, tr, ml, st, lr, dr, and sk,<br />

<strong>in</strong> slightly different order.<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g clusters used sound symbolically are found <strong>in</strong> Svensk<br />

Baklängesordbok but not <strong>in</strong> Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 4: fl, tS, gl, pl,<br />

rpl and br.<br />

The differences between the two dictionaries seem to be greater for the<br />

consonant clusters than for the semantic features, but the most common<br />

consonant clusters are to a great extent the same. Study<strong>in</strong>g not only the<br />

most common clusters, there are more sound symbolic clusters <strong>in</strong> Svensk<br />

Baklängesordbok. This might be due to the fact, described above, that<br />

Svensk Baklängesordbok is based on a larger selection.<br />

5.6 Comb<strong>in</strong>ations of <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonant<br />

clusters<br />

Initial and f<strong>in</strong>al clusters can comb<strong>in</strong>e to create mean<strong>in</strong>g, e.g. 'wetness' as<br />

<strong>in</strong> blaska, plaska, slaska, smaska, snaska. The f<strong>in</strong>al comb<strong>in</strong>aton -ska is<br />

connected with 'wetness' and the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters bl-, pl-, sl-, sm- and sncan<br />

also stand for 'wetness' (among other th<strong>in</strong>gs).<br />

174


The f<strong>in</strong>al clusters are comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> different ways with the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters,<br />

apart from with s<strong>in</strong>gle consonants. There are four groups:<br />

1. <strong>in</strong>itial cluster + no f<strong>in</strong>al cluster (e.g. skval 11)<br />

2. no <strong>in</strong>itial cluster + f<strong>in</strong>al cluster (e.g. babbla)<br />

3 a. <strong>in</strong>itial cluster + f<strong>in</strong>al cluster with the same mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(e.g. blaska, wetness+wetness)<br />

3 b. <strong>in</strong>itial cluster + f<strong>in</strong>al cluster with a different mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(e.g. drumla, pejorative + quick or strong movement)<br />

The analysis of group 1 is based on the analysis of chapter 4 (of Svensk<br />

Ordbok). The follow<strong>in</strong>g analysis of groups 2 and 3 is based on the f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters of Svensk Baklängesordbok. As the analysis <strong>in</strong> 5.5. shows, the<br />

most common semantic features are the same for both <strong>in</strong>itial clusters and<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al clusters.<br />

5.6.1 Initial cluster + no f<strong>in</strong>al cluster<br />

In order to study sound symbolism of the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters isolated from<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al clusters (cf. discussion <strong>in</strong> 4.1), the roots with a sound symbolic<br />

<strong>in</strong>itial cluster n o t end<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a f<strong>in</strong>al cluster were studied more closely.<br />

Table 5.22 shows the frequencies of semantic features when roots end<strong>in</strong>g<br />

with clusters or gem<strong>in</strong>ates are not taken <strong>in</strong>to account. The table shows the<br />

semantic features, the frequencies and root examples, and the clusters are<br />

ranked <strong>in</strong> frequency order. The words are extracted from Appendix 1.<br />

The roots of Table 5.22 are a subset of the roots <strong>in</strong> Appendix 1. Table<br />

5.22 thus only shows the roots which do not end with a f<strong>in</strong>al cluster or<br />

gem<strong>in</strong>ate, whereas Appendix 1 conta<strong>in</strong>s roots both with and without f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters or gem<strong>in</strong>ates.<br />

11 It is possible that there is sound symbolism also <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle consonants, <strong>in</strong>itially as well<br />

as f<strong>in</strong>ally. For example, [S] could have the semantic feature 'separation' as <strong>in</strong> skiva,<br />

skilja, sk<strong>in</strong>na. Also, s<strong>in</strong>gular sounds with special characteristics could attract sound<br />

symbolic mean<strong>in</strong>gs, e.g. [r] is a sound which is different from other sounds and [C] has<br />

low frequency <strong>in</strong> Swedish. This question is not <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>in</strong> this thesis, but is left for<br />

further research.<br />

175


Table 5.22 Comb<strong>in</strong>ations of <strong>in</strong>itial cluster + no f<strong>in</strong>al cluster. The<br />

recurr<strong>in</strong>g semantic features of <strong>in</strong>itial clusters <strong>in</strong> frequency order. Listed<br />

to the right are the roots.<br />

features freq. the words<br />

pejorative 45 bliga, drasut, dröse, fjäsa,<br />

fl<strong>in</strong>a, glop, glupa, glåpord,<br />

gnat, gneta, gnidare, gräma,<br />

gräslig, klåpa, knal, knasig,<br />

kneg, kneken, knöl, knös,<br />

krake, kreta, kruserlig,<br />

kräk, mjäkig, pjåkig, pryl,<br />

skral, slas, slasa, slok, slyna,<br />

slö, slösa, sniken, snyta,<br />

snål, snöd, snöpa, spol<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

strul, tradig, trasa, vräkig,<br />

vrövel<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 36 skåra, slana, släde, snabel,<br />

sno, snok, snöre, spene,<br />

speta, spik, spila, spira,<br />

spole, spö, spjut, spjäla<br />

spröt, stake, stav, stig,<br />

stilett, stör, streamer,<br />

strigel, stril, stripa, strut,<br />

strå, stråk, stråle, stråt,<br />

truta, tryne, tråd, tråg, trål<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 23 gnat, gnola, gny, gnöla,<br />

gråta, gräla, klaga, knota,<br />

kverulans, prat, pruta,<br />

skri, skrodera, skryta,<br />

skrål, skräna, skröna,<br />

smäda, snäsa, svada, svära,<br />

vrål, vrövel<br />

sound 21 braka, brus, bräka, bröl,<br />

fnysa, frasa, fräsa, gnod<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

knaka, knot, krakel, kras,<br />

kråka, kvida, kväda, kväka,<br />

skval, snusa, snyta, stön,<br />

stril<br />

176


quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

18 flanera, fluga, flyga, flyta,<br />

flåsa, flöda, frusa, gnida,<br />

gno, skaka, slira, slita,<br />

spruta, stöt, svepa, sväva,<br />

vrida, vräka<br />

wetness 15 blod, blöt, klena, kleta,<br />

kram, kräm, skval, smet,<br />

snor, snuva, spad, spola,<br />

sputum, spruta, stril<br />

th<strong>in</strong> form 13 flaga, flak, flarn, flisa, fnasig,<br />

krakmandel, krokett,<br />

krusta, krustad, spade,<br />

smooth surface<br />

structure<br />

spat, spatel, spån<br />

11 glací, glaciär, glas, glatt,<br />

glida, glissando, gnida, gno,<br />

slipa, slät, spegel<br />

destruction 11 bryta, bråte, bräck,<br />

bränn<strong>in</strong>g, frat, fräta, kvarn,<br />

skrot, skräp, trasa, vrak<br />

light 10 blek, bläs, glacé, glisa,<br />

gloria, glåmig, glöd, prål,<br />

spat, spraka<br />

gaze 10 bliga, glana, glaukom, glo,<br />

glosögd, glosa, plira, snegla,<br />

spana, speja<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive 8 gli, glipa, knåp, krabat,<br />

smula, små, späd, vret<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g 8 knata, krypa, kräla, snava,<br />

strosa, ströva, trava, träda<br />

round form 7 blåsa, knop, knota, knödel,<br />

knöl, kruka, skopa<br />

w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form 7 krok, krokan, krusa, kråma,<br />

kräla, krök, spiral<br />

bad mood 6 gräla, trulig, tråkig, träta,<br />

vrede, vresig<br />

hollow form 5 grav, grop, gryt, gräva,<br />

gröpa<br />

form 4 glipa, glob, plös, pryl<br />

bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g 4 kval, kvav, kväva, svida<br />

soft consistency 4 mjuk, plym, plymå, plysa<br />

f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong> 4 mjäla, mjöl, stöv, strö<br />

rough surface structure 4 fryna, krås, skråma, sträv<br />

separation 4 spagat, split, spreta, sprida<br />

fall<strong>in</strong>g 3 dråsa, drälla, drösa<br />

putt<strong>in</strong>g together 3 knipa, knut, knyta<br />

177


hardness 3 skal, skare, skava<br />

slackness 3 slak, slana, sloka<br />

quickness 3 snar, sno, snudig<br />

stiffness 3 styv, strak, stram<br />

slang 2 plit, snut<br />

beat 2 slag, slå<br />

mental feel<strong>in</strong>g 2 snopen, tråna<br />

jocular 2 spe, spjuver<br />

f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong> 2 stöv, strö<br />

In order to make a general comparison with the earlier results of chapter<br />

4 concern<strong>in</strong>g the semantic features found <strong>in</strong> the analysis of <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

consonant clusters, part of the results of diagram 4.5 are presented as a<br />

table below (table 5.24).<br />

The 11 most frequent semantic features for the comb<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>in</strong>itial cluster<br />

+ no f<strong>in</strong>al cluster (and no gem<strong>in</strong>ate consonant) are shown <strong>in</strong> table 5.23.<br />

This rank<strong>in</strong>g can be compared with diagram 4.5, which shows the<br />

frequencies of all the semantic features, both with and without f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters.<br />

Table 5.23 The 11 most frequent<br />

semantic features for the comb<strong>in</strong>ation<br />

<strong>in</strong>itial cluster + no f<strong>in</strong>al cluster.<br />

178<br />

Table 5.24 The 11 most frequent<br />

semantic features of diagram 4.5<br />

show<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>itial clusters irrespective of<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al cluster.<br />

pejorative 45 pejorative 163<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 36 sound 107<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 23 long th<strong>in</strong> form 97<br />

sound 21 quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

67<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

18 wetness 63<br />

wetness 15 talk<strong>in</strong>g 55<br />

th<strong>in</strong> form 13 light 32<br />

smooth surface<br />

11 dim<strong>in</strong>utive 31<br />

structure<br />

destruction 11 round form 23<br />

light 10 way of walk<strong>in</strong>g 22<br />

gaze 10 destruction 21


Table 5.23 shows the 11 most frequent semantic features of table 5.22<br />

(<strong>in</strong>itial clusters + no f<strong>in</strong>al clusters or gem<strong>in</strong>ates), whereas table 5.24<br />

shows the 11 most frequent features of diagram 4.5 (or Appendix 1),<br />

which conta<strong>in</strong>s roots with <strong>in</strong>itial clusters either with or without f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters or gem<strong>in</strong>ates.<br />

Naturally, there are fewer words <strong>in</strong> the table show<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>itial cluster + no<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al cluster. In both tables, i.e. for both <strong>in</strong>itial cluster + no f<strong>in</strong>al cluster<br />

and <strong>in</strong>itial cluster + f<strong>in</strong>al cluster, 'pejorative' is the most frequent feature.<br />

The features 'long th<strong>in</strong> form', 'sound', 'quick or strong movement',<br />

'wetness' and 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' are among the six most frequent features <strong>in</strong> both<br />

tables. In other words the same six features are among the six most<br />

frequent features <strong>in</strong> both cases, only <strong>in</strong> partially different order. This<br />

means that the semantic features are the same, and also approximately as<br />

frequent, irrespective of whether one analyzes roots with only <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

consonant clusters or roots with both <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters.<br />

This result strengthens one outcome of the analysis of chapter 4: the<br />

analysis of semantic features of <strong>in</strong>itial clusters actually showed the<br />

semantic features of the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters and not of the f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters.<br />

5.6.2 No <strong>in</strong>itial cluster + f<strong>in</strong>al cluster<br />

Table 5.25 shows the comb<strong>in</strong>ations of no <strong>in</strong>itial cluster + f<strong>in</strong>al cluster, <strong>in</strong><br />

frequency order.<br />

179


Table 5.25 Comb<strong>in</strong>ations of no <strong>in</strong>itial cluster + f<strong>in</strong>al cluster. The<br />

recurr<strong>in</strong>g semantic features of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters <strong>in</strong> frequency order. Listed to<br />

the right are the roots.<br />

semantic feature freq. rank the words<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

<strong>in</strong> 5.22<br />

51 5 rutscha, puffa, dangla,<br />

rangla, d<strong>in</strong>gla, r<strong>in</strong>gla,<br />

v<strong>in</strong>gla, famla, vimla,<br />

fumla, rumla, tumla,<br />

haspla, dimpa, gumpa,<br />

pumpa, rappa, tappa,<br />

kippa, tippa, vippa, hoppa,<br />

moppa, poppa, guppa,<br />

dallra, pillra, tillra,<br />

myllra, darra, irra,<br />

dirra, virra, burra,<br />

huttra, hafsa, sjafsa,<br />

nafsa, rafsa, tafsa, rufsa,<br />

tufsa, räfsa, mumsa,<br />

palta, bylta, hasta, kasta,<br />

rista, hysta, nysta<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 18 3 babbla, rabbla, gaffla,<br />

mumla, tassla, tissla,<br />

rossla, jollra, morra,<br />

hurra, murra, tjattra,<br />

muttra, jamsa, gasta,<br />

hosta, pusta, tjafsa<br />

sound 14 4 ratscha, p<strong>in</strong>gla, humla,<br />

sörpla, porla, sorla, rassla,<br />

vissla, bullra, mullra,<br />

kurra, surra, kuttra,<br />

puttra<br />

slang 12 - haffa, fiffa, piffa, roffa,<br />

buffa, luffa, ruffa, tuffa,<br />

töffa, dumpa, rumpa,<br />

sumpa<br />

pejorative 9 1 taffla, fiffla, ruffla, lolla,<br />

hafsa, tjafsa, lafsa, jamsa,<br />

dalta<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g 7 13 lulla, jumpa, lafsa, lufsa,<br />

halta, rulta, tulta<br />

round form 3 14 rolla, bulla, rulla<br />

short-wide form 2 - fimpa, limpa<br />

light 2 10 t<strong>in</strong>dra, skimra<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 2 2 ramsa, remsa<br />

180


The rank<strong>in</strong>g of the most frequent words can be compared with the<br />

rank<strong>in</strong>g of table 5.22. This comparison shows that 'pejorative' is not as<br />

frequently coded <strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al clusters as <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters.<br />

On the other hand, 'slang' (ma<strong>in</strong>ly coded <strong>in</strong> -ffa), 'walk<strong>in</strong>g' and 'quick or<br />

strong movement' are more frequent <strong>in</strong> words with only f<strong>in</strong>al clusters,<br />

compared with words with only <strong>in</strong>itial clusters. This might be due to the<br />

verbal nature of the clusters end<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a vowel. In the same way as table<br />

5.23 is compared with table 5.24, table 5.25 can be compared with table<br />

5.26, which conta<strong>in</strong>s all the roots (and not just the ones without <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

clusters) taken from table 5.2.<br />

Table 5.26 All roots from table 5.2, ordered by semantic features, <strong>in</strong><br />

frequency order.<br />

semantic feature freq. the words<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement<br />

85 rutscha, knuffa, puffa, dangla, rangla, skrangla,<br />

d<strong>in</strong>gla, r<strong>in</strong>gla, v<strong>in</strong>gla, famla, vimla, tromla,<br />

fumla, rumla, drumla, tumla, stöppla, haspla,<br />

rumla, dimpa, skvimpa, gumpa, skumpa, pumpa,<br />

klappa, snappa, rappa, tappa, greppa, steppa,<br />

kippa, skippa, tippa, vippa, hoppa, moppa,<br />

noppa, snoppa, poppa, droppa, proppa, guppa,<br />

knäppa, snäppa, fladdra, bläddra, dallra, pillra,<br />

tillra, myllra, flimra, darra, irra, dirra, virra,<br />

svirra, burra, plurra, snurra, glittra, splittra,<br />

klottra, huttra, hafsa, sjafsa, nafsa, snafsa, rafsa,<br />

krafsa, tafsa, plufsa, rufsa, tufsa, räfsa, plumsa,<br />

mumsa, palta, skralta, skvalta, bylta, hasta, kasta,<br />

rista, hysta, nysta<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 37 babbla, rabbla, gaffla, traggla, svamla, mumla,<br />

tassla, tissla, rossla, slabbra, bladdra, pladdra,<br />

sladdra, bluddra, sluddra, jollra, pjollra, blarra,<br />

snarra, kvirra, morra, knorra, hurra, murra,<br />

tjattra, fnittra, muttra, tjafsa, jamsa, grumsa,<br />

tramsa, gasta, hosta, pusta, frusta, knysta, krysta<br />

sound 34 klatscha, ratscha, p<strong>in</strong>gla, skramla, humla, sörpla,<br />

porla, sorla, rassla, prassla, gnissla, vissla,<br />

kvillra, skallra, bullra, mullra, bjällra, knarra,<br />

klirra, knirra, skorra, kurra, surra, smattra,<br />

knattra, snattra, skvattra, knittra, kvittra, kuttra,<br />

puttra, gläfsa, klafsa, slafsa<br />

181


slang 19 haffa, blaffa, klaffa, fiffa, sniffa, piffa, skoffa,<br />

roffa, buffa, skuffa, luffa, fluffa, ruffa, gruffa,<br />

tuffa, töffa, dumpa, rumpa, sumpa<br />

pejorative 19 taffla, fiffla, ruffla, fjolla, lolla, stolla, slampa,<br />

fjompa, klattra, plottra, lafsa, glufsa, flamsa,<br />

dalta, slafsa, hafsa, tjafsa, tramsa, jamsa<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g 15 lulla, drulla, klampa, trampa, stampa, jumpa,<br />

trippa, lufsa, halta, rulta, tulta, stulta, stylta,<br />

klafsa, lafsa<br />

wetness 9 blaska, plaska, slaska, smaska, snaska, vaska,<br />

glopp, klafsa, skvalta<br />

round form 7 rolla, skrolla, bulla, krulla, stropp, rulla, knollra<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 5 strimla, slamsa, ramsa, remsa, slimsa<br />

light 5 gl<strong>in</strong>dra, t<strong>in</strong>dra, skimra, glimra, flimra<br />

short-wide form 4 fimpa, limpa, klimpa, klumpa<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive 2 stumpa, loppa<br />

fall<strong>in</strong>g 1 ramla<br />

bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g 1 pirra<br />

gaze 1 stirra<br />

rough surface structure 1 knottra<br />

beat 1 bulta<br />

destruction 1 brista<br />

slackness 1 slimsa<br />

movement 1 knaggla<br />

w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form 1 kr<strong>in</strong>gla<br />

form 1 plumpa<br />

The analysis shows that the six most frequent features are the same and<br />

that they are so <strong>in</strong> exactly the same order. This result strengthens the<br />

analysis of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters <strong>in</strong> table 5.25.<br />

5.6.3 Initial cluster + f<strong>in</strong>al cluster<br />

The tables 5.27 and 5.28 show comb<strong>in</strong>ations of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters and <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

clusters. The examples are extracted from table 5.2. Table 5.27 shows<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ations where the semantic features are the same <strong>in</strong>itially and<br />

f<strong>in</strong>ally, and table 5.28 shows comb<strong>in</strong>ations where the features are<br />

different <strong>in</strong>itially and f<strong>in</strong>ally. The follow<strong>in</strong>g conclusions can be drawn:<br />

1. Fifty words have the same semantic feature <strong>in</strong>itially and f<strong>in</strong>ally,<br />

whereas 34 words have different features. However, of these 34 words, 8<br />

have <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al features that are similar to each other: 'sound' +<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g' and 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form' + 'round form'. It is more common, then,<br />

182


for semantic features to be the same, <strong>in</strong>itially and f<strong>in</strong>ally, than to be<br />

different.<br />

2. The semantic features of a root can be the same <strong>in</strong>itially and f<strong>in</strong>ally for<br />

each root with double features 12. These are the follow<strong>in</strong>g, listed <strong>in</strong> order<br />

of frequency. The mean<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>in</strong>itial clusters come from the analysis <strong>in</strong><br />

Appendix 1, and the mean<strong>in</strong>gs of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters come from the analysis <strong>in</strong><br />

table 5.2.<br />

Table 5.27 Semantic features (with absolute frequencies of the roots) that<br />

are the same <strong>in</strong>itially and f<strong>in</strong>ally <strong>in</strong> one and the same root.<br />

doubl<strong>in</strong>g<br />

freq. the words represent<strong>in</strong>g<br />

features<br />

root morphemes<br />

sound 18 klatscha, skramla,<br />

prassla, gnissla, skallra,<br />

kvillra, bjällra, knarra,<br />

klirra, knirra, skorra,<br />

smattra, knattra, snattra,<br />

knittra, kvittra, klafsa, gläfsa<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g 9 svamla, slabbra, bladdra,<br />

sladdra, bluddra, sluddra,<br />

blarra, tramsa, grumsa<br />

pejorative 9 fjolla, stolla, slampa, fjompa,<br />

plottra, slafsa, glufsa, flamsa,<br />

tramsa<br />

wetness 4 blaska, plaska, slaska, snaska<br />

quick or strong 3 tromla, skippa, fladdra<br />

movement<br />

light 3 gl<strong>in</strong>dra, flimra, glimra<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g 2 trampa, stulta<br />

short-wide form 2 klimpa, klumpa<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 1 slamsa<br />

Table 5.28 shows which different semantic features are comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong>itially<br />

and f<strong>in</strong>ally. A word represent<strong>in</strong>g a root morpheme may occur <strong>in</strong> two<br />

places if it conta<strong>in</strong>s two features <strong>in</strong>itially or two features f<strong>in</strong>ally, e.g.<br />

plumsa: pl- means both 'sound' and 'wetness' <strong>in</strong> this root morpheme.<br />

12 There are 50 words but 51 features, as can be seen <strong>in</strong> the table, because one word<br />

(tramsa) conta<strong>in</strong>s two features <strong>in</strong>itially and two features f<strong>in</strong>ally.<br />

183


Table 5.28 Semantic features that are different <strong>in</strong>itially and f<strong>in</strong>ally <strong>in</strong> one and<br />

the same root.<br />

<strong>in</strong>itial:<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al:<br />

semantic<br />

feature of<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

cluster:<br />

slang quick or<br />

strong<br />

movement<br />

semantic<br />

feature of<br />

<strong>in</strong>itial<br />

cluster:<br />

sound klappa<br />

steppa<br />

knäppa<br />

snäppa<br />

krafsa<br />

plumsa<br />

pejorative blaffa drumla<br />

klottra<br />

plufsa<br />

beat knuffa<br />

bad mood gruffa<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g gruffa<br />

destruction skrangla<br />

splittra<br />

skralta<br />

184<br />

wetness walk<strong>in</strong>g round<br />

form<br />

smaska klampa<br />

stampa<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

snarra<br />

kvirra<br />

knorra<br />

fnittra<br />

frusta<br />

knysta<br />

krysta<br />

drulla pjollra<br />

w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g<br />

form<br />

snurra krulla<br />

iterative stampa<br />

quickness snappa<br />

fall<strong>in</strong>g droppa<br />

wetness droppa<br />

plurra<br />

plumsa<br />

skvalta<br />

light glittra<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive splittra<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g conclusions can be drawn from table 5.28.<br />

1. The semantic feature which is most often comb<strong>in</strong>ed with other features<br />

is f<strong>in</strong>al 'quick or strong movement' – but this could partly be an effect of<br />

the choice to study the semif<strong>in</strong>al "a- end<strong>in</strong>gs".<br />

2. The most common feature comb<strong>in</strong>ations are:


'sound' + 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' (7 words)<br />

'sound' + 'quick or strong movement' (6 words)<br />

'wetness' + 'quick or strong movement' (4 words)<br />

In the comb<strong>in</strong>ation 'sound' + 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' the two features are closely related<br />

and they strengthen each other.<br />

3. '<strong>Sound</strong>' is the most common <strong>in</strong>itial feature <strong>in</strong> these comb<strong>in</strong>ations.<br />

4. The otherwise common semantic features 'sound' and 'pejorative' do not<br />

have f<strong>in</strong>al position <strong>in</strong> these comb<strong>in</strong>ations. 'Talk<strong>in</strong>g' and 'walk<strong>in</strong>g' are much<br />

more common <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al position than <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial.<br />

5.6.4 Summary of comb<strong>in</strong>ations<br />

The general results of the study of comb<strong>in</strong>ations of <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters are the follow<strong>in</strong>g: Sometimes the <strong>in</strong>itial cluster carries the sound<br />

symbolic mean<strong>in</strong>g because the word ends <strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle consonant (Group 1).<br />

The most common semantic feature of Group 1 is 'pejorative', which is<br />

also the case for all the sound symbolic roots <strong>in</strong> Appendix 1. The same six<br />

features are among the six most frequent ones <strong>in</strong> both groups and this<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicates that the analysis of chapter 4 showed what it was <strong>in</strong>tended to<br />

show: the mean<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>itial clusters.<br />

When there are both <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters <strong>in</strong> a root, these<br />

can comb<strong>in</strong>e by hav<strong>in</strong>g the same mean<strong>in</strong>g (Group 3a), e.g. bl- and -aska<br />

('wetness') – cf. table 5.27. Alternatively, different mean<strong>in</strong>gs can comb<strong>in</strong>e<br />

<strong>in</strong> a word (Group 3b), e.g. glittra – cf. table 5.28. The most common<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ations are those <strong>in</strong> which the mean<strong>in</strong>g is the same <strong>in</strong>itially and<br />

f<strong>in</strong>ally. The most common mean<strong>in</strong>g for group 3a is 'sound' + 'sound',<br />

followed by 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' + 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' and 'pejorative' + 'pejorative'. The most<br />

common mean<strong>in</strong>g comb<strong>in</strong>ations for Group 3b is 'sound' + 'talk<strong>in</strong>g',<br />

followed by 'sound' + 'quick or strong movement'.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, sometimes only the f<strong>in</strong>al cluster (or gem<strong>in</strong>ate) carries the sound<br />

symbolic mean<strong>in</strong>g, because the word beg<strong>in</strong>s with a s<strong>in</strong>gle consonant<br />

(Group 2). The most common mean<strong>in</strong>g is 'quick or strong movement'<br />

followed by 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' and 'sound'.<br />

185


5.7 Summary and discussion of <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters, and vowels<br />

This and the preced<strong>in</strong>g chapter have shown that certa<strong>in</strong> semantic features<br />

are connected with certa<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters and with<br />

vowels – <strong>in</strong> different constellations and to different extents. A study of<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ations of <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al clusters shows that the sound symbolic<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g of a root morpheme can also be dependent on the comb<strong>in</strong>ations<br />

of phonemes of the whole word, e.g. sl- 'wetness' + a + -sk(a) 'wetness'<br />

(=slaska (splatter)). (However, both 'slisk' and 'slusk' have the feature<br />

'pejorative' and not 'wetness' because of the semantic profile of sl-, cf.<br />

4.4.1).<br />

Partly the same semantic features are used <strong>in</strong> both <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters, while the vowels sometimes add a dimension of 'size' (smalllittle,<br />

high pitch-low pitch). The 'light/gaze' words, where the vowel is<br />

almost always a front vowel, could be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the 'size' group, s<strong>in</strong>ce<br />

'light' can be seen as synaesthetically connected with high pitch. The<br />

vowels, when they are used sound symbolically, thus often seem to have<br />

other semantic features than the consonant clusters do, and they add a<br />

special mean<strong>in</strong>g to roots – which otherwise get their sound symbolic<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g from <strong>in</strong>itial and/or f<strong>in</strong>al clusters – e.g. by show<strong>in</strong>g if a sound is<br />

high pitched or low pitched, or if a movement is quick or slow.<br />

For typically sound-symbolic (where all or almost all root morphemes are<br />

sound symbolic) or high frequent <strong>in</strong>itial clusters, vowels, and f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters, the mean<strong>in</strong>g of a word, especially of a neologism, should be<br />

predictable.<br />

A nonsense expression like pjaffla : pj- ('pejorative') + a + -fla<br />

('pejorative') ought to be <strong>in</strong>terpreted as a pejorative word. A neologism<br />

like brullra : br- ('sound') + P ('low pitch') + -lra ('sound' or 'quick or<br />

strong movement') ought to be <strong>in</strong>terpreted as imitat<strong>in</strong>g sound (a low<br />

pitched one) while a nonsense expression like flillra: fl- ('quick or strong<br />

movement') + i ('smallness', 'quickness' or 'high pitch') + -lra ('sound'<br />

186


or 'quick or strong movement') ought to be <strong>in</strong>terpreted as someth<strong>in</strong>g<br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g to do with quick movement 13.<br />

Look<strong>in</strong>g at real words, there can be a comb<strong>in</strong>ation effect like <strong>in</strong> skrangla<br />

(skranglig-rickety) "which has an unsteady construction, or has become<br />

unsteady because of long usage". This def<strong>in</strong>ition goes well with the<br />

phonesthemes skr- 'destruction' and -ngla 'quick or strong movement'.<br />

Another example is klatscha (crack): kl- 'sound' and -tscha [tSa] 'sound'.<br />

A more unconventional word klitscha (which is not <strong>in</strong> the dictionary)<br />

would stand for a more high pitched sound 14. For real words there can<br />

always be an effect of conventionalization which makes them less<br />

predictable.<br />

Conventionalization can always take over, at least partly, either <strong>in</strong> the<br />

arbitrary direction or <strong>in</strong> choos<strong>in</strong>g phonesthemic mean<strong>in</strong>gs. The<br />

phonestheme level is an underly<strong>in</strong>g level which can be activated by<br />

different types of context.<br />

The next chapter will present contrastive studies of some semantic fields<br />

of sound symbolism. In chapter 7 f<strong>in</strong>al clusters, as well as <strong>in</strong>itial clusters,<br />

will be discussed aga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> connection with neologisms.<br />

13 In the experiments with nonsense words <strong>in</strong> chapter 6 only the effect of the <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

clusters were tested. In further research, however, the <strong>in</strong>tention is to also test the<br />

comb<strong>in</strong>ed effects.<br />

14Two roommates were asked about the mean<strong>in</strong>g of this expression. They answered that<br />

it was not a real word, and then one of them gave a possible mean<strong>in</strong>g 'the sound of a<br />

bicycle spoke that breaks'. The other person said it meant 'the sound that comes when<br />

you drive on a th<strong>in</strong> layer of ice'. Both these def<strong>in</strong>itions have to do with a high pitched<br />

sound.<br />

187


6 Some contrastive studies <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism<br />

6.1 Introduction<br />

The phenomena of onomatopoeia and other sound symbolism have been<br />

described <strong>in</strong> part for different languages. Some earlier contrastive<br />

f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are described <strong>in</strong> the overview of 1.10.4.<br />

The aim of this chapter is to study some of the semantic features and<br />

consonant clusters discussed above <strong>in</strong> order to analyze what differences<br />

and similarities there are between some languages.<br />

In the first section I will present a contrastive lexical study made on some<br />

selected mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> a Thesaurus. The languages compared are Swedish<br />

and English. I will then present a contrastive study of some expressive<br />

<strong>in</strong>terjections, commands and greet<strong>in</strong>gs and a study of words for animal<br />

sounds. Furthermore I will present a contrastive experiment of<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation of Swedish sound symbolic words.<br />

6.2 The Thesaurus study<br />

6.2.1 Method<br />

In this experiment <strong>in</strong>formants chose the best words for 'stupidity',<br />

'roughness' and 'smoothness' <strong>in</strong> English and Swedish out of a Thesaurus.<br />

From the Swedish Thesaurus (Br<strong>in</strong>g, 1930) a couple of mean<strong>in</strong>gs were<br />

chosen, namely: 'dumhuvud' (501), 'oförstånd' (499), and 'glatthet' (255),<br />

'skrovlighet' (256). In the English Roget´s Thesaurus (1977) the<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gs were chosen: 'fool' (501), 'imbecility' (499),<br />

'<strong>in</strong>sanity' (503), 'smoothness' (255), and 'roughness' (256)<br />

Lists of all words under these head<strong>in</strong>gs were then <strong>in</strong>spected by native<br />

<strong>in</strong>formants, three for the Swedish material and three for the English<br />

material. The <strong>in</strong>formants were given the <strong>in</strong>struction "to mark words, the<br />

expressive forms of which are felt to be especially adequate for their<br />

content". 1<br />

1 S<strong>in</strong>ce I am not an active speaker of English and s<strong>in</strong>ce I have deepened my <strong>in</strong>tuitions of<br />

Swedish phonesthemes from the analysis of chapter 4, I used subjects for this task.<br />

189


6.2.2 Results<br />

6.2.2.1 Words for 'stupidity' <strong>in</strong> English<br />

S<strong>in</strong>ce English does not have the clusters typically pejorative for Swedish:<br />

fj-, fn-, pj-, there could be no similarities between Swedish and English<br />

here. (Cf. below and diagram 4.6.)<br />

There were 4 words where all three English subjects agreed:<br />

n<strong>in</strong>compoop, blockhead, dunce and dull.<br />

Two of the three subjects agreed on 9 words: simpleton, dolt, booby, oaf,<br />

clod, silly, muddleheaded, addleheaded, fool.<br />

Isolated contributions from either of the subjects were: tomfool, ass,<br />

noodle, gawk, mooncalf, dotard, driveler, old fogy, Simple Simon, goose,<br />

lout, idiot, dotage, idiocy, fatuity, gidd<strong>in</strong>ess, drivel, dote, stultify, bov<strong>in</strong>e,<br />

feeblem<strong>in</strong>ded, obtuse, stolid, fatuous, drivel<strong>in</strong>g, bewildered, maudl<strong>in</strong>,<br />

stupidity, foolishness, rashness, bra<strong>in</strong>less, childlike, vacant.<br />

Many of these words are not what we would normally th<strong>in</strong>k of as sound<br />

symbolic. Nevertheless there seems to be a slight tendency for the<br />

preferred ones to conta<strong>in</strong> a long [u] or an [a] and perhaps [d] <strong>in</strong>itially or<br />

medially. The value of the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters is difficult to judge<br />

from this material.<br />

6.2.2.2 Words for 'stupidity' <strong>in</strong> Swedish<br />

All three Swedish subjects agreed on the follow<strong>in</strong>g words: pjosker 2 ,<br />

pjasker, fjoller, fnasker, fjanter, flep, flepig, pjåk, fjolleri, fjant, pjoller,<br />

pjosk, fjollig, fjoskig, fjantig, pjoskig, pjollrig, pjåkig, pjaskig.<br />

The frequencies of the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters are the follow<strong>in</strong>g:<br />

pj- : 9 words<br />

fj- : 7 words<br />

fl- : 2 words<br />

fn-: 1 word<br />

2 The Swedish words listed under the different categories of the Thesaurus are not<br />

translated s<strong>in</strong>ce the Thesaurus head<strong>in</strong>g, e.g. "stupidity" shows the semantic category.<br />

What is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g is not the exact mean<strong>in</strong>g of all these words, but which sounds are the<br />

most frequent for each Thesaurus head<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

190


Except for fl- the result above mirrors the results of the percentally most<br />

frequent clusters well (diagram 4.6).<br />

Two of the subjects agreed on the follow<strong>in</strong>g words:<br />

mähä, tafser, tossa, tafsig, fjoskighet, fjanta, fjollas, pjollra, pjoska, pjåka,<br />

pjask.<br />

The most common clusters here are:<br />

pj-: 4 words<br />

fj-: 3 words<br />

There is a clear preference for the fj- and pj- clusters among the Swedish<br />

<strong>in</strong>formants.<br />

Isolated contributions were: våp, tåp, jöns, fån, sjasker, drummel, tos<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

stolle, drönig, tåpig, -snut, pund-, mes, schajas, flack, flärdfull, larvig,<br />

korkad, tafs, sjask, jolt, fåna, jolta, drumlig, slapp, slö, våpig, fånig,<br />

sjaskig, taskig, tölpig, show<strong>in</strong>g an additional preference for the vowels<br />

[o:], [O] and [a]. The consonant cluster which is the most common is dr-<br />

(3 roots). The cluster dr- is pejorative accord<strong>in</strong>g to the analysis <strong>in</strong><br />

chapter 4.<br />

6.2.2.3 Words for 'surface structure' <strong>in</strong> English<br />

For the categories of 'roughness' and 'smoothness' there where two<br />

English subjects who made an assessment.<br />

'Roughness'<br />

Both subjects agreed on the follow<strong>in</strong>g words: crest, ruffle, crumple,<br />

rumple, rugged, jagged, gnarly, scraggly, scraggy, craggy, cragged,<br />

prickly, bristly, bushy.<br />

The clusters cr- and scr- and the consonant r are favored.<br />

Isolated contributions were: asperity, corrugation, shag, cross-gra<strong>in</strong>ed,<br />

hirsute, shaggy, nappy, thatch, whiskers, feather, rough, cr<strong>in</strong>kle. Among<br />

these words there are two <strong>in</strong>stances of cr- and five <strong>in</strong>stances of s<strong>in</strong>gle r.<br />

'Smoothness'<br />

Both subjects agreed on the words sleek and glossy.<br />

191


Isolated contributions were: plane, level, polish, velvety, glassy, gloss,<br />

roller, roll, oily, silken, silky.<br />

The preferred consonant is l.<br />

6.2.2.4 Words for 'surface structure' <strong>in</strong> Swedish<br />

'Roughness'<br />

All three subjects agreed on the follow<strong>in</strong>g words: knotter and knottrig.<br />

Two out of three agreed on the follow<strong>in</strong>g: knotighet, knut, knyla, knotig,<br />

knagglig, klippig, knollrig, krullig, krusig, stripig, skrapa, skrynka,<br />

skrynkla, knottra, skrovlig, skrynkig, skrynklig 3<br />

These words show a preference for r and for the clusters kn- and skrand<br />

kr-. The kr- and skr- words both have the semantic feature 'rough<br />

surface structure' (<strong>in</strong> chapter 4) but kn- words are analyzed as 'round<br />

form'. The cluster with the highest number of root morphemes with the<br />

feature 'rough surface' structure (<strong>in</strong> the lexical analysis) is skr-.<br />

Isolated contributions were: raggighet, knöl, kornighet, rufsighet, burr,<br />

stripa, ludenhet, lurv, tovig, purrighet, ludd, borst, ragg, lugg, rugg,<br />

stubb, test, ull, kvist, ris, tistel, tova, tuva, kvast, visp, buckla, kugge,<br />

krusn<strong>in</strong>g, rysch, frans, dun, plysch, schagg, sträv, fårad, uddig, tandad,<br />

taggig, törnig, risig, buskig, tuvig, lummig, sträv, tofsig, noppig.<br />

These contributions, from s<strong>in</strong>gular subjects, show a preference for the<br />

vowel [P].<br />

'Smoothness'<br />

All three subjects agreed on glätta, glatta, glanska and glansa show<strong>in</strong>g<br />

preference for gl-.<br />

Two out of three agreed on: glättn<strong>in</strong>g, glans, slipprighet, slirn<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

glidn<strong>in</strong>g, blanka, glasera, glida, slira, glatt, glansig, glansk and slipprig.<br />

3 All words excerpted by the subjects were counted, and as a consequence the list also<br />

conta<strong>in</strong>s words that represent the same root.<br />

192


The recurr<strong>in</strong>g clusters are gl-, sl-, bl-, which all conta<strong>in</strong> the liquid l.<br />

These are, except for sl-, related to 'light', but as was shown <strong>in</strong> 4.6 there<br />

is an <strong>in</strong>dexical relation between 'light', 'reflect<strong>in</strong>g surface' and 'movement<br />

on reflect<strong>in</strong>g surface'. Slipprig and slira were analyzed as 'quick or<br />

strong' movement <strong>in</strong> the lexical analysis, (while slipa, slät, slätt were<br />

analyzed as 'smooth surface structure'). Sl- is also to a high degree<br />

connected with 'wetness'.<br />

Isolated contributions were: glänsa, glänsande, blank, glas, smörjn<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

smörja, smärgel, kristall, siden, silke, sammet, lackera, polera, slipa,<br />

kana, stryka, mangla, valsa, oljig, smidig, mjäll.<br />

Also <strong>in</strong> these words the l is very frequent, however not part of an <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

cluster.<br />

6.2.3 Conclusions of the Thesaurus study<br />

There was greater agreement between Swedish and English among the<br />

preferred sounds for 'surface structure', both 'roughness' (r, skr) and<br />

'smoothness' (sl, gl) than for 'pejorative'. An explanation to this might<br />

be that 'surface structure' is closer to a potentially common phenomenon,<br />

namely sound imitation, s<strong>in</strong>ce strok<strong>in</strong>g different surfaces gives different<br />

sound effects.<br />

Sometimes the isolated contributions perhaps mirror the fact that the task<br />

at hand for the test subjects can be difficult to keep <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d or that the<br />

language feel<strong>in</strong>g sometimes runs amock among words with similar<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Suddenly all words can feel motivated. Nevertheless, this<br />

method is a possible way of compar<strong>in</strong>g different languages, through the<br />

<strong>in</strong>tuitions of <strong>in</strong>formants. A problem with us<strong>in</strong>g the Thesaurus is that the<br />

word corpus listed is very heterogeneous. Examples of this are several<br />

words (however not chosen by the Swedish subjects) under the category<br />

'roughness': ojämnhet (unevenness), knävelborr (big and bushy<br />

moustache), brottyta (fracture), kartnagel (deformed nail), vårta (wart)<br />

and tuppkam (cock's crest).<br />

For cross l<strong>in</strong>guistic comparisons the Thesaurus method used above is an<br />

alternative to free production with<strong>in</strong> different semantic fields, (for those<br />

languages that have a Thesaurus). Naturally, native <strong>in</strong>formants have to be<br />

used.<br />

193


6.3 Some <strong>in</strong>terjections <strong>in</strong> different languages<br />

Expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections is one of the sound symbolic types described <strong>in</strong><br />

2.4. The relation between expression and mean<strong>in</strong>g is ma<strong>in</strong>ly <strong>in</strong>dexical; the<br />

expression is caused by a bodily or mental reaction. It is an <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g<br />

task to analyze how similar the expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections are <strong>in</strong> different<br />

languages (the question of universal traits of the expression). It is also<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to see, for the expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections of Swedish, to what<br />

extent the expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections are unsystematical, i. e. not relatable to<br />

phonesthemes.<br />

Expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections, commands and greet<strong>in</strong>gs (cf. Ideforss, 1928,<br />

categorization) are exemplified mostly with a fragment of the examples <strong>in</strong><br />

Ideforss. These were translated <strong>in</strong>to 8 different languages with the help of<br />

<strong>in</strong>formants. The languages are Icelandic, English, Polish, Hungarian,<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish, Ososo, Malagasi and Slovenian. The results are presented <strong>in</strong><br />

Appendix 2 and grouped accord<strong>in</strong>g to type of <strong>in</strong>terjection. The expressive<br />

<strong>in</strong>terjections, commands, and greet<strong>in</strong>gs are listed <strong>in</strong> table 6.2 and <strong>in</strong><br />

Appendix 2. They are written <strong>in</strong> normal spell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> table 6.2.<br />

In some cases it was difficult to ask subjects to translate word for word.<br />

The semantic contents of the <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>in</strong>terjection <strong>in</strong>stead had to be<br />

described (with<strong>in</strong> the semantic categories of table 6.2), after which the<br />

<strong>in</strong>formant gave the closest correspondence <strong>in</strong> his own language. The way<br />

the semantic contents was described to the <strong>in</strong>formant depended on his or<br />

her competence <strong>in</strong> Swedish or English, e.g. "Give me a word or<br />

expression for when you want to be depreciatory" or "Give me a word or<br />

expression for when you th<strong>in</strong>k somebody or someth<strong>in</strong>g is bad or ugly,<br />

etc".<br />

The semantic features used <strong>in</strong> earlier chapters, which correspond to the<br />

features of these <strong>in</strong>terjections, are shown <strong>in</strong> table 6.1.<br />

194


Table 6.1 The semantic features of <strong>in</strong>terjections and their<br />

correspondences <strong>in</strong> earlier chapters.<br />

Features of<br />

Correspond<strong>in</strong>g features<br />

<strong>in</strong>terjections<br />

of earlier chapters<br />

pejorative pejorative<br />

positive attitude<br />

surprise mental feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

song talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

other bodily or mental bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g or mental<br />

feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

commands talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

greet<strong>in</strong>gs talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

6.3.1 Swedish expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections<br />

In table 6.2, a sample of the Swedish expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections is presented.<br />

Table 6.2 Examples (<strong>in</strong> ord<strong>in</strong>ary spell<strong>in</strong>g, mostly from Ideforss, 1928) of<br />

Swedish expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections, commands and greet<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

EXPRESSIVE<br />

hihi(hi)<br />

mums<br />

pejorative<br />

mm<br />

namnam<br />

bu<br />

iih<br />

usch<br />

grr<br />

hu<br />

surprise<br />

COMMANDS<br />

"t" [|]<br />

oj<br />

to animals<br />

blä<br />

oh<br />

schas<br />

ha<br />

åh<br />

ptroo<br />

håhå(jaja)<br />

ä<br />

"t" [|]<br />

tvi<br />

öh<br />

åhå<br />

"p" [Ö]<br />

ä<br />

song<br />

to persons<br />

äh<br />

lala(la)<br />

jaja<br />

bah<br />

trala(la)<br />

aja(baja)<br />

asch<br />

sch<br />

äsch<br />

other bodily or<br />

vyss<br />

isch<br />

mental feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

lull lull<br />

uh<br />

urrk<br />

aha<br />

aj<br />

pst<br />

bu<br />

fy<br />

brr<br />

tss<br />

positive<br />

hm<br />

åhej(åhå)<br />

GREETINGS<br />

ohoj<br />

tjo(hej)<br />

ah<br />

puh<br />

pust<br />

hoho<br />

oh<br />

ojojoj<br />

åh<br />

vojvoj<br />

haha(ha)<br />

håhå(jaja)<br />

atjo<br />

195


6.3.2 Discussion of Swedish expressive<br />

<strong>in</strong>terjections, commands and greet<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

Some short comments are made here on the phonetic/phonological<br />

structure of the <strong>in</strong>terjections <strong>in</strong> table 6.2 <strong>in</strong> order to relate them to the<br />

analyses <strong>in</strong> chapters 2, 4 and 5. They often consist of only a vowel,<br />

sometimes extra long, or of only a consonant, often extra long. They can<br />

beg<strong>in</strong> with a vowel or a consonant. The more open vowels are preferred,<br />

especially [A] and [a] but also [O]. The most common phonemes are j and<br />

h.<br />

Of the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters, bl-, tv-, ts, tr-, br, gr-, ptr and pst, that occur<br />

among these <strong>in</strong>terjections, bl-, tr- and gr- conform with the analysis <strong>in</strong><br />

chapter 4, i.e. bl- can be 'pejorative', tr- can be 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' and gr- can be<br />

'bad mood'. The clusters ts, ptr and pst are unconventional. The f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters are very few: -rk, -st and -ms. None of these conform to the<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters of chapter 5.<br />

This result is <strong>in</strong> accordance with the analysis presented <strong>in</strong> 2.4, which said<br />

that the sound side of expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections are realized more<br />

unsystematically, i.e. they are not (partly) built up by phonesthemes as<br />

sound symbolic roots are. But, as can be seen, the borders are not<br />

absolute between expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections and sound symbolic<br />

phonesthemes. Three <strong>in</strong>terjections – blä (bl- 'pejorative'), tralala (tr-<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g') and grr (gr- 'bad mood') – conform with the phonesthemic<br />

analysis.<br />

Typical for the <strong>in</strong>terjections are the special sounds or sound comb<strong>in</strong>ations<br />

that occur, e.g. click sounds and non-standard phonotactic comb<strong>in</strong>ations,<br />

e.g. [ptro:], [ts:], [pst] and [hm], and isolated consonants like [S]. CVstructure<br />

and reduplicated CV-structure are also common, as well as the<br />

lengthen<strong>in</strong>g of vowels or consonants. Consonant frames with a shift of<br />

vowel, e.g. asch, äsch, usch, isch also occur. Here it is quite obvious that<br />

the vowel quality imparts different mean<strong>in</strong>gs (cf. 5.4). The same goes for<br />

ah, äh, eh, i(h), ö(h). Prosody is probably important but has not been<br />

studied here. None of the phonological or phonetic characteristics is<br />

clearly connected with a certa<strong>in</strong> semantic feature.<br />

196


To summarize, few of the <strong>in</strong>terjections have (conventional) <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

consonant clusters. Of those 5 conventional clusters that occur, 3 conform<br />

with the analysis of chapter 4: bl- (pejorative), tr- (talk<strong>in</strong>g) and gr- (bad<br />

mood). The f<strong>in</strong>al clusters of <strong>in</strong>terjections are very few, and none of them<br />

conforms with the analysis <strong>in</strong> chapter 5.<br />

6.3.3 Phonological and phonetic similarities and<br />

dissimilarities between <strong>in</strong>terjections of different<br />

languages<br />

One <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g question now is: Are there phonological and phonetic<br />

similarities between the <strong>in</strong>terjections of different languages? Expressive<br />

terms, which mirror bodily and mental states, could be similar <strong>in</strong><br />

different languages, with a larger or smaller language specific<br />

superstructure. (cf. Abel<strong>in</strong> and Allwood, 1984). An analysis of my<br />

material (presented <strong>in</strong> Appendix 2) gives the follow<strong>in</strong>g result:<br />

Each semantic category shows its sound pattern tendencies for the<br />

different languages. These tendencies are enhanced by big differences<br />

between the categories. The pejorative <strong>in</strong>terjections, for example, often<br />

conta<strong>in</strong> an [u:] or an [O], the positive <strong>in</strong>terjections an [i] or an [a], surprise<br />

often an [O] or an [a]. 'Pa<strong>in</strong>' (cf. Swedish aj) has an [a] and a [j] or a<br />

diphtongized open vowel. 'Sneeze' (cf. Swedish atjo 4 ) has a voiceless<br />

fricative or an affricate <strong>in</strong> all the languages be<strong>in</strong>g studied, and the<br />

<strong>in</strong>terjection for go away (to an animal) (cf. Swedish schas) has a voiceless<br />

fricative. 'Good taste' has a nasal <strong>in</strong> these 8 languages, and 'scar<strong>in</strong>g<br />

somebody' has a voiced stop (mostly b) <strong>in</strong> all these languages except for<br />

two.<br />

When count<strong>in</strong>g all <strong>in</strong>stances of vowels and consonants <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terjections<br />

<strong>in</strong> Appendix 2 (exclud<strong>in</strong>g the Swedish ones, s<strong>in</strong>ce they are greater <strong>in</strong><br />

number) the follow<strong>in</strong>g tendencies are found, shown <strong>in</strong> table 6.3. (Which<br />

languages contributed to which sounds can be seen <strong>in</strong> Appendix 2 and<br />

table 6.3). Many of the categories of table 6.3 are more specific than<br />

those <strong>in</strong> table 6.2, depend<strong>in</strong>g on what answers the <strong>in</strong>formants gave, cf.<br />

4 Different ways of conventionaliz<strong>in</strong>g this physical outburst (which of course can vary for<br />

different <strong>in</strong>dividuals and from time to time) is, <strong>in</strong> Swedish, mirrored <strong>in</strong> older spell<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

this word: atschi, atsji, aptschäh, apschohoj, aaah-tschah-katsch katsch, kaa-kah, tschah,<br />

tjas, hlutt, schtschi, tjihitt, tjihihitt, tjitjisit (Ideforss, 1928, p. 25).<br />

197


Appendix 2. The categories 'song' and 'greet<strong>in</strong>gs' were not elicited from<br />

the <strong>in</strong>formants.<br />

Table 6.3 The most common vowels and consonants for different<br />

categories of <strong>in</strong>terjections. The languages are Icelandic, English, Polish,<br />

Hungarian, F<strong>in</strong>nish, Ososo, Malagasi and Slovenian.<br />

vowels consonants<br />

pejorative O (9), u (9), i (5)<br />

O:<br />

Icelandic: 2<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 2<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

Ososo: 2<br />

Slovenian: 1<br />

u:<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

English: 3<br />

Hungarian: 3<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 2<br />

i:<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 2<br />

Slovenian: 2<br />

positive i (6), a (5)<br />

i:<br />

Icelandic: 2<br />

English: 2<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 2<br />

a:<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

English: 2<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 1<br />

Ososo: 1<br />

198<br />

j (10), f (6), h (6)<br />

j:<br />

Icelandic: 3<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 1<br />

Hungarian: 4<br />

Ososo: 1<br />

f:<br />

English: 2<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

Ososo: 1<br />

Slovenian: 2<br />

h:<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 5<br />

Ososo: 1<br />

j (3), p (3)<br />

j:<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

English: 1<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 1<br />

p:<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

English: 1<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 1


surprise O (7), a (4)<br />

O:<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 1<br />

Ososo: 1<br />

Slovenian: 4<br />

a:<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

English: 1<br />

Ososo: 1<br />

Malagasi: 1<br />

pa<strong>in</strong> a (9), u (6)<br />

a:<br />

Icelandic: 2<br />

English: 2<br />

Hungarian: 2<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 1<br />

Malagasi: 1<br />

u:<br />

Icelandic: 2<br />

Hungarian: 2<br />

Ososo: 2<br />

freez<strong>in</strong>g O (2)<br />

O:<br />

Polish: 2<br />

199<br />

j (3), h (3)<br />

j:<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

Ososo: 1<br />

Slovenian: 1<br />

h:<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

Slovenian: 1<br />

j (7)<br />

j:<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

Hungarian: 4<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 1<br />

Malagasi: 1<br />

b (3), r (3)<br />

b:<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 1<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

r:<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 1<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

thoughtfulness - h (5), m (6)<br />

h:<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

English: 2<br />

Polish: 1<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 1<br />

m:<br />

Icelandic: 2<br />

English: 2<br />

Polish: 1<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 1


exhaustion u (4)<br />

u:<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 1<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 2<br />

sudden <strong>in</strong>sight a (10)<br />

a:<br />

English: 4<br />

Hungarian: 2<br />

Ososo: 2<br />

Slovenian: 2<br />

sneeze i (8), a (6)<br />

i:<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 1<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

Ososo: 3<br />

Slovenian: 2<br />

a:<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 1<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

Slovenian: 2<br />

good taste a (6)<br />

a:<br />

Icelandic: 3<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 2<br />

commands to<br />

animals<br />

i (3), u (5)<br />

i:<br />

Icelandic: 2<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

u:<br />

Icelandic: 2<br />

English: 1<br />

Ososo: 1<br />

Malagasi: 1<br />

200<br />

h (4)<br />

h:<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 3<br />

h (7)<br />

h:<br />

English: 4<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

Ososo: 1<br />

Slovenian: 1<br />

t (6), C (5)<br />

t:<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

English: 1<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

Ososo: 1<br />

Slovenian: 2<br />

C :<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

Polish: 1<br />

Ososo: 1<br />

Slovenian: 2<br />

m (11), n (7)<br />

m:<br />

Icelandic: 4<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 2<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 1<br />

Slovenian: 3<br />

n:<br />

Icelandic: 3<br />

Polish: 2<br />

Slovenian: 2<br />

S (7)<br />

S:<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 1<br />

Hungarian: 2<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 1<br />

Malagasi: 1


mild warn<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

children<br />

- s (3)<br />

s:<br />

Icelandic: 3<br />

be quiet - S (4), t (3)<br />

S:<br />

English: 1<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 1<br />

Malagasi: 1<br />

t :<br />

Polish: 1<br />

scar<strong>in</strong>g somebody u (5)<br />

u:<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 2<br />

Hungarian: 2<br />

201<br />

Hungarian: 2<br />

b (6)<br />

b:<br />

Icelandic: 1<br />

English: 1<br />

Polish: 2<br />

Hungarian: 1<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish: 1<br />

The table shows that there are many vowels and consonants that are<br />

similar <strong>in</strong> the languages Icelandic, English, Polish, Hungarian, F<strong>in</strong>nish,<br />

Ososo, Malagasi and Slovenian for the categories 'pejorative', 'pa<strong>in</strong>',<br />

'sneeze', 'good taste', 'be quiet', 'thoughtfulness' respectively. These<br />

vowels and consonants of all the functions of table 6.3 are (approximately)<br />

[i], [a], [O], [u] and [p], [b], [m], [f], [t], [s], [n], [r], [j], [C], [S], [h]. Most of<br />

these consonants are labial or dental. There are no consonants produced<br />

beh<strong>in</strong>d the hard palate, except for [h]. The vowels are few and mostly [i],<br />

[u], and [a].<br />

It also seems that a certa<strong>in</strong> sound (or sound comb<strong>in</strong>ation) is preferred <strong>in</strong> a<br />

certa<strong>in</strong> language and is used for a variety of mean<strong>in</strong>gs. The Polish<br />

<strong>in</strong>formant, for example, prefers the sound comb<strong>in</strong>ations [ux], [Ox] and [O],<br />

while the Hungarian <strong>in</strong>formant prefers [jOj], [jaj], [juj].<br />

There are also different degrees of conventionalization <strong>in</strong> the language,<br />

both depend<strong>in</strong>g on speakers and on the situation (wild and tame forms <strong>in</strong><br />

Rhodes' (1995) term<strong>in</strong>ology). Thus, <strong>in</strong> Swedish there are both expiration


and the <strong>in</strong>terjection uh for 'tiredness', and an imitation of the horse's<br />

neigh or the <strong>in</strong>terjection gnägg. 5<br />

6.4 Imitations of animal calls<br />

A subgroup to the onomatopoeic <strong>in</strong>terjections (cf. 2.4) are those that<br />

imitate animal sounds.<br />

sound imitative<br />

Animal call imitative Imitative of physical and bodily reactions Other<br />

Figure 6.1. Relations between different types of onomatopoeic<br />

<strong>in</strong>terjections.<br />

Many animals have laryngeal and supraglottal organs and cavities similar<br />

to man, but with different resonance properties. Most of them produce<br />

sounds <strong>in</strong> the same way as humans (cf. L<strong>in</strong>dblad, 1992). These sounds<br />

seem to be sufficiently similar to allow for imitation <strong>in</strong> human languages.<br />

6.4.1 Expressions for animal calls <strong>in</strong> Swedish and<br />

other languages<br />

The human expressions that imitate animal calls are easy to categorize<br />

semantically. (I presume that e.g. a Swedish cat sounds the same as a cat<br />

from geographically distant countries.) Interjections imitat<strong>in</strong>g animal calls<br />

have been translated, with the help of <strong>in</strong>formants, <strong>in</strong>to 16 different<br />

languages: Icelandic, English, Polish, Hungarian, F<strong>in</strong>nish, Ososo,<br />

Malagasi, Korean, Japanese, Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, Estonian, Urdu, Persian, Kurdish,<br />

Arabic, and Spanish (see table 6.4).<br />

5There is reason to believe that comic strips have contributed to a conventionalization of<br />

both writ<strong>in</strong>g and speech (via writ<strong>in</strong>g).<br />

202


Table 6.4.a Interjections imitat<strong>in</strong>g 7 animal calls, from 16 different languages.<br />

mouse cat goat/<br />

sheep<br />

Korean tSikTSik jaON<br />

njaON<br />

Japanese tSiju: nija:<br />

nijaN<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>ese tSitSitSi mimio<br />

mijao<br />

dog pig horse cow<br />

mœœ: mUNmUN kulkul hihi:hiN Im:E:<br />

mœhœ: kENkEN<br />

mE:mE: waNwaN bu:bu: hihiN mo:<br />

mimi:<br />

mimie:<br />

w´w´w´ IhIhIh ahahaha Na:<br />

F<strong>in</strong>nish pyppyp miau mœ:œ: hauhau<br />

vuhvuh<br />

rØhrØh ihaha: am:u:<br />

Estonian pi:p miau mØk:<br />

mœ:<br />

auxaux ØhØh ihaha mu:<br />

Urdu tSitSi miaow mE:<br />

bE:<br />

bONbON – - m:<br />

Persian - miau bE:bE: howhow korokoro hihihi:<br />

daNtSo<br />

mE:<br />

Kurdish DikDikDik miau be: wOwwOw – - ma:<br />

me:<br />

bOrabOr<br />

Arabic - miau me:<br />

ba:<br />

habhab – hEehEe bu:<br />

Polish pipi miau me: hauhau – hihi mu:<br />

English skwi:k<br />

i:k<br />

miau ba: bauwau<br />

wOfwOf<br />

japjap<br />

Swedish pi:p pi:p miau bœ:<br />

mœ:<br />

vuv:uv<br />

vufvuf<br />

203<br />

OiNkOiNk neigh mu:<br />

nØf:nØf: gnEg:nEg m¨<br />

Spanish - miau be:be. guau OiNkOiNk - mu:<br />

Icelandic pi:p pi:p miau mœ: vufvuf ¯ njihaha møù<br />

Hungarian - miau bœ:œœ<br />

mœkmœk<br />

vauvau rØf:rØf: - mu:<br />

Ososo - miau bœœ bOubOu imitation - mu:<br />

Table 6.4.b Interjections imitat<strong>in</strong>g another 8 animal calls, from 16 different languag<br />

KoreanJapaneseCh<strong>in</strong>eseF<strong>in</strong>nishEstonian<br />

crow cuckoo owl duck hen rooster frog chicken<br />

ka:kka:k PUkukPuUNkwakkokotœpUkukpuUNkwakkoko ka:ka: kak:o: ho:kho:k ga:ga: kokoko kokekoko - -<br />

kokjo: - -<br />

tSijatSija pukupuku ahaha ka:kak kukuku kIkIkI - -<br />

va:kva:k kuk:u:kuk:u:<br />

huhu: kvak-kvak kO:tkotkot kuk:ukie:ku<br />

kva:k -<br />

- kuku uhu: prœ:k kaka kukeleku: - -<br />

Urdu kajkaj kokokoko - kvakva kONkONkON<br />

kOk´lONkON<br />

- -


PersianKurdishArabicPolishEnglishSwedish<br />

SpanishIcelandic<br />

kr¨k-<br />

--kr¨k<br />

HungarianOsoso<br />

204<br />

- -<br />

VarVar hoho huo: kwak- VodVod kukulikwakkoko<br />

- - ba:bu:kwak- kOkOkokO qOqOqOqO - -<br />

ba:bu:kwak - - hu:hu: waq ququ qoqoqo - -<br />

krakra kuku uhu: kwakwa koko kukuriku - -<br />

kOwkOw kuku: - kwak- kl´kkl´k kOk´kwakdud´ldu:krakskraks<br />

kuku: huhu: kvakkvak kakaka<br />

- - - kwakkwak<br />

- -<br />

kvak:vak: pi:p pi:p<br />

kPkElik¨:<br />

klPkklPk<br />

- kikiriki: - -<br />

- ¨hu: bra:bra: kakaka kPkElikP<br />

- pi:ppi:p<br />

ka:rka:r - - haphap kOtkOt kukuriku - tCiptCip<br />

- - u:u: kuOk klOkklOk kukuru:kuOkOkOrOkO<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g conclusions can be drawn from comparisons between<br />

expressions for animal sounds:<br />

1. There is imitation of animal calls <strong>in</strong> all the languages <strong>in</strong> the study.<br />

2. No animal call imitation is exactly the same <strong>in</strong> all languages.<br />

krukkruk tCiatCia<br />

3. Some animal call imitations are similar <strong>in</strong> the different languages,<br />

while others vary more. For example, the cat's sound is conventionalized<br />

as [miau] <strong>in</strong> all languages except <strong>in</strong> Korean where it is [njaON], Japanese<br />

where it is [nijaN] and Ch<strong>in</strong>ese where it can be [mimio] (but also [mijao]).<br />

The expression for the dog's sound, however, varies greatly between the<br />

languages. One possible explanation for this is that the calls of the<br />

different animals differ <strong>in</strong> complexity so that for the complicated animal<br />

calls, different languages attend to different acoustic characteristics with a<br />

po<strong>in</strong>t of departure <strong>in</strong> the phonology of the language. Another possibility<br />

is that some animals simply have a bigger repertoire of calls.<br />

4. There are mostly similarities between expressions for animal calls on<br />

the level of phonological features. For example, all imitations of the cats<br />

meow <strong>in</strong>clude a nasal, and the imitation of the rooster always conta<strong>in</strong>s a


voiceless plosive, velar or uvular. Sometimes the similarity is <strong>in</strong> the form<br />

of reduplication. The dog says [vuv:uv] or [vuf:vuf] <strong>in</strong> Swedish, the same<br />

<strong>in</strong> Icelandic, and [vau vau] <strong>in</strong> Hungarian, so <strong>in</strong> these languages the <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

consonant is the same. In Ososo the dog says [bOubOu]. The likeness with<br />

the previous examples lies <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant which is voiced and<br />

labial, and that there is an [u] vowel.<br />

5. Prosody is probably important but not analyzed here.<br />

6. With<strong>in</strong> each language here, as well as for the <strong>in</strong>terjections, there are<br />

likenesses which can be supposed to depend on, or reflect, the<br />

phonological structure of the language <strong>in</strong> question. For example, Arabic<br />

has [q] where e.g. Swedish has [k], Swedish has [¨] <strong>in</strong>stead of [u], Korean<br />

and Japanese often have a f<strong>in</strong>al [N].<br />

6.4.2 A test of expressions for animal calls of<br />

different languages<br />

Sixteen of the expressions for animal calls from different languages were<br />

chosen to test how speakers from other languages would <strong>in</strong>terpret them.<br />

Those chosen were the ones where the sound shape of the expression was<br />

the most deviant <strong>in</strong> the corpus <strong>in</strong> tables 6.4.a and 6.4.b, <strong>in</strong> order to make<br />

the test as difficult as possible. The expressions of animal calls are listed<br />

<strong>in</strong> table 6.5.<br />

The subjects <strong>in</strong> this test were 15 people, with the follow<strong>in</strong>g mother<br />

tongues: Swedish (8 subjects), French (2 subjects), English, Hungarian,<br />

Czech, Slovenian, Ososo. All were tested on the same occasion. They<br />

listened to pronunciations of the words for the animal calls and they saw<br />

them transcribed as <strong>in</strong> table 6.5. They were told to guess which animals<br />

had gotten their calls conventionalized <strong>in</strong> this way, and to write down<br />

their answers.<br />

205


Table 6.5 The 16 expressions of animal calls chosen for the <strong>in</strong>terpretation<br />

experiment.<br />

CALL ANIMAL LANGUAGE<br />

1. vau vau dog Hungarian<br />

mπ´ 2. : cow Icelandic<br />

3. njihaha horse Hungarian<br />

4. hap hap duck Hungarian<br />

5. rØf rØf pig Hungarian<br />

6. bra: bra: duck Icelandic<br />

7. kOt kOt hen Hungarian, F<strong>in</strong>nish<br />

8. kOkEkO‘ko: rooster Japanese<br />

9. prœ:k duck Estonian<br />

10. pakUk: pakUk: cuckoo Korean<br />

11. njaON cat Korean<br />

12. bu: cow, pig Arabic, Czech,<br />

Japanese<br />

13. kOk´lONkON rooster Urdu<br />

14. tSi tSi mouse Urdu<br />

15. maN maN dog Korean<br />

16. hab: hab: dog Arabic<br />

6.4.3 Results from a test of expressions of animal<br />

calls <strong>in</strong> different languages<br />

The results were as listed <strong>in</strong> table 6.6. Sometimes a subject had written<br />

down more than one answer. All answers have been counted except for<br />

when subjects gave a judgement on a word <strong>in</strong> his/her first language.<br />

Table 6.6 Assignment of animal calls to animals, by 8 speakers of Swedish<br />

and 7 speakers of other languages: French (2 subjects), English,<br />

Hungarian, Czech, Slovenian, Ososo.<br />

8 Swedish 7 other languages<br />

expected answer other answer expected answer other answer<br />

1.dog 8 6<br />

2. cow 8 7<br />

3. horse 5 donkey 5 3 donkey 2<br />

jackal 1 cat 1<br />

4. duck 0 ? 5 0 ? 4<br />

goose 1 frog 2<br />

hen 1<br />

hippopotamus 1<br />

5. pig 7 dog 1 2 dog 3<br />

? 1<br />

206


6. duck 0 sheep 5 1 donkey 1<br />

crow 2 raven 1<br />

lion 1 crow 1<br />

wolf 1 sheep 1<br />

? 1 ? 2<br />

7. hen 2 goose 1 4 ? 3<br />

squirrel 1<br />

? 4<br />

8. rooster 8 5 hen 1<br />

pigeon 1<br />

9. duck 0 turkey 1 2 frog 1<br />

sheep 1 ? 4<br />

magpie 1<br />

? 5<br />

10. cuckoo 0 hen 4 1 turkey 2<br />

rooster 1 hen 1<br />

? 3 rooster 1<br />

? 2<br />

11. katt 7 ? 1 7<br />

12. cow, pig<br />

(cow)<br />

2 sheep 2 5 (cow) frog 1<br />

owl 2 ? 1<br />

? 2<br />

13. rooster 7 hen 1 5 ? 2<br />

14. mouse 0 squirrel 1 1 bird 2<br />

great titmouse 1 sparrow 1<br />

small bird 1 chicken 1<br />

snake 1 ? 2<br />

? 3<br />

15. dog (ch<strong>in</strong>.) 1 ch<strong>in</strong>. peacock 1 0 Ch<strong>in</strong>. peacock 1<br />

pek<strong>in</strong>gese dog ? 6<br />

rabbit 1<br />

? 5<br />

16. dog 1 duck 1 1 frog 1<br />

? 6 duck 1<br />

? 4<br />

207


6.4.4 Discussion of the test on identify<strong>in</strong>g animal<br />

calls<br />

The animals that were correctly identified by all listeners (who gave an<br />

answer) were: dog (<strong>in</strong> Hungarian, [vau vau]), cow (<strong>in</strong> [mπ´<br />

Icelandic, :])<br />

and cat (<strong>in</strong> Korean, [njaON]). The Arabic word for the dog’s bark, [hab:<br />

hab:] and the Korean [maN maN] were not as accurately identified. The<br />

three <strong>in</strong>stances of the duck's sound (from Hungarian, Icelandic and<br />

Estonian) all gave a variety of answers. Only one animal sound was not<br />

identified by anybody, [hap hap] (Hungarian duck). The animal sounds<br />

that were identified by only 1 person were [bra: bra:] (Icelandic duck),<br />

[pakUk: pakUk:] (Korean cuckoo, however, was <strong>in</strong>terpreted as other<br />

birds), [tSi tSi] (mouse <strong>in</strong> Urdu, however, <strong>in</strong>terpreted as various small<br />

animals), [maN maN] (Korean dog).<br />

In general it can be said that certa<strong>in</strong> animal calls were more difficult to<br />

identify, others were easier. The words for the duck sounds were difficult<br />

while the word for the cat's meow was easy, <strong>in</strong> spite of the fact that the<br />

word chosen for a meow for the test was the one that was most deviant.<br />

There was no clear difference between the larger group of Swedish<br />

speakers and speakers of the other languages.<br />

Several times the <strong>in</strong>formants guessed on the correct k<strong>in</strong>d of animal, even<br />

though they did not give exactly the expected answer, e.g. 'bird' (see 6, 8,<br />

10) or 'small animal' (see 14). This means that they identified the size of<br />

the animal <strong>in</strong> question. Some subjects were more unsure than others. No<br />

subject guessed throughout on certa<strong>in</strong> animals. A few subjects were more<br />

imag<strong>in</strong>ative and more specific. 6<br />

6.5 Conclusions of studies of expressive<br />

<strong>in</strong>terjections and expressions of animal calls<br />

Interjections and expressions of animal calls exist <strong>in</strong> all the languages <strong>in</strong><br />

the study. There are similar sounds and mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> all the languages, but<br />

there are also categories that seem to be specific to a certa<strong>in</strong> language,<br />

e.g. Icelandic has a special command 'go away' directed to sheep. The<br />

expressions of the <strong>in</strong>terjections are not the same <strong>in</strong> all the languages, but<br />

on the other hand they are not totally different; for some categories they<br />

6 In question 15 there might be an error; somebody must have whispered "Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" aloud.<br />

It is still <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g that several subjects judged "Ch<strong>in</strong>ese" to be fitt<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

208


are very similar, e.g. 'sneeze', 'good taste' cf. table 6.3. These expressions<br />

have an onomatopoeic basis which can be conventionalized <strong>in</strong> different<br />

ways <strong>in</strong> the different languages (the expressive <strong>in</strong>terjections also have an<br />

<strong>in</strong>dexical basis, i.e. a bodily reaction produces a sound, e.g. a sneeze or a<br />

scream, cf. 2.4). The phonological system <strong>in</strong>fluences the perception of the<br />

sounds and the choice of an adequate expression for imitation, e.g. [D] <strong>in</strong><br />

Icelandic. There are, however, tendencies for the whole material: most of<br />

these consonants are labial or dental. The vowels are few and mostly [i],<br />

[a], [u].<br />

The study of expressions of animal calls shows similar phonological<br />

tendencies (the semantic categories <strong>in</strong>vestigated were fixed, as specific<br />

animal calls were asked for): The imitations of the animal calls are not the<br />

same <strong>in</strong> all the languages, but on the other hand they are not totally<br />

different. Also, some expressions of animal calls are more alike <strong>in</strong> the<br />

different languages, e.g. Persian cat [miau] and F<strong>in</strong>nish cat [miau] while<br />

others vary more, e.g. Korean pig [kulkul] and Swedish pig [nØf:nØf:] cf.<br />

table 6.4. The imitations of animal calls can be conventionalized <strong>in</strong><br />

different ways <strong>in</strong> the different languages, e.g. Swedish has [P] or [¨]<br />

where other languages have [u], for example Swedish [kPkelik¨:] and<br />

Hungarian [kukuriku]. The most common vowels are [i], [a], [u], i.e.,<br />

closed or open vowels.<br />

In the <strong>in</strong>terpretation test of expressions of animal calls (cf. 6.4.2–6.4.4),<br />

some animals were identified by all listeners (dog, cow and cat), some<br />

less correctly identified and one animal was not identified at all (duck,<br />

Hungarian). The <strong>in</strong>terpretation test shows that, given the context of<br />

animal calls, it was quite easy for listeners to <strong>in</strong>terpret animal calls from<br />

languages other than their own.<br />

Prosody, voice quality and gestures are most probably important for both<br />

<strong>in</strong>terjections and imitation of animal calls. These forms for expression<br />

need to be studied further.<br />

209


6.6 Test of cross language <strong>in</strong>terpretation of<br />

Swedish onomatopoeic and other sound symbolic<br />

words<br />

6.6.1 Method<br />

In order to see how non-Swedish speakers <strong>in</strong>terpret Swedish<br />

onomatopoeic and other sound symbolic expressions, an experiment<br />

(similar to that <strong>in</strong> 7.1.3) was performed.<br />

The subjects were six persons who did not have Swedish as a first<br />

language, one each from of the languages Arabic, Spanish, German,<br />

Dutch, Ibo and English.<br />

A list of 15 written words was presented and read aloud to each subject.<br />

One of the words was repeated. Three of the words were not real ones<br />

but were <strong>in</strong>stead constructed out of phonesthemes. The subjects were told<br />

to write down their answers <strong>in</strong> a language of their own choice. The words<br />

are:<br />

fjompig<br />

skvalpa<br />

skrälle<br />

pjaltig (constructed)<br />

vresig<br />

glansig<br />

slabbig<br />

bjaltig (constructed)<br />

blankig (constructed end<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

pladdig (constructed)<br />

trumpen<br />

kladdig<br />

slabbig<br />

fladdrig<br />

grubbel<br />

stripig<br />

The subjects were <strong>in</strong>structed to try to <strong>in</strong>terpret each word and suggest an<br />

appropriate mean<strong>in</strong>g. What is here called the conventional <strong>in</strong>terpretation<br />

of a constructed word is one where the answer has a semantic feature that<br />

210


can be connected with a certa<strong>in</strong> consonant cluster <strong>in</strong> accordance with the<br />

analysis of chapter 4. These <strong>in</strong>itial clusters: pj-, bj-, pl- and bl- were<br />

successful <strong>in</strong> different parts of the test <strong>in</strong> chapter 7 (especially pj) and<br />

could thus be assumed to represent a Swedish norm.<br />

Subjects who knew some Swedish were told to mark the words that they<br />

knew already. After the test the subjects were free to orally elaborate on<br />

their answers.<br />

6.6.2 Results of <strong>in</strong>terpretation of cross language<br />

Swedish onomatopoeic and other sound symbolic<br />

words<br />

The subjects seldom <strong>in</strong>terpreted the words <strong>in</strong> the conventional way.<br />

However, nearly all of the answers given belong to semantic features of<br />

the model <strong>in</strong> 2.6.2 and the semantic features of chapter 4. It thus seems<br />

that certa<strong>in</strong> semantic categories are preferred to be expressed sound<br />

symbolically, but that the expression can vary.<br />

The subjects will first be accounted for one by one:<br />

Arabic speaker : This subject had very little knowledge of Swedish and<br />

knew none of the words. He answered <strong>in</strong> English. He guessed accord<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

convention on one word. The mean<strong>in</strong>g suggestions belonged to the<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g categories: The ones <strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> an acceptable way, as<br />

concerns phonesthemes, were:<br />

fjompig - big mouth ('pejorative')<br />

pjaltig - speaks wrong ('pejorative', 'talk<strong>in</strong>g')<br />

slabbig - slap him/her on face ('beat')<br />

bjaltig - speaks wrong ('pejorative', 'talk<strong>in</strong>g')<br />

trumpen - drum’s sound ('sound')<br />

fladdrig - eruption ('quick or strong movement')<br />

For some of the mis<strong>in</strong>terpreted words (mis<strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> terms of<br />

phonesthemes), the semantic categories of the given answers were<br />

nevertheless <strong>in</strong> accordance with the sound symbolic categories of the<br />

model <strong>in</strong> 2.6.2 (These mis<strong>in</strong>terpreted words are labeled "unconventional<br />

but possible category" <strong>in</strong> table 6.7).<br />

211


'wetness'<br />

'light'<br />

'quick or strong movement', 'wetness'<br />

'quick or strong movement'<br />

The responses that did not fit <strong>in</strong>to any category of the semantic model (or<br />

were difficult to <strong>in</strong>terpret) are: shut up, close the door, k<strong>in</strong>d of penalty,<br />

<strong>in</strong>tuitive, my country.<br />

Spanish speaker : This subject had a good knowledge of Swedish, both<br />

passively and actively. Her answers were given <strong>in</strong> Spanish, after which<br />

they were translated. The subject knew 4 of the words and guessed<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to convention, <strong>in</strong> terms of phonesthemes, on 3. The ones<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> an acceptable way, as concerns phonesthemes, are:<br />

skrälle - chim<strong>in</strong>g ('sound') 7<br />

blankig - opaque ('light') 8<br />

As for the Arabic speaker, for some of the mis<strong>in</strong>terpreted words (<strong>in</strong><br />

terms of phonesthemes), the semantic categories of the given answers<br />

were <strong>in</strong> accordance with sound symbolic categories of the model <strong>in</strong> 2.6.2.<br />

'adhesion'<br />

'beat'<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> form' (2)<br />

'rough surface structure'<br />

'destruction'<br />

German speaker: This subject had a good passive knowledge of Swedish,<br />

but he did not know any of the words on the list. The answers were given<br />

<strong>in</strong> English. He guessed accord<strong>in</strong>g to convention on 3 words. The words<br />

that were given an acceptable <strong>in</strong>terpretation, <strong>in</strong> terms of phonesthemes,<br />

were:<br />

fjompig - condescend<strong>in</strong>g ('pejorative')<br />

vresig - angry ('mental feel<strong>in</strong>g')<br />

glansig - sh<strong>in</strong>y ('light')<br />

7 Skr- does not imitate a chim<strong>in</strong>g sound at all, but <strong>in</strong> the previous analysis no dist<strong>in</strong>ction<br />

has been made between different k<strong>in</strong>ds of sound.<br />

8 This is of course the opposite of 'light'.<br />

212


lankig - sh<strong>in</strong>y ('light')<br />

pladdig - looks like a blot of e.g. yogurt ('wetness')<br />

fladdrig - someth<strong>in</strong>g which is decomposed, th<strong>in</strong> ('th<strong>in</strong> form')<br />

The rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>g suggestions can be classified <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

semantic fields:<br />

'small size'<br />

'rough surface structure' (wr<strong>in</strong>kled sk<strong>in</strong> of tomato)<br />

'form' (distorted)<br />

'pejorative' (distorted; heavy, fat, uncontrolled; unordered)<br />

'mood' (aggressive - of females)<br />

More difficult to classify <strong>in</strong> the previously discussed categories are the<br />

answers: "powerful"; "no more energy".<br />

Dutch speaker : This speaker had studied Swedish for a year and was very<br />

fluent. She had lived <strong>in</strong> Sweden for a very short period. She gave her<br />

answers <strong>in</strong> Swedish or English. She knew 1 of the words and guessed<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to convention on 1 word. The words that were given a<br />

plausible <strong>in</strong>terpretation, <strong>in</strong> terms of phonesthemes, were:<br />

skvalpa - call names at someone ('talk<strong>in</strong>g', 'pejorative')<br />

skrälle - say someth<strong>in</strong>g with a lot of sound ('sound', 'talk<strong>in</strong>g')<br />

pjaltig - snobbish ('pejorative')<br />

slabbig - unorganized ('pejorative')<br />

trumpen - garbage ('pejorative')<br />

kladdig - to write <strong>in</strong> an ugly way ('pejorative')<br />

stripig - someth<strong>in</strong>g with many stripes ('long th<strong>in</strong> form')<br />

For the mis<strong>in</strong>terpreted words (<strong>in</strong> terms of phonesthemes), the semantic<br />

categories given were, however, <strong>in</strong> accordance with the model:<br />

'form'<br />

'mental feel<strong>in</strong>g'<br />

'round form'<br />

'quick or strong movement' (2).<br />

One answer could not be classified accord<strong>in</strong>g to the model: 'void'.<br />

213


Ibo (and English) speaker: This speaker knew none of the words and did<br />

not guess accord<strong>in</strong>g to convention <strong>in</strong> any case. The words that were given<br />

a plausible <strong>in</strong>terpretation (acceptable category), <strong>in</strong> terms of phonesthemes,<br />

were:<br />

slabbig - lazy ('pejorative')<br />

pladdig - dirty ('pejorative')<br />

trumpen - loud ('sound')<br />

grubbel - grumble ('talk<strong>in</strong>g')<br />

The semantic categories given were <strong>in</strong> accordance with the model<br />

(unconventional but possible category) for some of the mis<strong>in</strong>terpreted<br />

words (<strong>in</strong> terms of phonesthemes):<br />

'putt<strong>in</strong>g together'<br />

'bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g'<br />

Seven of the answers could not be classified accord<strong>in</strong>g to the model<br />

(unconventional category): deep, slowness, positive, closed, tight, empty,<br />

wide.<br />

English speaker This subject had good knowledge of Swedish, Norwegian<br />

and German and answered <strong>in</strong> English. She knew 4 of the test words and<br />

guessed accord<strong>in</strong>g to convention on 2 of the words. The words that were<br />

given an acceptable <strong>in</strong>terpretation, <strong>in</strong> terms of phonesthemes, were:<br />

skvalpa - to gossip ('talk<strong>in</strong>g')<br />

skrälle - to compla<strong>in</strong>, wh<strong>in</strong>e ('talk<strong>in</strong>g')<br />

vresig - twist<strong>in</strong>g ('quick or strong movement')<br />

fladdrig - flimsy ('th<strong>in</strong> form')<br />

grubbel - annoy<strong>in</strong>g, irritat<strong>in</strong>g stuff ('bad mood')<br />

However, the semantic categories given were <strong>in</strong> accordance with the<br />

model (unconventional but possible category), for some of the<br />

mis<strong>in</strong>terpreted words (<strong>in</strong> terms of phonesthemes):<br />

'round form'<br />

'adhesion'<br />

'sound'<br />

214


One of the answers could not be classified accord<strong>in</strong>g to the model<br />

(unconventional category): tricked<br />

6.6.3 Conclusions from the test on cross language<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation of Swedish onomatopoeic and sound<br />

symbolic words<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g table, 6.7, shows the numbers of answers with different<br />

degrees of accuracy for each subject.<br />

Table 6.7 Summary of the numbers of answers with different degrees of<br />

accuracy <strong>in</strong> the cross l<strong>in</strong>guistic word <strong>in</strong>terpretation study.<br />

subject’s<br />

language<br />

known<br />

words<br />

guess <strong>in</strong><br />

accordance<br />

with<br />

conven-<br />

tion<br />

accept.<br />

category<br />

215<br />

unconventional<br />

but<br />

possible<br />

category<br />

unconventional<br />

category<br />

Arabic 0 1 6 4 5 0<br />

Spanish 4 3 2 6 0 0<br />

German 0 3 6 5 2 0<br />

Dutch 1 1 7 5 1 0<br />

Ibo 0 0 4 2 7 2<br />

English 4 2 5 3 1 0<br />

sum 9 10 30 25 15 2<br />

Few of the words were known to the subjects, not even to those who had<br />

good knowledge of Swedish. There were also quite few correct guesses.<br />

Their performance is better when their answers are classified <strong>in</strong>to<br />

semantic categories which are related to phonesthemes of the <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

clusters (column "acceptable category").<br />

The frequencies of words that were judged accord<strong>in</strong>g to convention (<strong>in</strong><br />

terms of phonesthemes) are shown <strong>in</strong> table 6.8.<br />

All 15 test words are represented <strong>in</strong> the results and there is no<br />

great preference for any word or words.<br />

no<br />

answer


Table 6.8 Frequencies of words that were most often successfully<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpreted accord<strong>in</strong>g to the Swedish norm, by the six speakers.<br />

Test words Number of speakers<br />

fladdrig 3<br />

skrälle 3<br />

trumpen 3<br />

blankig 2<br />

fjompig 2<br />

grubbel 2<br />

pjaltig 2<br />

pladdig 2<br />

skvalpa 2<br />

slabbig 2<br />

vresig 2<br />

bjaltig 1<br />

glansig 1<br />

kladdig 1<br />

stripig 1<br />

Table 6.9 shows which semantic categories were used most often, when<br />

the subjects guessed <strong>in</strong> an unconventional way (i.e. there is no possibility,<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to the lexical analysis of chapter 4, that the consonant cluster <strong>in</strong><br />

question can have this mean<strong>in</strong>g).<br />

This result means that even when subjects guess <strong>in</strong> an unconventional way,<br />

they still guess with<strong>in</strong> the semantic categories of the model. The answers<br />

that can not be classified with<strong>in</strong> the semantic categories of the model (the<br />

column “unconventional category” of table 6.7) are fewer. Most of them<br />

were produced by the Arabic and Ibo speaker, which suggests an<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluence of cultural (or l<strong>in</strong>guistic) differences, i.e. European vs. non-<br />

European. 9<br />

9 These results are very prelim<strong>in</strong>ary and clearly a larger <strong>in</strong>vestigation is needed.<br />

216


Table 6.9 Most commonly preferred mean<strong>in</strong>gs for unconventional but<br />

possible mean<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

semantic features number of<br />

words<br />

quick or strong movement 4<br />

wetness 2<br />

adhesion 2<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form 2<br />

rough surface structure 2<br />

round form 2<br />

form 2<br />

pejorative 1<br />

putt<strong>in</strong>g together 1<br />

bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g 1<br />

mood 1<br />

mental feel<strong>in</strong>g 1<br />

small size 1<br />

destruction 1<br />

beat 1<br />

sound 1<br />

light 1<br />

6.7 General conclusions and discussion of the<br />

cross language studies<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g conclusions can be drawn from the different contrastive<br />

studies reported <strong>in</strong> this chapter: The Thesaurus study has shown that the<br />

expression of phonesthemes can differ between languages as closely<br />

related as English and Swedish. In some phonesthemes, however, the<br />

expression (and mean<strong>in</strong>g) is the same.<br />

The study of <strong>in</strong>terjections shows that there are similarities of expression<br />

as well as of mean<strong>in</strong>g between unrelated languages. There is variation of<br />

expression with<strong>in</strong> semantic categories, but certa<strong>in</strong> sounds are still<br />

preferred.<br />

The study of expressions for animal calls <strong>in</strong> different languages shows<br />

that there are similarities as well as differences. The understand<strong>in</strong>g test<br />

217


showed that it is quite easy for subjects to guess correctly on expressions<br />

for animal calls <strong>in</strong> other languages. Some animals were more difficult to<br />

identify. Often the type of animal was identified, e.g. bird or small<br />

animal.<br />

The last study, on <strong>in</strong>terpretation of Swedish sound symbolic words and<br />

non-words (constructed out of phonesthemes), showed that it was quite<br />

difficult for subjects to <strong>in</strong>terpret the mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> accordance with the<br />

Swedish norm, but that the mis<strong>in</strong>terpretations most often were with<strong>in</strong> the<br />

semantic categories of the model <strong>in</strong> 2.6.2.<br />

The general conclusion from these studies is that there are greater<br />

differences between languages for expression than for mean<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> sound<br />

symbolism. The more onomatopoeic expressions are easier to <strong>in</strong>terpret<br />

than other sound symbolism for speakers of other languages.<br />

Interpretation of other sound symbolism often goes wrong (because<br />

expressions differ <strong>in</strong> different languages), but the semantic categories<br />

guessed on are most often with<strong>in</strong> the semantic model of this thesis.<br />

218


7 Experiments with constructed words conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

phonesthemes<br />

7.1 Production and understand<strong>in</strong>g<br />

In section 1.4 the hypothesis was formulated that phonesthemes are<br />

productive, to a greater or lesser degree. In order to test this hypothesis I<br />

have carried out several experiments. The purpose of the experiments<br />

that will be described <strong>in</strong> this chapter is to test this hypothesis, also <strong>in</strong><br />

more detail for some of the phonesthemes. In chapter 4 the results of the<br />

lexical analysis made by one person were presented. The results of this<br />

analysis are to a certa<strong>in</strong> extent dependent on the idiosyncrasies of the<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual subject, the material and other circumstances and are therefore<br />

prelim<strong>in</strong>ary. Therefore the results of the lexical analysis were used as a<br />

basis for test<strong>in</strong>g the sound symbolic value of certa<strong>in</strong> consonant clusters on<br />

a great number of <strong>in</strong>dividuals.<br />

Method and materials<br />

The tests (see Appendix 3) are constructed as free choice, multiple-choice<br />

and match<strong>in</strong>g tests.<br />

Test 1.a. is a free choice test, which goes from mean<strong>in</strong>g to expression, to<br />

test the production of sound symbolism, e.g. "Invent a short word for<br />

somebody who is stupid".<br />

Test 1.b. is a forced choice test which also goes from mean<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

expression. An example of a task is: "Which of the follow<strong>in</strong>g words fits<br />

best for a person who is silly: a) pjotig b) brotig c) splotig?" Only one<br />

answer should be possible accord<strong>in</strong>g to the previous analysis. For some of<br />

the questions, one of the answers is supposed to be clearly contradictory,<br />

i. e. the expected answer<strong>in</strong>g score is zero (where there are contrast<strong>in</strong>g<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs, e. g. 'dry'-'wet'). In a few questions a word which sounds<br />

similar to the test word, but with a non-expected cluster is <strong>in</strong>cluded to test<br />

which is more important, word analogy or sound symbolic clusters, e.g.<br />

"Which of the follow<strong>in</strong>g words best describes a broken (trasig) object". a)<br />

bjatig b) skratig c) tratig?" Skr- is the expected cluster but tratig is very<br />

similar to trasig.<br />

219


Test 2.a is a free choice test which goes from expression to mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

order to test the understand<strong>in</strong>g of presumptive sound symbolic clusters,<br />

e.g. "What would be a good mean<strong>in</strong>g for the word fnotig?"<br />

In test 2.b, a forced choice test (which also goes from expression to<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g), each nonsense word shall be matched with a certa<strong>in</strong> number of<br />

recurr<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gs of a certa<strong>in</strong> abstractness, e.g. "someth<strong>in</strong>g which is<br />

soft". More specifically, for each nonsense word tested there will be 3<br />

alternative mean<strong>in</strong>gs, only 1 of which is possible accord<strong>in</strong>g to the<br />

previous analysis. An example of this is "What do you th<strong>in</strong>k slatig means:<br />

a) somebody who is silly b) someth<strong>in</strong>g that is dry c) someth<strong>in</strong>g which is<br />

unpleasant?"<br />

Test 3 is a match<strong>in</strong>g test where the subject has to choose between two<br />

different mean<strong>in</strong>gs and two different expressions. An example of this is:<br />

Which word best describes a th<strong>in</strong>g that is wet and which word best<br />

describes a th<strong>in</strong>g that is dry: fnottig or skvottig?"<br />

For the tests 2a and b, nonsense words were constructed from <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

consonant clusters that are commonly used for onomatopoeia and other<br />

sound symbolism, accord<strong>in</strong>g to the lexical analysis <strong>in</strong> chapter 4. To avoid<br />

motivated mean<strong>in</strong>g appear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the non-<strong>in</strong>itial part of the neologism the<br />

words constructed are short. Also the end<strong>in</strong>gs (-t or -t:) of the neologisms<br />

were checked <strong>in</strong> Svensk baklängesordbok (1981) <strong>in</strong> order to a v o i d<br />

motivated mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> the end<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

A complication with us<strong>in</strong>g monosyllables is that this syllable structure,<br />

when repeated <strong>in</strong> a list, gives an impression of 'beat' or 'sound imitation',<br />

especially when it ends <strong>in</strong> a long consonant. For this reason the test words<br />

do not have a long consonant (except <strong>in</strong> a few test cases). Furthermore,<br />

the test words have been added with the semantically neutral end<strong>in</strong>g -ig.<br />

A problem with this might be, however, that <strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g longer words, more<br />

consonants and vowels will be <strong>in</strong>volved, and it is more difficult to keep<br />

control over entire test words.<br />

220


Aside from construct<strong>in</strong>g monosyllabic words (with an added end<strong>in</strong>g -ig),<br />

another simplification is that <strong>in</strong> natural sound symbolic words, the<br />

semantic feature, e. g. 'pejorative', of an <strong>in</strong>itial consonant cluster is often<br />

repeated <strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al consonant cluster (cf. 5.6).<br />

As was shown <strong>in</strong> diagram 4.5, the 13 most common semantic features of<br />

Swedish <strong>in</strong>itial clusters are, <strong>in</strong> descend<strong>in</strong>g order: 'pejorative', 'sound',<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> form', 'quick or strong movement', 'wetness', 'talk<strong>in</strong>g', 'light',<br />

'dim<strong>in</strong>utive', 'round form', 'walk<strong>in</strong>g', 'destruction', 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form', 'bad<br />

mood'. These semantic features were used <strong>in</strong> the tests except for 'sound',<br />

'dim<strong>in</strong>utive', 'round form' and 'way of walk<strong>in</strong>g' 1. Also some more<br />

<strong>in</strong>frequent features, shown <strong>in</strong> the list below, were used. The formulations<br />

on the test sheets correspond to the semantic features above <strong>in</strong> the way<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> table 7.1.<br />

Table 7.1 The semantic features correspond<strong>in</strong>g to the formulations on the<br />

test sheets.<br />

formulations semantic features<br />

löjlig (silly) pejorative<br />

fånig (silly) pejorative<br />

obehaglig (unpleasant) pejorative<br />

dum och klumpig (stupid and<br />

clumsy)<br />

pejorative<br />

pratar dumheter (talks nonsense) pejorative, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

rak (straight) long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

smal form (narrow form) long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

rörelse fram och tillbaka (movement<br />

back and forth)<br />

quick or strong movement<br />

plötslig rörelse (sudden movement) quick or strong movement<br />

(mycket) blöt ((very) wet) wetness<br />

1 '<strong>Sound</strong>' was not tested s<strong>in</strong>ce it is usually easier for speakers to produce sound imitative<br />

words. 'Dim<strong>in</strong>utive', 'round form', and 'walk<strong>in</strong>g' were not <strong>in</strong>cluded because the f<strong>in</strong>al lexical<br />

analysis was not completed at the time the test was constructed, and therefore these categories<br />

were not <strong>in</strong>cluded. The category of 'form' is, however, well represented <strong>in</strong> the study.<br />

221


hur en människa kan låta (how a<br />

person can sound)<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

starkt lysande (sh<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g brightly) light<br />

trasig (broken) destruction<br />

sl<strong>in</strong>grig form (w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form) w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

arg (angry) bad mood<br />

på dåligt humör (bad mood) bad mood<br />

ihålig (hollow) hollow form<br />

pekar åt olika håll (po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

different directions)<br />

separation<br />

går ihop (comes together) putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

(mycket) torr ((very) dry) dryness<br />

tydlig hård ytstruktur (pronounced<br />

hard surface structure)<br />

rough surface structure<br />

följsam ytstruktur (adaptable surface<br />

structure)<br />

soft consistency<br />

sitter fast (sticks) adhesion<br />

vidhäftande (stick<strong>in</strong>g) adhesion<br />

hårt slag (hard beat) beat<br />

The most common consonant clusters, <strong>in</strong> absolute numbers and<br />

percentally, are shown <strong>in</strong> diagrams 4.1 and 4.4. In this test all of these<br />

clusters except nj- are used. The tests <strong>in</strong>cludes the clusters: kl-, gr-, vr-<br />

, spj-, str-, sp-, mj-, kv-, skv-, skr-, kr-, bl-, sm-, gr-, st-, tr-<br />

, bl-, br-, spl-, gn-, spr-, fr-, sk-, kn-, gl-, sv-, dr-, pl-, sl-,<br />

sn, fl-, bj-, pj-, fn- and fj-.<br />

The test words are grotig, gratig, vrotig, vratig, spjotig, spjatig, stratig,<br />

spatig, mjatig, kvatig, skvatig, skratig, bjetig, bjatig, kretig, kratig, bletig,<br />

blatig, smatig, statig, tratig, platig, fnatig, krotig, strotig, trotig, pjotig,<br />

brotig, splotig, snitig, gnitig, gritig, klatig, spratig, smitig, gritig, spitig,<br />

platig, snatig, smatig, dratig, blattig, mjattig, flattig, snattig, smattig,<br />

drattig, etc (see Appendix 3).<br />

222


Test procedure<br />

The test questions were written four to a page and the pages were stapled<br />

together. The test questions were mixed <strong>in</strong> order to keep the test words<br />

from <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g each other (e.g. no two questions about 'light' were put<br />

on the same page). The subjects were told not to go back and check and<br />

were not given time for that either. Half of the subjects were tested <strong>in</strong> the<br />

order: 1 (a)(b) -2(a)(b) -3 and the other half <strong>in</strong> the order 2 (a)(b) -1(a)(b)<br />

-3 (see Appendix 3).<br />

Test (a) which is free production, is the most difficult, test (b) which is<br />

multiple-choice, is less difficult, and test 3, which is match<strong>in</strong>g, is the<br />

easiest. The largest number of presumed answers are expected for test 3<br />

and the least number of presumed (or consistent) answers are expected for<br />

1a and 2a.<br />

The subjects were l<strong>in</strong>guistics students at the beg<strong>in</strong>ner's level, with<br />

Swedish as a first language, males and females, ages 21–57. There were<br />

15 <strong>in</strong>formants. 14 of them took the tests from mean<strong>in</strong>g to expression and<br />

15 <strong>in</strong>formants took the tests from expression to mean<strong>in</strong>g. Thirteen of<br />

them took the match<strong>in</strong>g test.<br />

7.1.1 Forced choice for production – from<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g to phonological (graphic) form<br />

Of the 39 test questions where a mean<strong>in</strong>g was presented along with three<br />

alternatives for phonetic (graphic) form, one of which was possible<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to the model, 28 test questions showed a majority for expected<br />

answers, 4 had a shared majority for the expected and an unexpected<br />

answer and 7 showed a majority for an unexpected answer. Sometimes<br />

the majority was overwhelm<strong>in</strong>g, sometimes not, see table 7.2.<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g 19 (out of 39) test questions received the best results: no.<br />

37, 38, 39, 35, 30, 31, 32, 29, 21, 22, 23, 24, 13, 16, 9, 10, 5, 7 and 8<br />

(cf. Appendix 3). These questions concerned the features and words<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> table 7.2.<br />

223


Table 7.2 The semantic features of the 19 test questions that received the best<br />

results <strong>in</strong> the forced choice test of choos<strong>in</strong>g phonological form from<br />

different semantic features. Words are presented <strong>in</strong> order from best match<br />

with expected to worst. 14 <strong>in</strong>formants.<br />

semantic feature word ratio of expected<br />

answers<br />

'pejorative' fjotig 14/14<br />

'dryness' fnotig 13/14<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> form' stratig 13/14<br />

'wetness' spatig 13/14<br />

'bad mood' vratig 13/14<br />

'pejorative' pjotig 13/14<br />

'wetness' platig 12/14<br />

'pejorative' bjatig 12/14<br />

'quick or strong<br />

movement'<br />

flattig 12/14<br />

'adhesion' klatig 12/14<br />

'bad mood' tratig 12/14<br />

'separation' spratig 11/14<br />

'hollow form' gratig 11/14<br />

'wetness' svotig 11/14<br />

'quick or strong<br />

movement'<br />

skottig 11/14<br />

'pejorative' drotig 11/14<br />

'hollow form' grotig 10/14<br />

'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form' krotig 9/14<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g' snatig 9/14<br />

Seen <strong>in</strong> relation to the number of <strong>in</strong>stances of each feature there were <strong>in</strong><br />

the test, the semantic features shown <strong>in</strong> table 7.3 are the most successfully<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpreted:<br />

224


Table 7.3 The most successfully <strong>in</strong>terpreted semantic features of table 7.2.,<br />

14 <strong>in</strong>formants.<br />

'wetness:' 3/3 2 (100%)<br />

'hollow form': 2/2 (100%)<br />

'separation': 1/1(100%)<br />

'dryness': 1/1(100%)<br />

'pejorative': 4/6 (67%)<br />

'bad mood': 2/3 (67%)<br />

'quick or strong movement': 2/4 (50%)<br />

'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form': 1/2 (50%)<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g': 1/2 (50%)<br />

'adhesion': 1/2 (50%)<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> form': 1/3 (33%)<br />

The questions that did not give the expected answers perta<strong>in</strong>ed to the<br />

features and words <strong>in</strong> table 7.4 (for a majority of subjects).<br />

Table 7.4 Semantic features for neologisms that did not give the expected<br />

answers. The substitutions of words (with frequencies) are shown <strong>in</strong> the<br />

right column. 14 <strong>in</strong>formants.<br />

'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form' stratig (6) <strong>in</strong>stead of kratig (4)<br />

'light' kratig (7) <strong>in</strong>stead of blatig (3)<br />

'light' snitig (8) <strong>in</strong>stead of gnitig (2)<br />

'quick or strong movement' mjatig (6) <strong>in</strong>stead of flatig (3)<br />

'light' snotig (9) <strong>in</strong>stead of gnotig (2)<br />

'light' kretig (6) <strong>in</strong>stead of gletig (4)<br />

It is clear that the 'light' feature gets least expected answers. The clusters<br />

tested are the well known bl-, gl- and gn- clusters, but obviously<br />

someth<strong>in</strong>g more is required to get a light-associative effect. The 'light'<br />

words might also belong to a closed lexical class which would imply that<br />

gl- , gn-, bl- are not productive, unlike phonesthemes such as kl- or<br />

2 i.e. 3 of the 3 wetness words were identified by most subjects.<br />

225


pj-. Another possible explanation, <strong>in</strong> light of the results <strong>in</strong> 5.4, is that the<br />

vowels have to be taken <strong>in</strong>to account. The lexical analysis showed that<br />

front vowels, and especially i, are much more common <strong>in</strong> root<br />

morphemes with the features 'light' or 'gaze'. Two of the constructed<br />

words <strong>in</strong> the test, gnitig and gletig, had front vowels (i and e) but were<br />

nevertheless not <strong>in</strong>terpreted accord<strong>in</strong>g to expectations.<br />

The feature most successfully coded <strong>in</strong> phonesthemes is 'wetness', for<br />

which 3 out of 3 words where identified by most subjects, 'hollow form'<br />

(2 out of 2), 'separation' (1 out of 1) and 'dryness' (1 out of 1), cf. table<br />

7.3. The feature that is the least successfully <strong>in</strong>terpreted is 'light'; none of<br />

the 4 test words were <strong>in</strong>terpreted accord<strong>in</strong>g to expectations, cf. table 7.4.<br />

S<strong>in</strong>ce some of the vowels <strong>in</strong> the constructed words were <strong>in</strong> accordance<br />

with expectations, the non-expected results of the 'light' words can not be<br />

expla<strong>in</strong>ed with reference to the importance of vowels. It seems rather that<br />

the 'light' phonesthemes are not productive. One explanation for why<br />

they are not productive could have to do with the fact that 'light' is the<br />

only category which is metaphorically connected with 'sound', i .e. by<br />

sense analogy. Many other categories are connected metonymically with<br />

sound, i.e. they occur simultaneously, e.g. 'movement', 'surface<br />

structure', 'wetness', 'form', or are more abstract (cf. 2.5, 2.6).<br />

7.1.2 Forced choice for understand<strong>in</strong>g - from<br />

phonological form to mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Out of the 38 test questions where a constructed word was presented<br />

along with three mean<strong>in</strong>g alternatives, one of which was possible (or most<br />

possible 3) accord<strong>in</strong>g to the model of 2.6.2 and the analysis of chapter 4,<br />

29 showed a majority for expected answers, 1 had a shared majority for<br />

expected and unexpected answers and 8 had a majority for an unexpected<br />

answer. Sometimes the majority was overwhelm<strong>in</strong>g, sometimes not. The<br />

test questions that had the best results were the follow<strong>in</strong>g: 24, 32, 26, 29,<br />

21, 23, 25, 18, 10, 12, 5, 3, 4, 14. These questions concerned the words<br />

and features presented <strong>in</strong> table 7.5.<br />

3 The only exception is fnotig (question 23) where 'rough surface structure' is slightly more<br />

expectable than 'dryness'. However, as can be seen <strong>in</strong> table 7.5, fnotig is <strong>in</strong>terpreted as<br />

'dryness' by 15 out of 15 subjects.<br />

226


Table 7.5 The words <strong>in</strong> the test questions that received the best results <strong>in</strong> the<br />

forced choice test of choos<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gs for different constructed words. 15<br />

<strong>in</strong>formants.<br />

word semantic feature ratio of expected<br />

answers<br />

grotig 'hollow form' 15/15<br />

fnotig 'dryness' 15/15<br />

snattig 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' 15/15<br />

pjotig 'pejorative' 15/15<br />

skottig 'quick or strong<br />

movement'<br />

14/15<br />

spratig 'separation' 14/15<br />

grotig 'bad mood' 11/15<br />

fjotig 'pejorative' 13/15<br />

vratig 'bad mood' 13/15<br />

blatig 'pejorative' 12/15<br />

spitig 'long th<strong>in</strong> form' 12/15<br />

snatig 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' 11/15<br />

skratig 'rough surface<br />

structure'<br />

10/15<br />

flattig 'quick or strong<br />

movement'<br />

10/15<br />

The semantic features which were the most successful <strong>in</strong> the forced choice<br />

test of choos<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gs for different constructed words were 'hollow<br />

form' (for gr-), 'dryness' (for fn-), 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' (for sn-), and 'pejorative'<br />

(for pj-)<br />

The questions that did not give the expected results concerned the forms<br />

and mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> table 7.6 (for a majority of speakers).<br />

227


Table 7.6 Words that did not give the expected answers. The substitutions of<br />

features. 15 <strong>in</strong>formants.<br />

word substitutions of<br />

features<br />

gnotig 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' <strong>in</strong>stead of<br />

'light'<br />

flatig 'long th<strong>in</strong> form' <strong>in</strong>stead<br />

of 'quick or strong<br />

movement'<br />

gletig 'separation' <strong>in</strong>stead of<br />

'light'<br />

gnitig 'stick<strong>in</strong>g' <strong>in</strong>stead of<br />

'light'<br />

glatig 'pejorative' <strong>in</strong>stead of<br />

'light'<br />

smatig 'pejorative' <strong>in</strong>stead of<br />

'beat'<br />

blatig 'hollow form' <strong>in</strong>stead<br />

of 'light'<br />

228<br />

comment<br />

This is <strong>in</strong> fact more<br />

expectable accord<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

the analysis <strong>in</strong> 4.5<br />

This is also expectable.<br />

There might be an<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluence from the<br />

word flat (flat).<br />

In this case a similar<br />

word gnatig has no<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluence. The third,<br />

but not chosen,<br />

alternative was actually<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g' which is also<br />

possible accord<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

the model<br />

'Pejorative' is <strong>in</strong> fact<br />

also expectable<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to the model<br />

'Pejorative' is <strong>in</strong> fact<br />

also expectable<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to the model<br />

The semantic feature which is the least successful is 'light' (for gn-, gland<br />

bl-). Even where the vowel was front, as <strong>in</strong> gletig and gnitig, the<br />

result was not accord<strong>in</strong>g to expectations.<br />

Summary of forced choice tests<br />

In the first test, 28 out of 39 test questions showed a majority for<br />

expected answers accord<strong>in</strong>g to the model, and 19 of these received good<br />

results, i.e. at least 64% (9/14) expected answers (table 7.2). In the second<br />

test, 29 out of 38 test questions showed a majority for expected answers,


and 13 of these received good results, i.e. at least 67% (10/15) expected<br />

answers.<br />

In both tests the semantic feature which was the least successfully<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpreted or produced was 'light'. Even when the vowel was front,<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to expectations, the constructed words were not <strong>in</strong>terpreted or<br />

produced accord<strong>in</strong>g to expectations. It thus seems as if the phonesthemes<br />

connected with 'light' are not productive. In the test of mean<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

phonological form, 28 of the 39 test questions showed a majority for<br />

expected answers. The six phonesthemes that were the most successfully<br />

coded were 'pejorative': fj-, 'dryness': fn-, long th<strong>in</strong> form': str-,<br />

'wetness': sp-, 'bad mood': vr-, 'pejorative': pj-. (The features most<br />

successfully coded totally were 'wetness', 'hollow form', 'separation' and<br />

'dryness'.) In the test from phonological form to mean<strong>in</strong>g, 29 of the 38<br />

test questions showed a majority for expected answers. The six<br />

phonesthemes most successfully <strong>in</strong>terpreted were 'hollow form': gr-,<br />

'dryness': fn-, 'talk<strong>in</strong>g': sn-, 'pejorative': pj-, 'quick or strong<br />

movement': sk- and 'separation': spr-. For both the <strong>in</strong>terpretation and<br />

production tests, the most successful cases were 'dryness': fn- and<br />

'pejorative': pj-. Both of these are lexically low frequency clusters that<br />

are sound symbolic to a very high degree.<br />

7.1.3 Free production test from constructed words<br />

to mean<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

The free production test from constructed forms to mean<strong>in</strong>gs asked the<br />

question "What do you th<strong>in</strong>k would be a good mean<strong>in</strong>g for ...?" This gave<br />

the results shown <strong>in</strong> table 7.7.<br />

229


Table 7.7 Free <strong>in</strong>terpretations by 15 subjects of 6 constructed words.<br />

skvatig fnotig vratig krötig pjotig skratig<br />

härsken tråkig vrensk stöddig tokig skarp<br />

(rancid) (bor<strong>in</strong>g) (refractory) (stuck up) (mad) (sharp)<br />

blöt obeslutsam tjatig trög pjoskig glad<br />

(wet) (irresolute) (nagg<strong>in</strong>g) (sluggish) (mawkish) (happy)<br />

tröttsam<br />

(tir<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

- sned<br />

(crooked)<br />

ihopklumpad<br />

(lumped<br />

together)<br />

barnslig<br />

(childish)<br />

bristfällig<br />

(defective)<br />

blöt<br />

(wet)<br />

torr och<br />

fladdrig<br />

(dry and<br />

flapp<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

vriden<br />

(twisted)<br />

trött och lite<br />

sjuk<br />

(tired and a<br />

little ill)<br />

fånig<br />

(silly)<br />

snurrig<br />

(giddy,<br />

crazy)<br />

blöt fnasig grov tillgjord petig dålig<br />

(wet) (chapped) (coarse) (affected) (f<strong>in</strong>ical) (bad)<br />

kaxig krokig tjatig knotig dum glad<br />

(cocky) (crooked) (nagg<strong>in</strong>g) (bony) (stupid) (happy)<br />

schlager- pustande skrytsam sluddrande gnetig trög<br />

musik<br />

(popular<br />

song)<br />

(pant<strong>in</strong>g) (boastful) (slurr<strong>in</strong>g) (crabbed) (sluggish)<br />

flamsig löjlig pratig rörig pjåskig kantig<br />

(silly) (ridiculous) (chatty) (messy) (mawkish) (angular)<br />

galen gammal arg mosig ojämn knyckig<br />

(crazy) (old) (angry) (fuddled) (uneven) (jerky)<br />

blöt knäpp vrängd krokig plaskig smal<br />

(wet) (stupid) (turned<br />

<strong>in</strong>side out)<br />

(crooked) (splashy) (narrow)<br />

något kantigt<br />

och blött<br />

(sth angular<br />

and wet)<br />

något svårt<br />

(sth difficult)<br />

pratsam<br />

tråkig<br />

(talkative<br />

bor<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

<strong>in</strong>krökt<br />

(focused on<br />

one´s own<br />

problems)<br />

besvärlig<br />

(troublesome)<br />

vissen<br />

(withered)<br />

skvalande skrumpen tjatig urgröpt fånig full med<br />

(pour<strong>in</strong>g) (shrunk) (nagg<strong>in</strong>g) (hollowed (ridiculous) revor<br />

out)<br />

(full of rips)<br />

kantig eller<br />

hård<br />

(angular or<br />

hard)<br />

knölig<br />

(knobbly)<br />

arg<br />

(angry)<br />

berusad<br />

(drunk)<br />

liten<br />

rar<br />

(small<br />

sweet)<br />

trasig<br />

(ragged)<br />

230


skratta<br />

(laugh)<br />

halvdålig* 4<br />

(half bad)<br />

frysa<br />

(freeze)<br />

knäpp*<br />

tokrolig*<br />

(crazy<br />

funny)<br />

vrida<br />

(wr<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

halvnerriven<br />

vägreklam*<br />

(half torn<br />

down<br />

advertizement<br />

by the<br />

road side)<br />

krånglig<br />

(trouble-<br />

231<br />

some)<br />

försupen*<br />

(sottish)<br />

passande<br />

(suitable)<br />

löjlig*<br />

liten*<br />

(ridiculous<br />

small)<br />

skrattande<br />

(laugh<strong>in</strong>g)<br />

en härjad<br />

smal kv<strong>in</strong>na*<br />

(a worn and<br />

haggard th<strong>in</strong><br />

woman)<br />

1. skvatig gives 6/15 expected answers, i.e., words with a semantic<br />

feature 'wetness'. Of the other mean<strong>in</strong>gs only one gives an answer with a<br />

feature 'wetness' – pjotig<br />

2. fnotig gets the expected 'pejorative' <strong>in</strong> 5/14 (perhaps more depend<strong>in</strong>g<br />

on which answers should be classified as 'pejorative'). It also gets the<br />

expected 'dry' <strong>in</strong> 2/14. (cf. diagram 4.20).<br />

3. vratig gives 3/15 clear answers conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the feature 'bad mood' and<br />

some other answers which are less clear.<br />

4. krötig does not give words with the expected semantic feature 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g<br />

form' but rather po<strong>in</strong>t to some sort of 'pejorative', which is the second<br />

expected feature for kr- (cf. diagram 4.19).<br />

5. pjotig, where the expected semantic feature 'pejorative' gives 10/15.<br />

6. skratig, where expected semantic feature 'destruction' gives 7/15.<br />

'Pejorative' has a higher percentage of skr- than 'destruction', but<br />

'destruction' is typical for skr-, cf. table 4.16.<br />

The nonsense word that is most successful is pjotig (pejorative). Pj- is<br />

percentally the fourth most sound symbolic cluster (cf. diagram 4.4) and<br />

is ma<strong>in</strong>ly 'pejorative' (cf. diagram 4.23). The second most successful<br />

word is skratig, which only belongs to the eleventh most sound symbolic<br />

cluster (percentally), skr-. However, it is a typical cluster for<br />

'destruction'. In third place comes skvatig (wetness).<br />

In other words, no constructed word is <strong>in</strong>terpreted as expected by all<br />

subjects, but all of the constructed words, except krötig (w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form),<br />

are <strong>in</strong>terpreted correctly by some subjects. Krötig is, however, given a<br />

4 The * marks the answers from the subject who deviated most <strong>in</strong> the "match<strong>in</strong>g test of<br />

nonsense words and mean<strong>in</strong>gs" (see 7.1.5).


second best <strong>in</strong>terpretation, 'pejorative'. The success rank<strong>in</strong>g of these six<br />

phonesthemes are thus, <strong>in</strong> order from best match to worst: 'pejorative'<br />

(for pj-), 'destruction' (for skr-), 'wetness' (for skv-), 'pejorative' (for<br />

fn-), 'bad mood' (for vr-), 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form' (for kr-).<br />

There are examples both of where the associations have gone to a word<br />

with a similar phonetic form, and examples of where the phonestheme has<br />

an <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>in</strong> spite of a similar sound<strong>in</strong>g word 5. An example of the first<br />

case is krötig where the associations often seem to go to kröka (dr<strong>in</strong>k<br />

alcohol): slang word): sluddrande, berusad, försupen. Skratig sometimes<br />

gives associations to skratt (laughter): glad, skrattande. There are,<br />

however, many answers under skratig which conta<strong>in</strong> the mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

component of 'destruction': bristfällig, lite sjuk, dålig, vissen, full med<br />

revor, trasigt, en härjad smal kv<strong>in</strong>na.<br />

The conclusion is that these nonsense words are <strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> accordance<br />

with expectations by some subjects, but not all. The nonsense words have<br />

a phonesthemic mean<strong>in</strong>g potential that is used by some subjects <strong>in</strong> the test<br />

situation. The phonestheme most easily <strong>in</strong>terpreted was 'pejorative': pj-.<br />

Explanations for this might have to do with the fact that pj- is a lexically<br />

low frequent highly sound symbolic cluster. 'Pejorative' is also the most<br />

frequent semantic feature accord<strong>in</strong>g to the lexical analysis. The cluster<br />

most difficult to <strong>in</strong>terpret was 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form': kr-. Kr- is lexically high<br />

frequent but also sound symbolic to quite a high degree, however not<br />

only with the feature 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form'. It seems as lexically low frequent<br />

highly sound symbolic clusters are easy to access.<br />

7.1.4 Free production from mean<strong>in</strong>g to constructed<br />

word.<br />

The question "Make up a short word for somebody or someth<strong>in</strong>g which is<br />

(has) ...?", where subjects were supposed to <strong>in</strong>vent an expression for one<br />

of the mean<strong>in</strong>gs 'silly', 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g', 'angry', 'dry', 'wet', and 'rough<br />

surface', gave the results shown <strong>in</strong> table 7.8.<br />

5 There are, as is well known, many types of relations <strong>in</strong> the lexicon, and thus relations<br />

between words, as well as between phonesthemes, cf. e.g. Garman (1990).<br />

232


Table 7.8 Words produced by 14 subjects for 6 different mean<strong>in</strong>gs. The<br />

words are non-words. Most of them follow the Swedish phonotactic rules.<br />

'silly' 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g' 'angry' 'dry' 'wet' 'rough<br />

surface'<br />

smurk slirv gurp lirv slish pritt<br />

spjal islig kral spri subl flarb<br />

flong plyr orn krasp svåsk donk<br />

fjän ril vrag fnat plat raster<br />

koos pis faaby kirl trippp hitt<br />

flutt sjl<strong>in</strong>g grol fnus svurp gilb<br />

floppig siloln börr kritto slasli teppig<br />

pjöl krel vram fnöl svomm knupp<br />

fjutt kril burr krasp mollo skrak<br />

krumpig vr<strong>in</strong>lig trossk prusskig schjaflig dank<br />

fnölp tirori rark fnuskig splass n<strong>in</strong><br />

- slio furn kln splurr klik<br />

loup* 6 evans* hram* srrats* paupe* kovo*<br />

knork vrom dramm frok blu tlak<br />

When it comes to the production of forms, we are not restricted to <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

clusters; the subjects have also used vowel qualities, reduplication and<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al clusters to express the different mean<strong>in</strong>gs. However, <strong>in</strong>itial clusters<br />

are much more common <strong>in</strong> these neologisms. An analysis of the<br />

neologisms produced follows below.<br />

Semantic feature 'pejorative'<br />

For the mean<strong>in</strong>g 'silly' (semantic feature: 'pejorative') the produced<br />

<strong>in</strong>itial clusters <strong>in</strong> the test are, <strong>in</strong> frequency order: fl- (3), fj- (2), and then<br />

one <strong>in</strong>stance each of pj-, fn-, kn-, kr-, sm- and spj-. These results can<br />

be compared with diagrams 4.6 and 4.7 where we see that pj- fn- and fjare<br />

very frequent percentally, while kn- and kr- are very frequent <strong>in</strong><br />

absolute numbers. Thus both percental and absolute frequency of<br />

phonesthemes <strong>in</strong> the lexical analysis of chapter 4 correlate with the<br />

6 The * marks the answers from the subject who deviated most <strong>in</strong> the "match<strong>in</strong>g test of<br />

nonsense words and mean<strong>in</strong>gs" (see 7.1.5).<br />

233


clusters that the subjects use <strong>in</strong> free production of sound symbolic words.<br />

The most frequently produced cluster fl- is quite frequent <strong>in</strong> absolute<br />

numbers but not so frequent percentally. Only one of the produced words<br />

conta<strong>in</strong>s a cluster which is not pejorative at all, namely spj-.<br />

The f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters produced are, <strong>in</strong> frequency order: -rk (2),<br />

and one <strong>in</strong>stance each of -mp and -lp. These clusters can be compared<br />

with tables 5.2 and 5.3 and diagrams 5.1 and 5.2. The tables show that -<br />

mp can be pejorative (but is usually not), and that -rk and -lp are not<br />

sound symbolic at all. The conclusion here must be that the f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

consonant clusters are of less importance than the <strong>in</strong>itial ones <strong>in</strong><br />

produc<strong>in</strong>g new pejorative words.<br />

Semantic feature 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form'<br />

For the semantic feature 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form', the produced <strong>in</strong>itial clusters <strong>in</strong><br />

the test are, with two <strong>in</strong>stances each: kr-, vr-, sl- pl- and sjl-. These can<br />

be compared with table 4.16. The clusters that have the feature 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g<br />

form' are: kr- (typical) and sn- (possible). In other words, kr- is<br />

expected. The cluster sjl- breaks the phonotactic pattern.<br />

The f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters produced are, with one <strong>in</strong>stance each: -rv, -<br />

ln and -ns. Of these three, -rv and -ns can be sound symbolic accord<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to the analysis <strong>in</strong> chapter 5 (cf. tables 5.2 and 5.3). There are however, <strong>in</strong><br />

this analysis, no f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters with the feature 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form'<br />

(only one word kr<strong>in</strong>gla).<br />

Instead, there seems to be an iconicity <strong>in</strong> any position <strong>in</strong> the word for the<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form' reflected e.g. <strong>in</strong> a contrast between s and l, <strong>in</strong><br />

the words slirv, islig, sjl<strong>in</strong>g, siloln, slio. As seen earlier (cf. for<br />

example diagram 4.1), sl- is also the most sound symbolic cluster <strong>in</strong><br />

absolute numbers. This issue is left for further research.<br />

The vowels are ma<strong>in</strong>ly i, and there is a dom<strong>in</strong>ance for front, closed or<br />

half closed vowels (with high F2).<br />

234


Semantic feature 'bad mood'<br />

For the mean<strong>in</strong>g 'angry' (semantic feature: 'bad mood') the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters<br />

produced <strong>in</strong> the test are, <strong>in</strong> order of frequency: vr- (2) and one <strong>in</strong>stance<br />

of each of gr-, kr-, tr-, dr- and hr-. The most frequent ones, vr-, as trand<br />

gr- (and only these clusters) also have the semantic feature 'bad<br />

mood' accord<strong>in</strong>g to the earlier analysis. All the produced clusters except<br />

one have an r, which is what the conventional ones have <strong>in</strong> common. The<br />

last one, hr- breaks the phonotactic pattern. The phonestheme 'bad mood'<br />

seems to have the expression obstruent + r.<br />

The f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters produced are: -rn (2) (it is unclear if the<br />

subjects pronounced this as a cluster or as a retroflex) and one <strong>in</strong>stance<br />

each of -rp, -rk, and -ssk. Obviously r dom<strong>in</strong>ates also <strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters, the only exception be<strong>in</strong>g -ssk. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to the analysis <strong>in</strong><br />

chapter 5, there are no conventional f<strong>in</strong>al clusters with the semantic<br />

feature 'bad mood', so the <strong>in</strong>itial cluster phonemes seem to have been<br />

transferred to the f<strong>in</strong>al position.<br />

There are no front, closed or half closed vowels <strong>in</strong> these words but there<br />

is a preference for vowels with lower F2.<br />

Semantic feature 'dryness'<br />

For the semantic feature 'dryness' the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters produced <strong>in</strong> the test<br />

are, <strong>in</strong> order of frequency: fn- (4), kr- (3) and one <strong>in</strong>stance each of fr-,<br />

pr-, spr-, srr-, kln (a whole word). The most frequent one fn- has the<br />

semantic feature 'dryness' accord<strong>in</strong>g to the earlier analysis, and fn- is the<br />

only cluster with the feature 'dryness'. The rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g ones consequently<br />

do not have the feature 'dryness'; srr- and kln break the phonotactic<br />

pattern.<br />

The f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters produced are: -sp, -sk, -rv, -ts and -dt.<br />

'Dryness' is not a feature of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters accord<strong>in</strong>g to the analysis of<br />

chapter 5. The cluster -dt breaks the phonotactics. Aga<strong>in</strong>, the subjects<br />

seem to encode the semantic feature <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itial cluster.<br />

235


For 'dryness' there are more clusters that break the phonotactic pattern<br />

than for the other semantic features. Perhaps this has to do with the fact<br />

that 'dryness' is a very <strong>in</strong>frequent sound symbolic feature, e.g. <strong>in</strong><br />

comparison with wetness.<br />

Semantic feature 'wetness'<br />

For the semantic feature 'wetness', the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters produced <strong>in</strong> the test<br />

are, <strong>in</strong> order of frequency: sl- (2), sv- (2), spl- (2) and one <strong>in</strong>stance each<br />

of pl-, tr-, schj- [Sj], bl- and bw-. The clusters with the feature<br />

'wetness' are two of the most common ones, sl- and sv- (but not spl-).<br />

The rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g ones, pl- and bl-, also have the feature 'wetness', but not<br />

tr-. Schj- [Sj] breaks the phonotactic pattern and bw- has the sound [w]<br />

which is not even a phoneme <strong>in</strong> Swedish. So, for this feature, four<br />

expected clusters are used, two unexpected ones and two unconventional.<br />

The f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters produced are: -bl, -sk, -rp, -sl(i) and -fl.<br />

Of these, the only f<strong>in</strong>al clusters for 'wetness', accord<strong>in</strong>g to the analysis of<br />

chapter 5, is -sk. -bl is not phonotactically possible <strong>in</strong> the absolute f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

position.<br />

In both the <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al clusters produced for 'wetness' the most<br />

common phonemes are the follow<strong>in</strong>g: l (6 <strong>in</strong>stances), s (5 <strong>in</strong>stances) and<br />

p (4 <strong>in</strong>stances), as could be expected, cf. diagrams 4.14 and 4.15.<br />

Semantic feature 'rough surface structure'<br />

For the semantic feature 'rough surface structure', the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters<br />

produced <strong>in</strong> the test are: fl- (2) and one <strong>in</strong>stance each of pr-, skr-, kn-,<br />

kl-, tl-. Of these only skr- has the feature 'rough surface structure'. Tlbreaks<br />

the phonotactic pattern. There are other cluster with this feature,<br />

fr- and kr-, accord<strong>in</strong>g to the analysis <strong>in</strong> chapter 4, but they were not used<br />

by the subjects. Perhaps the formulation <strong>in</strong> Swedish : 'tydlig, hård<br />

ytstruktur' was not clear enough.<br />

The f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters produced are: -Nk (2), -rb and -lb. In the<br />

analysis of chapter 5, the only roots classified as 'rough surface structure'<br />

236


are knottra (ttr) and rispa-raspa (sp). Rough surface structure is hardly a<br />

feature of f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters.<br />

Discussion of free production tests from mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to constructed word<br />

Subjects seem to encode semantic features <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters rather<br />

than <strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al ones when produc<strong>in</strong>g new sound symbolic words. This is<br />

mirrored <strong>in</strong> the results of chapter 5: many of the semantic features are<br />

not conventionally encoded <strong>in</strong> specific f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters. However,<br />

subjects do produce f<strong>in</strong>al clusters <strong>in</strong> these words, but these f<strong>in</strong>al clusters<br />

seem to be either mirrored <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters or are haphazard. There<br />

is of course also the possibility that semantic features other than those <strong>in</strong><br />

the study are relevant for f<strong>in</strong>al clusters.<br />

Subjects do produce clusters that break phonotactics, both <strong>in</strong>itially and<br />

f<strong>in</strong>ally. For sound symbolically low frequent features (like 'dryness'),<br />

with few possible clusters, there are more unconventional forms<br />

produced.<br />

The semantic features that to the greatest extent were expressed accord<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to the model were 'pejorative' (163 7), 'bad mood' (18) and 'wetness'<br />

(63). Less successfully expressed were 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form' (20), 'dryness' (1)<br />

and 'rough surface' (10). There is a tendency for frequent features to be<br />

more successfully coded. The <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters of 'pejorative',<br />

with the exception of one, are accord<strong>in</strong>g to expectations. The most<br />

commonly produced <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters for 'bad mood' are <strong>in</strong><br />

accordance with the phonesthemes vr-, tr-, gr-. Of the rest of the words<br />

produced all except one have an r. For the <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters of<br />

'wetness' the most common phonemes are: l, s and p. The feature<br />

'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form' stands out <strong>in</strong> that it uses the non-expected phonemes s and<br />

l <strong>in</strong> different positions of the words. This might be an effect of the<br />

trigger word 'sl<strong>in</strong>grig form' ('w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form').<br />

7 The absolute frequencies of these features, for the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters, are shown <strong>in</strong> table 4.2.<br />

237


A match<strong>in</strong>g test with context<br />

Yet another study was done <strong>in</strong> order to shed further light on the results<br />

presented <strong>in</strong> table 7.8. A match<strong>in</strong>g test between the mean<strong>in</strong>gs and all<br />

columns of words of table 7.8 8 was performed on a group of 9 l<strong>in</strong>guistics<br />

students with Swedish as their first language. The subjects were told to<br />

match the six columns of expressions with a random list of the six<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

This test gave 100 % correct results. The results depend partly on the<br />

possibility of compar<strong>in</strong>g the words <strong>in</strong> the different columns. We get an<br />

effect from context. An isolated read<strong>in</strong>g of the columns might be<br />

somewhat more difficult, and might not yield the same degree of correct<br />

results. Even more difficult would be to read (or hear) one word at a<br />

time and suggest a mean<strong>in</strong>g. Another <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g test would be to ask<br />

people to pick out the word most suitable for every mean<strong>in</strong>g of each<br />

labeled column.<br />

7.1.5 Match<strong>in</strong>g test of nonsense words and<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

In this test (Test 3), pairs of nonsense words, e.g. fnottig-skvottig, were<br />

to be matched with pairs of contrast<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gs, e.g. 'wet' - 'dry'. The<br />

subjects were 13 of those <strong>in</strong> tests 1 and 2. This test gave the follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

results, for each question:<br />

Question 1 (skv- 'wetness', fn- 'dryness'): 92 % expected answers<br />

Question 2 (str- 'long th<strong>in</strong> form', kr- 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form'): 84% expected<br />

answers<br />

Question 3 (spr- 'separation', kn- 'putt<strong>in</strong>g together': 84% expected<br />

answers<br />

Question 4 (skr- 'rough surface structure', mj- 'soft consistency'): 100%<br />

expected answers<br />

Question 5 (fj- 'silly' (pejorative), vr- 'arg' (bad mood): 100% expected<br />

answers.<br />

8 One column of words is e.g. slirv, islig, plyr, ril, pis, sjl<strong>in</strong>g, siloln, krel, kril, vr<strong>in</strong>lig,<br />

tirori, slio, evans, vrom.<br />

238


In question 1, one person (out of 13) deviated, on questions 2 and 3 two<br />

persons deviated. The same person deviated on questions 1, 2 and 3. In<br />

other words, one person stands for 3 out of 5 unexpected answers. (This<br />

is the person who's answers are marked with an * <strong>in</strong> tables 7.8. and 7.7.)<br />

7.1.6 Summary of results of tests on <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs and sounds<br />

The forced choice tests<br />

The forced choice tests (1b and 2b) had very good results for 19 out of<br />

39 test questions <strong>in</strong> the test from mean<strong>in</strong>g to constructed words, and 28<br />

test questions showed a majority for expected answers. In the test from<br />

constructed words to mean<strong>in</strong>g (1a and 2a) 14 out of 38 test questions<br />

showed very good results, and 29 test questions showed a majority for<br />

expected answers. This is an overall good result for the <strong>in</strong>terpretation of<br />

the hypothesized phonesthemes. The phonesthemes that were most<br />

successfully <strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> the test from mean<strong>in</strong>g to constructed words<br />

were fj- 'pejorative', fn- 'dryness', str- 'long th<strong>in</strong> form', sp- 'wetness',<br />

vr- 'bad mood', and pj- 'pejorative'. The phonesthemes that were most<br />

successfully <strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> the test from constructed words to mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

were gr- hollow form', fn- 'dryness', sn- 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' and pj- 'pejorative'.<br />

Comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the results of both tests, pj- 'pejorative' and fn- 'dryness'<br />

were the most successful.<br />

The question arises whether the <strong>in</strong>terpretation of semantic features <strong>in</strong><br />

general correlate with the most frequent features accord<strong>in</strong>g to the lexical<br />

analysis. The features 'pejorative': pj- and 'dryness': fn- (both lexically<br />

small clusters with a high degree of sound symbolism) showed the highest<br />

expected correlations between mean<strong>in</strong>g and sound among the six most<br />

successful features for both forced choice mean<strong>in</strong>g to constructed word<br />

and constructed word to mean<strong>in</strong>g tests (tables 7.2 and 7.5). It seems then<br />

that it is generally easier to <strong>in</strong>terpret and code phonesthemes of lexically<br />

low frequent, highly sound symbolic clusters. The mean<strong>in</strong>g that gave the<br />

smallest number of expected answers was 'light', (also for <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sounds). There were five test words with the expected feature 'light' and<br />

the clusters tested were bl-, gl- and gn-. Even when the vowel was <strong>in</strong><br />

accordance with the expectations of the lexical analysis, i.e. hav<strong>in</strong>g an i or<br />

239


e vowel like <strong>in</strong> gletig and gnitig, the words were not <strong>in</strong>terpreted as 'light'<br />

by a majority of speakers, and the category 'light' was not coded with bl-<br />

, gl- or gn- by a majority of speakers. , To quite a great extent, gn- is a<br />

lexically low frequent, highly sound symbolic cluster, as is the case with<br />

bl- and gl. However, this does not seem to be enough for productivity.<br />

As mentioned above, the 'light' phonesthemes (which are not productive 9)<br />

have the only semantic feature which is metaphorically connected with<br />

sound.<br />

The free production tests<br />

The free production of mean<strong>in</strong>gs or words tests (1a and 2a) gave a<br />

variety of mean<strong>in</strong>gs and new sound symbolic words, many of which were<br />

predicted but some were not. In the free production test from form to<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g no constructed word was <strong>in</strong>terpreted as expected by all subjects,<br />

but all of the constructed words (except one which is given a second best<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation) are <strong>in</strong>terpreted correctly by some of the subjects. The<br />

success rank<strong>in</strong>g of the six phonesthemes are, <strong>in</strong> order from best match to<br />

worst: 'pejorative' (for pj-), 'destruction' (for skr-), 'wetness' (for skv-<br />

), 'pejorative' (for fn-), 'bad mood' (for vr-), 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form' (for kr-).<br />

The features (of the six tested) that were most successful were<br />

'pejorative', 'bad mood', and 'wetness'. Also the fact that all the test<br />

subjects will<strong>in</strong>gly produced nonsense words, and mean<strong>in</strong>gs from nonsense<br />

words, is an <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g result. Thus, these nonsense words are <strong>in</strong>terpreted<br />

<strong>in</strong> accordance with expectations by some subjects, but not all. The<br />

nonsense words have a phonesthemic mean<strong>in</strong>g potential that is used by<br />

some subjects <strong>in</strong> the test situation.<br />

The free production test from mean<strong>in</strong>g to form shows that phonesthemes<br />

are used. This test also shows that for all semantic features both percental<br />

and absolute frequencies of the lexical analysis correlate with the clusters<br />

that the subjects use <strong>in</strong> free production of sound symbolic words. Subjects<br />

seem to encode semantic features <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters rather than <strong>in</strong> the<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al ones when produc<strong>in</strong>g new sound symbolic words. They also produce<br />

clusters that break phonotactic rules, both <strong>in</strong>itially and f<strong>in</strong>ally. The<br />

9 There may well be productive 'light' phonesthemes, but it might be the case that the<br />

expression side of these phonesthemes has to <strong>in</strong>clude a vowel, e.g. i.<br />

240


semantic features that to the greatest extent were expressed accord<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

the model, were 'pejorative', 'bad mood' and 'wetness'. Less successfully<br />

identified were 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form', 'dryness' and 'rough surface'. There is a<br />

tendency for frequent features to be more successfully coded.<br />

For both free production tests, the most successful features (out of six<br />

possible) were 'pejorative', 'bad mood' and 'wetness'.<br />

Thus <strong>in</strong> both the forced choice tests and free production tests the most<br />

successful semantic feature is 'pejorative'.<br />

The match<strong>in</strong>g test<br />

The match<strong>in</strong>g test (c), where two words are to be matched with two<br />

semantic features, produced the highest percentages of expected answers.<br />

The results partly depend on the possibility to compare the word pairs.<br />

We get an effect from context.<br />

Summary and discussion<br />

Most of the phonesthemes are more or less successfully <strong>in</strong>terpreted or<br />

coded, while some are more clearly not <strong>in</strong>terpreted or coded. In table 7.9<br />

below, the phonesthemes that were the most successfully <strong>in</strong>terpreted and<br />

coded <strong>in</strong> the experiments are presented, <strong>in</strong> order to show which are<br />

recurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the different experiments.<br />

241


Table 7.9 The phonesthemes most successfully <strong>in</strong>terpreted or coded <strong>in</strong><br />

experiments with neologisms.<br />

Test:<br />

Phonestheme:<br />

most<br />

successfulphonesthemes<br />

leastsuccessfulphonesthemes<br />

forced<br />

choice<br />

from<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

phonol.<br />

form<br />

pejorative:<br />

fjdryness:fnlong<br />

th<strong>in</strong><br />

form:<br />

strwetness:spbad<br />

mood:<br />

vrpejorative:pj-<br />

light:<br />

gn-<br />

forced<br />

choice from<br />

phonol.<br />

form to<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

hollow form:<br />

grdryness:fntalk<strong>in</strong>g:snpejorative:pjquick<br />

or<br />

strong<br />

movement:<br />

skseparation:sprlight:gnlight:gl-<br />

242<br />

free<br />

production<br />

from<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

phonol.<br />

form<br />

pejorative<br />

(several<br />

clusters)<br />

bad mood<br />

(several<br />

clusters)<br />

wetness<br />

(several<br />

clusters)<br />

w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g<br />

form,<br />

dryness,<br />

rough<br />

surface<br />

free<br />

production<br />

from<br />

phonol.<br />

form to<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

pejorative:<br />

pj-<br />

w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g<br />

form:<br />

kr-<br />

As can be seen from table 7.9 the most successful phonestheme is<br />

'pejorative': pj- and the least successful is 'light': gn- and 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form'<br />

(kr-). Pj- and gn- are lexically low frequent, highly sound symbolic<br />

clusters.<br />

The clusters of most of the most successful phonesthemes <strong>in</strong> table 7.9<br />

above – fj-, fn-, vr-, pj-, spr-, skr- and mj- – are lexically low<br />

match<strong>in</strong>g test<br />

rough<br />

surface<br />

structure vs.<br />

soft<br />

consistency:<br />

skr- vs mjpejorative<br />

vs.<br />

bad mood:<br />

fj- vs. vr-<br />

-


frequent, very sound symbolic clusters. Str-, sp-, gr-, sn- and sk- are<br />

not. The most successful semantic feature of table 7.8 is 'pejorative'. The<br />

general success of the feature 'pejorative' is discussed <strong>in</strong> chapter 8. Of the<br />

least successful phonesthemes, gn- is lexically low frequent, highly sound<br />

symbolic, but not gl- and kr-. The degree of success <strong>in</strong> these tests is not<br />

then exclusively restricted to the lexically low frequent, highly sound<br />

symbolic clusters.<br />

243


8 Summary and discussion<br />

The purpose of this thesis has been to study different aspects of sound<br />

symbolism – with special reference to Swedish. The largest part of the<br />

descriptive study has been devoted to Swedish phonesthemes. Initial and<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters were primarily studied, but vowels were also<br />

<strong>in</strong>cluded.<br />

Other important issues have been: study<strong>in</strong>g productivity of new sound<br />

symbolic words and similarities between languages (connected with the<br />

issue of universals), and f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g a reasonable explanatory model for (a<br />

part of) sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> Swedish.<br />

The role of sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> language was discussed and a model for<br />

the position of sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> language was constructed, tak<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

number of factors <strong>in</strong>to account, such as <strong>in</strong>nateness, learn<strong>in</strong>g, productivity,<br />

context and conventionalization. (cf. 1.9).<br />

The sound symbolic properties of consonant clusters and vowels were<br />

described – the expressions and the mean<strong>in</strong>gs. The mean<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

phonesthemes were found to be relatable to each other and different<br />

explanations for the relationship between sound and mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> sound<br />

symbolism were also discussed, especially <strong>in</strong> relation to <strong>in</strong>dexicality and<br />

iconicity. A semantic model for expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the different semantic features<br />

of sound symbolism was constructed.<br />

With a po<strong>in</strong>t of departure <strong>in</strong> the description of Swedish phonesthemes a<br />

number of studies were made <strong>in</strong> order to <strong>in</strong>vestigate universality and,<br />

above all, the role of productivity.<br />

8.1 The research questions were as follows:<br />

1. What are the properties of sound symbolic sounds and sound sequences<br />

<strong>in</strong> Swedish? More specifically the questions are:<br />

Which <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters are used <strong>in</strong> sound<br />

symbolism?<br />

245


Which mean<strong>in</strong>gs are used <strong>in</strong> sound symbolism?<br />

How do these comb<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> phonesthemes?<br />

What are the sound symbolic characteristics of some vowels?<br />

How do <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al clusters and vowels comb<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> words?<br />

2. Are phonesthemes productive <strong>in</strong> Swedish? And, if so, are some<br />

phonesthemes more productive than others? Are neologisms created or<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> accordance with the semantic model of chapter 2 and the<br />

analysis of chapters 4 and 5?<br />

3. Are there similarities or dissimilarities between different languages <strong>in</strong><br />

some aspects of sound symbolism?<br />

4. Do non-Swedish speakers <strong>in</strong>terpret Swedish phonesthemes <strong>in</strong><br />

accordance with the semantic model of chapter 2 and the analysis of<br />

chapters 4 and 5?<br />

The results perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g to these questions are summarized and discussed<br />

below:<br />

8.1.1 Question 1<br />

What are the properties of sound symbolic sequences <strong>in</strong> Swedish?<br />

The lexical study of <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters h<strong>in</strong>ts at about 1,000 roots<br />

with sound symbolic beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gs of the 65,000 lexemes (or 8,300 root<br />

morphemes 1) <strong>in</strong> the Swedish vocabulary. The <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters<br />

vary <strong>in</strong> the degree to which they are sound symbolic and the same is true<br />

for the f<strong>in</strong>al clusters and the vowels. Almost all <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters<br />

and about 22% of the f<strong>in</strong>al clusters are used for sound symbolism. (From<br />

the experiments <strong>in</strong> chapter 7 it seems that the f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters are<br />

of less importance than the <strong>in</strong>itial ones <strong>in</strong> produc<strong>in</strong>g new words, see<br />

below.)<br />

1 This number of morphemes is the result of an <strong>in</strong>vestigation of a one million word<br />

newspaper corpus (NFO 4). It is most probable that newspaper language conta<strong>in</strong>s<br />

more roots than spoken language, but these figures are not known at present. The<br />

estimate of 65, 000 lexemes excludes transparent compounds.<br />

246


A restricted number of mean<strong>in</strong>gs that are semantically relatable to each<br />

other are used <strong>in</strong> Swedish phonesthemes. These are <strong>in</strong> most cases<br />

connected with perception or cognition (cf. the models <strong>in</strong> 1.9 and 2.6.2).<br />

A consonant cluster usually has more than one possible sound symbolic<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g, and the semantic profiles vary for different clusters.<br />

The most frequent semantic features for <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al clusters are<br />

partly the same and partly different. For the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters, the 10 most<br />

common features are, <strong>in</strong> descend<strong>in</strong>g order:<br />

Table 8.1 The 10 most common<br />

semantic features of <strong>in</strong>itial clusters.<br />

'pejorative'<br />

'sound'<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> form'<br />

'quick or strong movement'<br />

'wetness'<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g'<br />

'light'<br />

'dim<strong>in</strong>utive'<br />

'round form'<br />

'walk<strong>in</strong>g'<br />

For the f<strong>in</strong>al clusters, the 10 most common semantic features of Svensk<br />

Baklängesordbok and the 6 most common features of Nusvensk<br />

Frekvensordbok 4 (NFO 4) are shown <strong>in</strong> table 8.2.<br />

When the most common semantic features of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters of Svensk<br />

Baklängesordbok and Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 4 are comb<strong>in</strong>ed, the<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g features are most frequent, <strong>in</strong> descend<strong>in</strong>g order: 'sound' and<br />

'pejorative', 'talk<strong>in</strong>g', 'quick or strong movement', 'long th<strong>in</strong><br />

form', 'wetness' and 'slang' ,'walk<strong>in</strong>g', 'round form', 'light'.<br />

247


The first six features are also the six most common features of <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

clusters, cf. table 8.1 (cf. also table 5.4).<br />

Table 8.2 The 10 most common semantic features of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters of<br />

Svensk Baklängesordbok and the six most common features of the<br />

Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 4.<br />

Svensk Baklängesordbok Nusvensk Frekvensordbok 4 2<br />

'quick or strong movement' 'pejorative'<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g' 'sound'<br />

'sound' 'long th<strong>in</strong> form'<br />

'pejorative' 'talk<strong>in</strong>g'<br />

'slang' 'wetness'<br />

'walk<strong>in</strong>g' 'quick or strong movement'<br />

'wetness'<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> form'<br />

'round form'<br />

'light'<br />

This can be compared with the features that were the most successfully<br />

coded and <strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> the experiments of chapter 7. In the experiments<br />

of free production tests from mean<strong>in</strong>g to constructed word, 'pejorative',<br />

'bad mood', and 'wetness' were the most successful.<br />

Of the six features listed above 'pejorative and 'wetness' were the most<br />

successful <strong>in</strong> the experiments of free production tests from mean<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

constructed word.<br />

In the forced choice tests the most successful semantic features were<br />

'pejorative' and 'dryness' ('sound', 'dim<strong>in</strong>utive', 'round form' and<br />

'walk<strong>in</strong>g' were not tested <strong>in</strong> the forced choice experiments, see 7.1).<br />

2 There are only six semantic features <strong>in</strong> this column s<strong>in</strong>ce they are the result of the<br />

analysis presented <strong>in</strong> table 5.18, which showed the most common clusters and their<br />

semantic features; other semantic features are therefore not very frequent <strong>in</strong> Nusvensk<br />

Frekvensordbok 4.<br />

248


Of the six features listed above, 'pejorative' was the most successful <strong>in</strong> the<br />

forced choice tests.<br />

The phonological characteristics of the most common sound symbolic<br />

clusters, <strong>in</strong>itially and f<strong>in</strong>ally and <strong>in</strong> particular for certa<strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>gs, were<br />

as follows:<br />

Initial clusters<br />

For <strong>in</strong>itial clusters, the most common ones (cf. table 4.8) are sl-<br />

(absolutely) and pj- (percentally). For the five most frequent mean<strong>in</strong>gs,<br />

<strong>in</strong> absolute numbers, there is a strong tendency for the consonant clusters<br />

to end with l. For the five most frequent mean<strong>in</strong>gs, percentally, there is a<br />

slight tendency that the consonant clusters end with j. The clusters end<strong>in</strong>g<br />

with j are lexically low frequent ones and are thus marked. This can<br />

make them more useful for sound symbolic functions, cf. 8.3.<br />

Consider<strong>in</strong>g specific semantic features, the results for the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters<br />

are: all two-consonant clusters conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a j-phoneme are 'pejorative'<br />

and they are so to quite a great extent. Look<strong>in</strong>g at absolute figures,<br />

clusters beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with s, especially sl-, sn-, skr- are dom<strong>in</strong>ant among<br />

the pejorative roots. The semantic feature 'bad mood' is ma<strong>in</strong>ly coded<br />

with the clusters gr-, vr- and tr- (but these clusters can have many other<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs). Furthermore 'light' is dom<strong>in</strong>ated by voiced plosives + l or n –<br />

bl-, gl-, gn- (dl-, bn-, dn- are not allowed <strong>in</strong> Swedish) – and 'wetness' is<br />

dom<strong>in</strong>ated by sl-, kl-, sp-, sm-, skv-, spr-, sn-, bl-, pl-, sv-, i.e. s or<br />

<strong>in</strong>itial unvoiced plosives are preferred (the only exception is bl-). The<br />

feature 'long th<strong>in</strong> form' is dom<strong>in</strong>ated by three three-consonant clusters:<br />

spj-, str-, spr-. These all beg<strong>in</strong> with s (a fact of all Swedish <strong>in</strong>itial threeconsonant<br />

clusters). In addition, with the exception of two clusters<br />

connected with the feature 'long th<strong>in</strong> form', the above mentioned clusters<br />

plus sp-, sl-, sn-, st-, sk- and sv-, also beg<strong>in</strong> with an s. The only ones<br />

that do not beg<strong>in</strong> with s are tr- and tv-. These are however, voiceless<br />

dentals like s.<br />

249


Most clusters that are percentally most frequent are lexically very<br />

<strong>in</strong>frequent ones, cf. discussion <strong>in</strong> 8.3.<br />

The results of the lexical analysis of chapter 4 concern<strong>in</strong>g the pj-<br />

'pejorative' cluster, the gr-, tr-, vr- 'bad mood', the skr- 'destruction',<br />

the skv- 'wetness' and the fn- 'dryness' clusters correlate with the results<br />

of the free production or free <strong>in</strong>terpretation experiments of chapter 7 (cf.<br />

question 2 below).<br />

F<strong>in</strong>al clusters<br />

As discussed <strong>in</strong> the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of chapter 5, it is not self-evident which are<br />

the f<strong>in</strong>al clusters <strong>in</strong> Swedish. Morphological structure has to be taken <strong>in</strong>to<br />

account s<strong>in</strong>ce some sequences only occur <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>flected or derived forms<br />

(e.g. -ndsk <strong>in</strong> bondsk) and some sequences, which are obviously<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g, cannot occur <strong>in</strong> the absolutely f<strong>in</strong>al position (e. g. -dr- as <strong>in</strong><br />

fladdra. The mean<strong>in</strong>g profiles for the sound symbolic f<strong>in</strong>al clusters differ,<br />

as is also the case for <strong>in</strong>itial clusters, cf. diagrams 5.3–5.11.<br />

For the f<strong>in</strong>al clusters, the ma<strong>in</strong> results are as follows: The most common<br />

clusters with sound symbolic mean<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> order of frequency –<br />

<strong>in</strong>dependent of mean<strong>in</strong>g - are the follow<strong>in</strong>g (from NFO4): -Nk, -sk, -fs,<br />

-nd, -tr (absolutely) or -fs, -dr, -lr, -ml, -Nl (percentally) 3. Of these, 5<br />

of 9 end with a liquid. Of the 11 percentally most sound symbolic f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

consonant clusters, 8 end with a liquid and 3 with a voiceless obstruent (s<br />

or k). Of the 13 most common f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters <strong>in</strong> absolute<br />

numbers, 6 end with a liquid and 7 with a voiceless obstruent (s, t or k).<br />

The high frequency of liquids and voiceless obstruents among the f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters could be due to lexical dom<strong>in</strong>ance of these clusters. (Swedish f<strong>in</strong>al<br />

clusters can also end <strong>in</strong> voiced obstruents and nasals.) It is beyond the<br />

scope of this thesis to <strong>in</strong>vestigate this issue. The high sound symbolic<br />

frequency of liquids and voiceless obstruents could also have to do with<br />

the fact that they are especially suited for certa<strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>gs. As seen <strong>in</strong><br />

table 5.3, however, none of the two groups are especially tied to any<br />

3 The five most frequent f<strong>in</strong>al clusters have a percentage of 100% because there is only<br />

one – sound symbolic – root, for every cluster. These are not considered here, cf.<br />

however, diagram 5.2.<br />

250


semantic feature. In the NFO 4 material, -fs (pejorative) is the most<br />

common phonestheme both absolutely and percentally (cf. tables 5.18 and<br />

5.19).<br />

It is not the case, for f<strong>in</strong>al clusters, that the high frequent semantic<br />

features seem to prefer certa<strong>in</strong> sounds or sound comb<strong>in</strong>ations, cf. table<br />

5.3. The more low frequent semantic features, however, use certa<strong>in</strong><br />

consonant clusters: 'wetness' -ska; 'long th<strong>in</strong> form' -msa; 'round form' -<br />

lla; 'light' -<strong>in</strong>dra, -imra; 'shortwide form' -mpa. These clusters, except<br />

for -<strong>in</strong>dra, -imra ('light') are, as can be seen <strong>in</strong> table 5.3, used by other<br />

semantic features as well.<br />

Vowels<br />

Vowels, a selection of which were studied <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>imal pairs, seem to have<br />

other semantic characteristics than consonant clusters, often modify<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g of consonant clusters, e.g. be<strong>in</strong>g dim<strong>in</strong>utive or augmentative. [i]<br />

tends to have the mean<strong>in</strong>gs 'smallness', 'quickness', 'high pitch', while [P]<br />

seems to have the mean<strong>in</strong>gs of 'low pitch' and 'largeness'. [a] seems<br />

neutral. The vowel [P] is also connected with 'pejorative'. The semantic<br />

feature 'light' is connected with front vowels, especially with [i].<br />

Comb<strong>in</strong>ations<br />

The study of comb<strong>in</strong>ations of <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters showed<br />

that it is more common for semantic features to be the same <strong>in</strong>itially and<br />

f<strong>in</strong>ally, than to be different. If new semantic features had been searched<br />

for <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>vestigation of f<strong>in</strong>al clusters, there is a possibility that this<br />

result would have to be modified, because of the appearance of new roots.<br />

The semantic features under consideration are however, mostly the same<br />

<strong>in</strong>itially and f<strong>in</strong>ally.<br />

It seems then, that <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al clusters strengthen each other rather<br />

than contribut<strong>in</strong>g to different sound symbolic mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> the same root.<br />

However, for those consonant clusters that can occur both <strong>in</strong>itially and<br />

f<strong>in</strong>ally it is not the case that they are comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the same word, e.g. bland<br />

-bl, but neologisms as blabbla are conceivable.<br />

251


8.1.2 Question 2<br />

Are phonesthemes productive <strong>in</strong> Swedish? And, if so, are some<br />

phonesthemes more productive than others? Are neologisms created and<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> accordance with the semantic model of chapters 1 and 2 and<br />

the analysis of chapters 4 and 5? The model of 1.9 says, among other<br />

th<strong>in</strong>gs, that sound symbolism is productive. Furthermore, the model of<br />

2.6.2 claims that semantic features of sound symbolism are, due to the<br />

<strong>in</strong>nateness of categories of th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g - such as predispositions for see<strong>in</strong>g<br />

contiguity and similarity, and due to learn<strong>in</strong>g - relatable to sense<br />

impressions and emotions, restricted to certa<strong>in</strong> types.<br />

The characteristics of the consonant clusters (cf. above, question 1) are<br />

mirrored <strong>in</strong> the experiments of chapter 7 <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g way:<br />

Forced choice tests<br />

The phonesthemes, <strong>in</strong> the constructed words, that were the most<br />

successfully coded, i.e. given a phonological form based on a presented<br />

semantic feature, were 'pejorative': fj-, 'dryness': fn-, long th<strong>in</strong> form':<br />

str-, 'wetness': sp-, 'bad mood': vr-, 'pejorative': pj-. The features most<br />

successfully coded totally were 'wetness', 'hollow form', 'separation' and<br />

'dryness'. The six phonesthemes most successfully <strong>in</strong>terpreted were<br />

'hollow form': gr-, 'dryness': fn-, 'talk<strong>in</strong>g': sn-, 'pejorative': pj-, 'quick<br />

or strong movement': sk- and 'separation': spr-. For both the<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation and production tests, the most successful phonesthemes<br />

were 'dryness': fn- and 'pejorative': pj-. Both of these are (lexically low<br />

frequent) clusters that are sound symbolic to a very high degree. A<br />

possible explanation for the success of these phonesthemes is that they can<br />

be more easily accessed. Phonesthemes could be stored <strong>in</strong> a way different<br />

from other morphemes s<strong>in</strong>ce there is a motivated connection between<br />

sound and mean<strong>in</strong>g. This could make them more reliable and more<br />

frequent <strong>in</strong> production and <strong>in</strong>terpretation.<br />

The 'light' words stand out because of many <strong>in</strong>stances of bad results, both<br />

<strong>in</strong> the word-to-mean<strong>in</strong>g and mean<strong>in</strong>g-to-word tests. It is clear that the<br />

'light' category gets the least number of expected answers. The 'light'<br />

words might belong to a closed lexical class which would imply that gl-,<br />

252


gn-, bl-, are not productive, unlike phonesthemes such as kl- or pj-.<br />

Even when the 'light' words had a vowel with high F2, as many of the<br />

'light' words have accord<strong>in</strong>g to 5.4, the results of the experiment were no<br />

better. The semantic feature 'light' is positioned quite high <strong>in</strong> the analysis<br />

of the most common features (cf. diagram 4.5).<br />

Free production tests<br />

Phonesthemes<br />

In the free production test from expression to mean<strong>in</strong>g, the percentally<br />

most common phonestheme pj- (pejorative) was the most accurately<br />

identified. In the free production test from mean<strong>in</strong>g to expression the jclusters,<br />

among others, for 'pejorative' were produced as expected. The<br />

r-clusters for 'bad mood' were also produced as expected, plus some<br />

additional r-clusters, both <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al. For the feature 'wetness',<br />

there was a majority of s-, l- and p-clusters (as expected), both <strong>in</strong>itially<br />

and f<strong>in</strong>ally.<br />

Explanations of these results, for the j-clusters, could have to do with the<br />

facts that these are (lexically low frequent) clusters that are sound<br />

symbolic to a very high degree; they could thus be more easily accessed<br />

and this could, <strong>in</strong> turn, make them more reliable – and frequent – <strong>in</strong><br />

production and <strong>in</strong>terpretation. As concerns the r-clusters for 'bad mood'<br />

they can have their base <strong>in</strong> expressions which are spontaneous<br />

vocalizations <strong>in</strong> connection with an angry feel<strong>in</strong>g, and the s-, l- and pclusters<br />

for 'wetness' have their base <strong>in</strong> sound imitation. The question of<br />

whether one of these bases gives better results than the other <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation and production cannot be answered from these experiments<br />

alone; further experimentation is needed.<br />

The phonestheme most difficult to <strong>in</strong>terpret was 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form': kr-.<br />

Semantic features<br />

The semantic features of the free production mean<strong>in</strong>g to word<br />

experiments that were most successfully expressed (<strong>in</strong> accordance with the<br />

lexical analysis) were 'pejorative' (163), 'bad mood' (18) and 'wetness'<br />

(63). Less successfully expressed were 'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form' (20), 'dryness' (1)<br />

253


and 'rough surface' (10) (The absolute frequencies of these semantic<br />

features, for the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters, are shown with<strong>in</strong> the parenthesis, cf. table<br />

4.2.) These results can be compared with the most frequent semantic<br />

features accord<strong>in</strong>g to the lexical analysis summarized <strong>in</strong> tables 8.1 and<br />

8.2. Of the three most successful features – 'pejorative', 'bad mood' and<br />

'wetness' – of the free production experiments, 'pejorative' and 'wetness'<br />

are among the six most common features accord<strong>in</strong>g to the lexical analysis.<br />

A probable explanation for these similarities is that phonesthemes of the<br />

most frequent semantic features are stored <strong>in</strong> such a way that they are<br />

more accurately accessed by the language user. The way they are stored is<br />

dependent on stable <strong>in</strong>termodal connections, cf. the discussion <strong>in</strong> 1.11.2.<br />

Of the three less successful features of the free production experiments -<br />

'w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form', 'dryness' and 'rough surface' - none belonged to the 10<br />

most common semantic features of the lexical analysis. A probable<br />

explanation for this is that the phonesthemes of the less frequent semantic<br />

features are stored <strong>in</strong> such a way that they are less accurately accessed.<br />

The conclusion is that phonesthemes are – more or less – productive, both<br />

<strong>in</strong> production of new forms and understand<strong>in</strong>g of neologisms. There is a<br />

tendency for the most common semantic features to be more successfully<br />

coded, <strong>in</strong> accordance with the ma<strong>in</strong> results of the analysis of<br />

phonesthemes <strong>in</strong> chapter 4; these might be more accessible. It could also<br />

be the case that categories related to negative emotions (e.g. 'pejorative',<br />

'bad mood') are more important to humans (at least Swedes) than more<br />

abstract categories like 'form'. It also seems that subjects tend to encode<br />

semantic features <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itial clusters rather than <strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al ones <strong>in</strong> free<br />

production, and it seems that for low frequent features (like 'dryness'),<br />

with few possible clusters, there are a larger number of unconventional<br />

forms produced. These tendencies need to be <strong>in</strong>vestigated further.<br />

An <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g result from the free choice test from expression to<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g is that the mean<strong>in</strong>gs produced all belong to the classes found <strong>in</strong><br />

the lexical study. Even the <strong>in</strong>formants who started the test by freely<br />

suggest<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gs based on constructed neologisms (and therefore had<br />

no expectancies as to what the answers ought to be) produced mean<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

254


with<strong>in</strong> these classes (although not always with<strong>in</strong> the expected class for a<br />

certa<strong>in</strong> nonsense word - the reason for this might be <strong>in</strong>dividual contextual<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluences at the moment of the test). These classes are: 'pejorative' (often<br />

'destruction'), 'mental feel<strong>in</strong>g' (often irritated), 'surface structure',<br />

'wetness', 'form', 'consistency', 'movement', 'dim<strong>in</strong>utive', 'sound'. These<br />

results can also be compared with the most frequent semantic features<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to the lexical analysis summarized <strong>in</strong> table 8.1. Six of the n<strong>in</strong>e<br />

general semantic features result<strong>in</strong>g from the free choice test from<br />

expression to mean<strong>in</strong>g ('pejorative', 'wetness', 'form', 'movement',<br />

'dim<strong>in</strong>utive' and 'sound') are among (or superord<strong>in</strong>ate to) the n<strong>in</strong>e most<br />

common semantic features of table 8.1. This supports the model of 2.6.2<br />

where the phonesthemes for these categories are seen as a result of<br />

<strong>in</strong>nateness, learn<strong>in</strong>g and conventionalization; the semantic categories of<br />

phonesthemes are predictable rather than haphazard. This model shows<br />

that <strong>in</strong> many cases the semantic features of phonesthemes are potentially<br />

relatable to neurological connections between the senses, cf. 8.3.2. It<br />

could also imply that phonesthemes that concern the most frequent<br />

semantic features are stored <strong>in</strong> such a way that they are most readily and<br />

accurately accessed 4.<br />

8.1.3 Question 3<br />

Are there similarities or dissimilarities between different languages <strong>in</strong><br />

various aspects of sound symbolism,?<br />

In the Thesaurus study of the concepts 'stupidity', 'rough surface<br />

structure', and 'smooth surface structure', for English and Swedish, the<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g was found: The phonological agreement between words<br />

belong<strong>in</strong>g to these semantic fields <strong>in</strong> English and Swedish was greater<br />

among the words for 'surface structure' than for the words for<br />

'pejoratives'. One obvious reason is that some of the clusters used <strong>in</strong><br />

Swedish (fj-, fn-, pj-) are not allowed <strong>in</strong> English. Another reason could<br />

be that 'surface structure' is closer to a potentially common phenomenon,<br />

namely sound imitation, s<strong>in</strong>ce strok<strong>in</strong>g different surfaces give different<br />

4 This is a question for further experiments, e.g. lexical decision experiments, and<br />

development of on l<strong>in</strong>e models.<br />

255


sound effects. Of course, Swedish and English have many cognates but not<br />

<strong>in</strong> these results (cf. 6.2), except for some cases.<br />

In the tests with <strong>in</strong>formants concern<strong>in</strong>g different <strong>in</strong>terjections <strong>in</strong> 8<br />

languages the follow<strong>in</strong>g similarities and dissimilarities were found<br />

concern<strong>in</strong>g expressions: The pejorative <strong>in</strong>terjections often conta<strong>in</strong> an u or<br />

an O, the positive <strong>in</strong>terjections an a, 'surprise' often an a or an O. 'Pa<strong>in</strong>'<br />

(e.g. Swedish aj) has a diphtongized open vowel, 'sneeze' (e.g. Swedish<br />

atjo) has an affricate and the <strong>in</strong>terjection for go away (to an animal) (e.g.<br />

Swedish schas) has a fricative <strong>in</strong> all the 8 languages.<br />

In the tests with <strong>in</strong>formants of 16 different languages concern<strong>in</strong>g different<br />

expressions that are imitative of animal noises, the ma<strong>in</strong> results are the<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g: No animal call imitation is exactly the same <strong>in</strong> all the 16<br />

languages. Some animal call imitations are more alike, e.g. the cat's<br />

meow, while others vary more, e.g. the dog's bark<strong>in</strong>g. One reason for<br />

this could be that some animal cries are more complicated than others.<br />

There are, however, always similarities on the level of phonetic features<br />

between the expressions for the same animal call <strong>in</strong> the different<br />

languages. With<strong>in</strong> each language there are, as well as for the expressive<br />

<strong>in</strong>terjections, similarities which can be assumed to depend on the<br />

phonological structure of the language <strong>in</strong> question.<br />

In the identification test, expressions for different animal sounds by<br />

speakers of 9 different languages were tested on 15 persons with 6<br />

different first languages. The ma<strong>in</strong> results are that some animals were<br />

identified by all listeners (e.g. dog, cow and cat), some less correctly<br />

identified and one animal not identified at all (e.g. duck, Hungarian<br />

expression). The <strong>in</strong>terpretation test shows that, given the context of<br />

animal calls, it was quite easy for listeners to <strong>in</strong>terpret animal calls from<br />

languages other than their own (cf. table 6.6).<br />

Thus, for these contrastive studies, the follow<strong>in</strong>g general conclusion can<br />

be drawn. There are both similarities and dissimilarities between the<br />

256


expressions <strong>in</strong> the different languages 5. This is true for most phenomena<br />

<strong>in</strong> language, and sound symbolism is no exception here. The variation is<br />

greater for some semantic fields than for others; expressions for rough or<br />

smooth surface structure are more alike than expressions for pejoratives.<br />

A possible explanation of this is that 'surface structure' is closer to a<br />

potentially common phenomenon, namely sound imitation, either because<br />

strok<strong>in</strong>g different surfaces give different sound effects or because of the<br />

auditory-tactile neural connection. Certa<strong>in</strong> animals are imitated more<br />

consistently than others, possibly because certa<strong>in</strong> animal cries are shorter<br />

and less complicated than others, cf. e.g. the cries of the cat and the<br />

rooster.<br />

8.1.4 Question 4<br />

Do non-Swedish speakers <strong>in</strong>terpret Swedish phonesthemes <strong>in</strong> accordance<br />

with the semantic model of chapter 2 and the analysis of chapters 4 and 5?<br />

In the test of understand<strong>in</strong>g Swedish sound symbolic words (and some<br />

neologisms constructed from phonesthemes) the general results are as<br />

follows: The subjects seldom <strong>in</strong>terpreted the words correctly. However,<br />

the answers given almost all belong to categories with<strong>in</strong> the semantic<br />

model of 2.6.2 (cf. table 6.7). It therefore seems that for certa<strong>in</strong><br />

categories, often related to sense impressions or emotions, there is a<br />

preference for sound symbolic expressions; it is more natural for<br />

speakers to imag<strong>in</strong>e that an unknown word, where the expression is<br />

supposed to reflect the mean<strong>in</strong>g, means e.g. 'quick or strong movement'<br />

than that it means e.g. 'my country'.<br />

The conclusion from the studies under questions 3 and 4 is that there are<br />

greater differences between these languages 6 for expression than for<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g. The expressions imitat<strong>in</strong>g animal sounds are easier to <strong>in</strong>terpret<br />

than other sound symbolism for speakers of different languages.<br />

5 For crossl<strong>in</strong>guistic studies <strong>in</strong> other semantic fields, see e.g. Viberg (1999).<br />

6 Swedish, English, Icelandic, Polish, Hungarian, F<strong>in</strong>nish, Ososo, Malagasi,<br />

Slovenian, Korean, Japanese, Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, Estonian, Urdu, Persian, Kurdish, Arabic and<br />

Spanish. Persons who <strong>in</strong>terpreted Swedish sound symbolic words spoke Arabic,<br />

Spanish, German, Dutch, Ibo, English.<br />

257


Interpretation of other sound symbolism often goes wrong (because<br />

expressions probably differ <strong>in</strong> different languages), but the semantic<br />

categories guessed at are most often with<strong>in</strong> the semantic models of this<br />

thesis.<br />

8.2. Comparison of the studies<br />

The next table (8.3) is a comparison between the most frequent<br />

phonesthemes of the lexical studies with the most accurately <strong>in</strong>terpreted<br />

and coded phonesthemes of the experimental studies. The most common<br />

clusters and the most common mean<strong>in</strong>gs are also shown. The data are<br />

from different tables and diagrams as <strong>in</strong>dicated <strong>in</strong> table 8.3.<br />

Table 8.3 The most frequent 7 phonesthemes or the most successful 8<br />

phonesthemes (their consonant clusters and mean<strong>in</strong>gs), whichever is most<br />

appropriate, from the different studies. The examples are presented <strong>in</strong><br />

frequency order. In some cases, however, consecutive examples might<br />

have the same frequency. 9<br />

7 Accord<strong>in</strong>g to the lexical analysis.<br />

8 In the case of the experiments where subjects produced or <strong>in</strong>terpreted neologisms.<br />

9 Squares are empty for the experiments where there are no relevant data.<br />

258


lexically <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

consonant<br />

clusters<br />

experimentally<br />

f<strong>in</strong>al consonant<br />

clusters<br />

forced choice 14<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

expression<br />

phonesthemes consonant<br />

clusters<br />

pejorative:<br />

sl-, pjsound:<br />

kl-, kn-, fnlong<br />

th<strong>in</strong> form:<br />

sp-, spjquick<br />

or strong<br />

movement:<br />

flwetness:<br />

sl-, skv-10 quick or strong<br />

movement: -NNl,<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g: -tr,<br />

sound: -st,<br />

pejorative: -fs,<br />

wetness: -sk,<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

-Nk11 fj-: pejorative<br />

fn-: dryness<br />

str-: long th<strong>in</strong><br />

form<br />

sp-: wetness<br />

vr-:bad mood<br />

pj-<br />

:pejorative15 259<br />

<strong>in</strong> absolute<br />

numbers:<br />

sl-, sn-, kn-,<br />

kr-, kl-, sp-,<br />

gl-, st-, tr-<br />

(cf. diagram<br />

4.1)<br />

<strong>in</strong> %:<br />

fn-, gn-, skv-<br />

, pj-, kn-,<br />

spr-, spj-, gl-<br />

, mj-, vr- (cf.<br />

diagram 4.4)<br />

-Nk, -sk,-fs, -<br />

nd, -tr (cf.<br />

diagram 5.1<br />

and table 5.21)<br />

-dr, -lr, -ml,<br />

-NNl 12<br />

10From table 4.8.<br />

11 From tables 5.3 and 5.18.<br />

12 From NFO4, diagram 5.2 (except for the 1 root clusters).<br />

13 From NFO4 and Svensk Baklängesordbok, section 5.5.3.<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

pejorative,<br />

sound, long<br />

th<strong>in</strong> form,<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement,<br />

wetness,<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g, light,<br />

dim<strong>in</strong>utive,<br />

round form,<br />

walk<strong>in</strong>g (cf.<br />

diagram 4.5)<br />

sound,<br />

pejorative,<br />

talk<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement,<br />

long th<strong>in</strong> form,<br />

wetness13 wetness,<br />

hollow form<br />

separation<br />

dryness,<br />

pejorative (cf.<br />

table 7.3)


cross<br />

cultural<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation<br />

forced choice<br />

expression to<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g16 free production<br />

expression to<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g<br />

free production<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

expression<br />

gr-: hollow<br />

form<br />

fn-: dryness<br />

sn-: talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

pj-: pejorative<br />

sk-: quick or<br />

strong<br />

movement<br />

spr-:<br />

separation17 pj-: pejorative<br />

skr-:<br />

destruction<br />

skv-:<br />

wetness18 pejorative: fl-,<br />

fjbad<br />

mood: rclusters<br />

wetness: sl-,<br />

sv-19 fl20-: quick or<br />

strong<br />

movement<br />

skr- :<br />

destruction<br />

tr-: bad mood<br />

260<br />

quick or strong<br />

movement21 14 The semantic feature 'sound' was not tested.<br />

15 From table 7.2.<br />

16 For both the production and <strong>in</strong>terpretation experiments added together, the most<br />

successful phonesthemes were fn-: 'dryness' and pj-: 'pejorative'<br />

17 From table 7.5.<br />

18 From table 7.7.<br />

19 From table 7.8.<br />

20Phonesthemes of words that were most successfully <strong>in</strong>terpreted accord<strong>in</strong>g to the<br />

Swedish norm. Cf. table 6.8.<br />

21 Most preferred mean<strong>in</strong>g for unconventional but possible mean<strong>in</strong>gs. 'Pejorative' and<br />

'sound' were NOT among the more preferred mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Cf. table 6.9.


Table 8.3 shows, among other th<strong>in</strong>gs, the follow<strong>in</strong>g relations between<br />

different studies: The most successful <strong>in</strong>itial phonesthemes of the<br />

experimental studies that correspond to <strong>in</strong>itial phonesthemes of the most<br />

frequent semantic features are: pj-: pejorative (<strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters,<br />

forced choice mean<strong>in</strong>g to expression, forced choice expression to<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g, free production expression to mean<strong>in</strong>g); skv-:wetness (<strong>in</strong>itials,<br />

free production expression to mean<strong>in</strong>g); sl-:wetness (<strong>in</strong>itials, free<br />

production mean<strong>in</strong>g to expression) <strong>in</strong> this most condensed version of the<br />

results.<br />

Sl- is the percentally largest cluster for 'wetness' (and THE most sound<br />

symbolic cluster – for all semantic features – <strong>in</strong> absolute figures), pj- is<br />

the percentally largest 'pejorative' cluster and skv- is the percentally fifth<br />

largest wetness cluster 22. Pj- and skv- are both small but percentally<br />

highly sound symbolic clusters.<br />

An explanation for these results is that phonesthemes that are<br />

proportionally large (i. e. where a certa<strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g has a proportionally<br />

large part of the (sound symbolic) roots of a cluster) are stored <strong>in</strong> such a<br />

way that they are more accurately accessed by the language user.<br />

However, all phonesthemes of the free production experimental results<br />

above match the lexical analysis. A possible explanation for most of these<br />

similarities could be that the phonesthemes of semantic features are<br />

relatable to neurological connections between the senses (<strong>in</strong> accordance<br />

with the model of 2.6.2).<br />

The most successful phonesthemes (which were <strong>in</strong> fact not very<br />

successful, cf. table 6.8) of the cross cultural <strong>in</strong>terpretation experiment do<br />

not reflect the frequent Swedish semantic feature 'pejorative'. An<br />

explanation for the lack of success of this feature could be that the<br />

negative emotion that 'pejorative' is based on is a cultural trait of<br />

Swedish, but not of the other languages <strong>in</strong>vestigated here. However, of the<br />

phonesthemes that did succeed, one of the clusters, fl-, is the<br />

22 Cf. also table 4.8.<br />

261


proportionally most sound symbolic cluster for 'quick or strong<br />

movement', and the other two clusters skr- and tr- are the proportionally<br />

the most sound symbolic clusters for the semantic features 'destruction'<br />

and 'bad mood' respectively. These results are then <strong>in</strong> accordance with<br />

Swedish competence. One obvious explanation for the overall bad results<br />

is that non-native Swedes do not connect accurate expressions with the<br />

semantic features as a natural consequence of different phonological<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g environments. 23<br />

The vowels were not taken <strong>in</strong>to consideration <strong>in</strong> this table, s<strong>in</strong>ce they<br />

were studied <strong>in</strong> a different way, which <strong>in</strong>volved contrast<strong>in</strong>g some<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs and sounds <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>imal pairs.<br />

The cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic comparisons concern<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terjections and the<br />

expressions for animal cries are not comparable to the data <strong>in</strong> table 8.3,<br />

because they concern semantic fields other than the Swedish lexical study<br />

and the experiments. The Thesaurus study, compar<strong>in</strong>g English and<br />

Swedish, showed that, for the concepts 'stupidity', 'rough surface<br />

structure' and 'smooth surface structure', similar phonemes were used for<br />

the last two concepts, but not for the first. A possible explanation for this<br />

is that 'surface structure' is closer to a potentially simultaneous<br />

phenomenon, namely sound imitation, s<strong>in</strong>ce strok<strong>in</strong>g different surfaces<br />

give different sound effects. The comparison of different languages <strong>in</strong> the<br />

studies of <strong>in</strong>terjections showed certa<strong>in</strong> agreements <strong>in</strong> vowels. Most of the<br />

<strong>in</strong>terjections consist of vowels and s<strong>in</strong>gle consonants and are therefore not<br />

comparable with sound symbolism of <strong>in</strong>itial or f<strong>in</strong>al consonant clusters.<br />

For the animal sounds, there were great expressive similarities between<br />

the different languages. It was quite easy for listeners of different first<br />

languages to correctly identify animals from the way <strong>in</strong> which their cries<br />

were represented <strong>in</strong> different languages, cf. summary <strong>in</strong> 8.1.3.<br />

Compar<strong>in</strong>g these last two studies with the Thesaurus study (only <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the related languages Swedish and English but show<strong>in</strong>g great differences),<br />

23 However, as can be seen <strong>in</strong> table 6.9, subjects mostly guessed with<strong>in</strong> the expected<br />

semantic categories of the model. A potential explanation for these preferences has to<br />

do with neurological connections between the senses, i. e. the parts of the model <strong>in</strong><br />

2.6.2 connected with <strong>in</strong>nate predispositions.<br />

262


the conclusion is that the expressions where there is a more direct (ma<strong>in</strong>ly<br />

<strong>in</strong>dexical or iconic) connection between expression and mean<strong>in</strong>g are more<br />

alike <strong>in</strong> different languages.<br />

8.3 Possible explanations of onomatopoeia and<br />

sound symbolism<br />

Why is it that certa<strong>in</strong> consonant clusters are connected with certa<strong>in</strong><br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs? And why is it that we want certa<strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>gs to be expressed<br />

sound symbolically, as well as with conventional morphemes; what is the<br />

function of sound symbolic morphemes as compared with full<br />

morphemes?<br />

The first question is easy to answer when it concerns sound imitation. The<br />

expressive side of words are articulated sounds and articulation can<br />

imitate sounds, <strong>in</strong> a more or less conventionalized way. One possible<br />

explanation to other sound symbolism is that phonesthemes were<br />

orig<strong>in</strong>ally onomatopoeic and later developed <strong>in</strong>to metaphorically (e.g.<br />

gn-: 'sound' and later also 'light') or metonymically (e.g. skv-: 'sound'<br />

and later also 'wetness') related mean<strong>in</strong>gs (cf. 2.6.1). Underly<strong>in</strong>g this are<br />

the <strong>in</strong>nate capacities for metaphor and metonymy. This is <strong>in</strong> accordance<br />

with the theories of e.g. Herder (cf. 1.6.) about the orig<strong>in</strong> of language.<br />

This is not an impossible view s<strong>in</strong>ce almost all <strong>in</strong>itial consonant clusters<br />

have the feature 'sound' or 'talk<strong>in</strong>g' (which later feature is a sub-category<br />

of 'sound', cf. 4.2.3) to a greater or lesser extent. The only exceptions are<br />

pj-, spj-, spl- and spr-.<br />

There are some l<strong>in</strong>guistic facts about Swedish and analyses of several<br />

languages by various l<strong>in</strong>guists that show along what l<strong>in</strong>es other sound<br />

symbolism <strong>in</strong> Swedish can partly be expla<strong>in</strong>ed. A tendency is that lexically<br />

<strong>in</strong>frequent clusters (i.e. marked clusters) are exploited sound symbolically<br />

to a higher degree than lexically frequent clusters. It also seems as if a<br />

large proportion of the three-consonant s-clusters are used for sound<br />

symbolism <strong>in</strong> Swedish, cf. diagram 4.4. This is <strong>in</strong> accordance with the<br />

discussion of H<strong>in</strong>ton, Nichols, Ohala (1994) about marked sounds be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

used for sound symbolism. Also, sounds and sound comb<strong>in</strong>ations<br />

263


otherwise non-existent <strong>in</strong> the language (wild forms) occur <strong>in</strong><br />

onomatopoeic and sound symbolic neologisms.<br />

Another aspect of markedness is that sounds that are new <strong>in</strong> a language<br />

are often used sound symbolically (cf. Austerlitz, 1994, on ø <strong>in</strong> F<strong>in</strong>nish).<br />

In Swedish, the latest great consonant change was the collapse of several<br />

consonant clusters (e. g. skj-, stj-, sj-, kj-, tj-) <strong>in</strong>to the fricatives S and<br />

ç. A possible explanation is that the clusters conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g j that were left<br />

over – spj-, bj-, fj-, pj- – became more unusual and marked <strong>in</strong> Swedish<br />

and therefore useful for sound symbolism.<br />

Classify<strong>in</strong>g the different mean<strong>in</strong>gs of sound symbolism, it is clear that<br />

most of them fall under the perceptual category, but also under cognitive<br />

factors, viz. evaluation, but not under a social category. This h<strong>in</strong>ts at<br />

sound symbolism be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> some way biologically grounded, rather than<br />

learned.<br />

Most of the mean<strong>in</strong>gs of the phonesthemes are relatable to the senses<br />

(probably so because of metaphorical or metonymical extensions from<br />

sound and/or neurological connections between the senses), apart from the<br />

'pejoratives' and 'mental feel<strong>in</strong>g'. Thus the phonesthemes are related to<br />

hear<strong>in</strong>g, see<strong>in</strong>g, touch<strong>in</strong>g, (but not to smell<strong>in</strong>g, tast<strong>in</strong>g) 24. The semantic<br />

features occurr<strong>in</strong>g over and over aga<strong>in</strong> are relatable to stable <strong>in</strong>termodal<br />

connections, i.e. one sense is connected to another. There are, however,<br />

many problems with an explanation <strong>in</strong> terms of synaesthesia (see 1.11.2).<br />

Neurological aspects relevant for a model (see chapter 2) are neurological<br />

connections between the senses, or a common ground for the senses.<br />

Modalities that have especially strong connections (accord<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

Geschw<strong>in</strong>d, 1965, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Cytowic, 1989) are visual-auditory and<br />

tactile-auditory.<br />

Semantic features hav<strong>in</strong>g to do with movements can ontogenetically be<br />

expla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> terms of metonymy, i.e., simultaneity <strong>in</strong> time and space<br />

between sound and movement, and metaphor. In some cases<br />

24 There also exist, on a lexical level, mean<strong>in</strong>g extensions from sensory modalities to<br />

mental phenomena (see Abel<strong>in</strong>, 1988).<br />

264


proprioception might be <strong>in</strong>volved, as <strong>in</strong> an explanation of pejoratives<br />

where a feel<strong>in</strong>g of disgust is experienced simultaneously with contractions<br />

of certa<strong>in</strong> facial muscles, which contractions have an effect on articulation<br />

(cf. Darw<strong>in</strong>, "1965").<br />

Ohala's (e.g. 1994) frequency code offers a solution for sound symbolism<br />

of dim<strong>in</strong>utives and augmentatives. The common factor is high-low<br />

frequency for F0 (of <strong>in</strong>tonation), noise or F2. High frequency is<br />

connected to smallness, low frequency to largeness.<br />

The second question – Why is it that we want certa<strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>gs to be<br />

expressed sound symbolically, as well as with conventional morphemes? –<br />

can be answered with appeal to a k<strong>in</strong>d of redundancy <strong>in</strong> the l<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

sign. In language, there is partly a "triple articulation". The "third<br />

articulation" is mean<strong>in</strong>g bear<strong>in</strong>g and motivated (cf. 1.2.5), and this makes<br />

sound symbolic words very effective. There are, <strong>in</strong> sound symbolic<br />

words, other ties between expression and mean<strong>in</strong>g than merely the<br />

arbitrary conventional ones, namely those that are motivated but still to<br />

some extent conventional. We can still wonder if sound symbolism is a<br />

remnant from earlier stages, where it could have had a high survival<br />

value because of its connection with the senses and with th<strong>in</strong>gs present at<br />

the moment someth<strong>in</strong>g is spoken. An argument for this is, for example,<br />

Ohala's frequency code. Also, the connection between emotions and facial<br />

contractions possibly result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> sounds for pejoratives could also<br />

be a remnant from earlier stages. On the other hand, it is possible, as<br />

Jespersen claimed, that languages grow richer and richer <strong>in</strong> sound<br />

symbolic words.<br />

8.3.1 Pejoratives<br />

The pejorative phonesthemes resist a simple explanation. Is there an<br />

ugl<strong>in</strong>ess code? An ugl<strong>in</strong>ess metaphor? Or do they have to do with a basic<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>ction between approval and disapproval? One of the 6 basic<br />

emotions, accord<strong>in</strong>g to e.g. Ekman (1973) is disgust, which is an emotion<br />

underly<strong>in</strong>g pejoratives.<br />

265


Darw<strong>in</strong> (1872) gives a possible explanation for sound symbolic words<br />

related to emotions, e.g. disgust (related to pejoratives) and <strong>in</strong>terjections.<br />

The explanation builds on the <strong>in</strong>st<strong>in</strong>ctive contractions of facial muscles<br />

connected with a certa<strong>in</strong> emotion. Pejoratives could be of a more<br />

<strong>in</strong>dexical nature, the result of <strong>in</strong>terjections which <strong>in</strong> turn could be<br />

conditioned by <strong>in</strong>st<strong>in</strong>ctive facial contractions. A similar type of word is<br />

one that could be termed truly iconic. These are words like pluta, truta<br />

and pussa, perhaps gr<strong>in</strong>a and a few more, where the pronunciation of the<br />

vowel <strong>in</strong> particular can be seen as be<strong>in</strong>g dependent on the shape of the<br />

face and sometimes connected to emotions.<br />

Hamano (1994) proposes an explanation <strong>in</strong> child directed speech. He<br />

studied palatalization <strong>in</strong> Japanese sound symbolism. Palatalization of<br />

alveolar stops and fricatives is associated with "childishness" and<br />

"immaturity". He connects this to studies on language acquisition<br />

report<strong>in</strong>g palatalization as one of the universal characteristics of early<br />

stages of children’s language acquisition. Palatalization is also reported as<br />

one of the commonest devices of baby-talk, i.e., child directed speech<br />

(Snow and Ferguson, 1977). Perhaps this is a way of expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the<br />

Swedish pejorative clusters bj-, fj-, pj-, which do not fit <strong>in</strong>to a<br />

synaesthetic and metaphoric pattern.<br />

The high frequency of pejoratives <strong>in</strong> Swedish phonesthemes may well be a<br />

cultural peculiarity, s<strong>in</strong>ce it was not mirrored <strong>in</strong> the cross cultural<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretation experiment. The pejorative could be based on a general<br />

negative emotion, which could be favored <strong>in</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong> social environment.<br />

Naturally, further cross l<strong>in</strong>guistic research is needed before any<br />

conclusions can be drawn with respect to this.<br />

8.3.2 Summary<br />

To summarize:<br />

1) The unique feature of phonesthemes <strong>in</strong> relation to ord<strong>in</strong>ary morphemes<br />

is that they are bound morphemes that are both mean<strong>in</strong>g bear<strong>in</strong>g and<br />

motivated (cf. 1.2.5). Words with phonesthemes are very effective s<strong>in</strong>ce<br />

there are ties between expression and mean<strong>in</strong>g other than merely the<br />

266


arbitrary conventional, namely the motivated and to some extent the<br />

conventional.<br />

2a) Certa<strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>gs are suitable to be conveyed symbolically <strong>in</strong> sound<br />

e.g. mean<strong>in</strong>gs related to the senses because of the <strong>in</strong>teraction between, on<br />

the one hand, <strong>in</strong>nate capacities based on neurological connections between<br />

the senses and, on the other hand, learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the present. The<br />

neurological connections which (accord<strong>in</strong>g to Geschw<strong>in</strong>d, 1965,<br />

accord<strong>in</strong>g to Cytowic, 1989) are especially strong are the connections<br />

visually-auditorily and tactile-auditorily. They make it possible for sound<br />

imitative expressions and phonesthemes to lead to other sound symbolic<br />

connections between these sound sequences and e.g. forms and movements<br />

(which have to do with visual and/or tactile experience), and surface<br />

structures and consistency (which have to do with tactile experience). The<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g process, of which a part is perceiv<strong>in</strong>g sounds and mimick<strong>in</strong>g<br />

them, <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation with conventionalization, causes the sound symbolic<br />

expressions to vary (slightly) <strong>in</strong> different languages.<br />

2b) The connection between the expression and mean<strong>in</strong>g of pejoratives is<br />

more difficult to relate to neurological connections between the senses and<br />

can be given various explanations, connected with e. g. markedness, child<br />

language or proprioception. The emotion of disgust might, however, be<br />

beh<strong>in</strong>d the semantic feature 'pejorative'.<br />

3) The result that f<strong>in</strong>al clusters predom<strong>in</strong>antly end with a liquid or a<br />

voiceless obstruent could perhaps be expla<strong>in</strong>ed by the auditory salience <strong>in</strong><br />

the case of several voiceless obstruents; what is heard well is more useable<br />

<strong>in</strong> general. It could be that such sounds are chosen for sound symbolism<br />

s<strong>in</strong>ce they could then fulfill functions other than merely the dist<strong>in</strong>ctive. It<br />

is more difficult to give an explanation of the predom<strong>in</strong>ance of liquids<br />

s<strong>in</strong>ce this is a group based on both acoustic similarities and common<br />

phonological pattern<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The usefulness of sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> general is connected with the<br />

strong tie between mean<strong>in</strong>g and expression, discussed <strong>in</strong> 3.3. It is<br />

imag<strong>in</strong>able that peoples' reactions are quicker, stronger and more<br />

267


accurate to sound symbolic expressions, and therefore they can be e.g.<br />

more effective, more emotionally arous<strong>in</strong>g, and more poetic.<br />

8.4 Predictions for sound symbolism <strong>in</strong> Swedish<br />

When a new non-arbitrary word is created or understood it is most likely<br />

to have the follow<strong>in</strong>g characteristics:<br />

1. Its semantic content belongs to those described <strong>in</strong> chapter 4 (except for<br />

'light') and 5 25.<br />

<strong>Sound</strong>: <strong>Sound</strong><br />

Talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Beat<br />

Movement: Movement<br />

Quick or strong movement<br />

Walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Fall<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Potential movement<br />

Quickness<br />

Light: Light<br />

Gaze<br />

Surface structure: Rough surface structure<br />

Smooth surface structure<br />

Consistency: Soft consistency<br />

(Plasticity) Hardness<br />

Slackness<br />

Stiffness<br />

Wetness: Wetness<br />

Adhesion<br />

25 The order of presentation follows the order <strong>in</strong> 2.6 and 4.2.3, which is based on the<br />

prelim<strong>in</strong>ary lexical frequencies of this study. After the lexical study of <strong>in</strong>itial clusters<br />

was completed the order of the semantic features was somewhat changed, but the<br />

orig<strong>in</strong>al order was kept <strong>in</strong> some lists. It therefore does not represent an analytical order.<br />

For the order of frequency of the completed lexical study of <strong>in</strong>itial clusters, see diagram<br />

4.5.<br />

268


Dryness: Dryness<br />

Attitude: Attitude<br />

Secrecy<br />

Slang: Slang<br />

Jocular: Jocular<br />

Pejorative: Pejorative<br />

Destruction<br />

Mental feel<strong>in</strong>g: Mental feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Bad mood<br />

Bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g: Bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Suffocation<br />

Separation: Separation<br />

Putt<strong>in</strong>g together: Putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

Dim<strong>in</strong>utive: Dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

Augmentative: Augmentative<br />

Form: Form<br />

Round form<br />

Short-wide form<br />

Th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

Hollow form<br />

W<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

Long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

Narrow form<br />

Small end form<br />

Bent form<br />

Iterative: Iterative<br />

F<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong><br />

2. Its <strong>in</strong>itial and f<strong>in</strong>al consonant cluster is one that is appropriate for its<br />

semantic content, see chapters 4 (except for 'light') and 5. The most sound<br />

symbolic <strong>in</strong>itial clusters <strong>in</strong> absolute numbers and percentally, for the five<br />

most frequent mean<strong>in</strong>gs, are shown <strong>in</strong> table 8.4.<br />

Table 8.4 The most sound symbolic <strong>in</strong>itial clusters <strong>in</strong> absolute numbers<br />

and percentally, for the five most frequent mean<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

269


mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> freq. % <strong>in</strong> % % freq.<br />

freq.<br />

'pejorativ sl- 24 16 pj- 71 5<br />

e'<br />

'sound' kl-, 15 13, fn- 33 3<br />

kn-<br />

19<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong> sp- 23 17 spj- 33 2<br />

form'<br />

'quick or fl- 18 19 fl- 19 18<br />

strong<br />

movement<br />

'<br />

'wetness' sl- 12 8 skv- 40 4<br />

The most frequent semantic features for f<strong>in</strong>al clusters accord<strong>in</strong>g to NFO 4<br />

are shown <strong>in</strong> table 8.5. Clusters and cluster frequencies are shown to the<br />

right (cf. tables 5.4, 5.18 and 5.19).<br />

Table 8.5 The most frequent semantic features for f<strong>in</strong>al clusters accord<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to NFO 4.<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

freq.<br />

freq. <strong>in</strong> % %<br />

'quick or<br />

Nl 4 Nl, 40,<br />

strong<br />

movement'<br />

dr,<br />

lr<br />

25,<br />

22<br />

'talk<strong>in</strong>g' tr, bl 5, 4 dr, 25,<br />

bl 24<br />

'sound' st, sl, 5 ,4, lr, sl 44,<br />

lr, tr 4, 4<br />

27<br />

'pejorative' fs, 10, fs, 59,<br />

sk, 5, 4 ms, 27,<br />

ms<br />

dr 25<br />

'wetness' sk 8 - -<br />

'long th<strong>in</strong><br />

Nk, 7, 4 - -<br />

form'<br />

lk<br />

270


8.5 Ma<strong>in</strong> objective and further research<br />

The ma<strong>in</strong> objective of this thesis is the description of Swedish<br />

phonesthemes, which can constitute a basis for further research <strong>in</strong> this<br />

area.<br />

Another important result is the <strong>in</strong>sight that sound symbolism is not an<br />

issue of whether or not phonesthemes exist. Instead, sound symbolism is<br />

present <strong>in</strong> consonant clusters <strong>in</strong> vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees. Some clusters are more<br />

sound symbolic than others while some mean<strong>in</strong>gs are more frequent than<br />

others. Some phonesthemes are more easily <strong>in</strong>terpretable than others.<br />

Some phonesthemes are more productive than others and <strong>in</strong> slightly<br />

vary<strong>in</strong>g ways for different speakers. It is obvious that sound symbolic<br />

categories <strong>in</strong> Swedish show a conceptual organization that is compatible<br />

with both <strong>in</strong>nate categories of th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, such as similarity and contiguity,<br />

and with learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> connection with sense impressions and emotion.<br />

An <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g cont<strong>in</strong>uation of this <strong>in</strong>vestigation is to study the effect of<br />

context on the <strong>in</strong>terpretation of phonesthemes, e.g. <strong>in</strong> experiments with<br />

neologisms <strong>in</strong> different sentence contexts.<br />

Special spoken corpora would also be <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to study, <strong>in</strong> search of<br />

neologisms (<strong>in</strong> context), e.g. corpora of child and adolescent language.<br />

It would also be <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to study the stability of (the most common)<br />

phonesthemes over time, e.g. by analysis of "dead" words <strong>in</strong> the SAOB<br />

(Svenska Akademiens ordlista över svenska språket).<br />

Yet another <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g cont<strong>in</strong>uation would be to study the dispersion of<br />

(the most common) phonesthemes <strong>in</strong> dialect lexica.<br />

And, of course, it would be very <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to expand the cross-l<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

comparisons to more semantic fields and to more languages.<br />

It is also very tempt<strong>in</strong>g to refresh e.g. the experiments of chapter 6 and<br />

the lexical decision experiment (Abel<strong>in</strong>, 1996) with auditive stimuli, with<br />

the aid of reaction time programs which were not available at the time<br />

271


when I <strong>in</strong>itiated the experimentation. Naturally, the context effect should<br />

also be studied <strong>in</strong> this way. Connected with this is the aim to construct a<br />

psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic model for onl<strong>in</strong>e process<strong>in</strong>g for the perception and<br />

production of sound symbolic words.<br />

272


Bibliography<br />

Abel<strong>in</strong>, Å., Allwood, J. (1984) Tolkn<strong>in</strong>g av känsloprosodi - en kontrastiv studie,<br />

unpubl. paper, Göteborg University, Department of L<strong>in</strong>guistics.<br />

Abel<strong>in</strong>, Å. (1988) 'Patterns of synaesthesia <strong>in</strong> the Swedish vocabulary,' <strong>in</strong> <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>in</strong><br />

Computer-Aided lexicology, Almqvist & Wiksell Int. Stockholm<br />

Abel<strong>in</strong>, Å. (1996) 'A lexical decision experiment with onomatopoeic, sound symbolic<br />

and arbitrary words', <strong>in</strong> TMH-QPSR 2/1996. Stockholm<br />

Adams, D., Lloyd, J. (1983) The mean<strong>in</strong>g of Liff, Pan Books and Faber & Faber.<br />

London<br />

Allén, S., Eeg-Olofson, M., Gavare, R., Sjögreen, C. (1981) Svensk baklängesordbok,<br />

Esselte Studium. Stockholm<br />

Allén, S., Berg, S., Järborg, J., Löfström, J., Ralph, B., Sjögreen, C. (1980)<br />

Nusvensk frekvensordbok 4, Almqvist & Wiksell International. Stockholm<br />

Allott, R. (1973) The physical foundation of language, ELB Pr<strong>in</strong>ters. Seaford Sussex<br />

Allwood, J. (1983) Om dist<strong>in</strong>ktionerna mellan semantik och pragmatik, Gothenburg<br />

papers <strong>in</strong> theoretical l<strong>in</strong>guistics S3, Göteborg University, Department of L<strong>in</strong>guistics<br />

Allwood, J. (1983) 'Kan man tänka oberoende av språk' <strong>in</strong> U. Teleman (ed.) Tal och<br />

tanke, Liber förlag. Lund<br />

Allwood, J. (1985) 'Tvärkulturell kommunikation', <strong>in</strong> Papers <strong>in</strong> anthropological<br />

l<strong>in</strong>guistics 12, Göteborg University, Department of L<strong>in</strong>guistics, pp. 9-61<br />

Allwood, J. (1989) Om begrepp – deras bestämn<strong>in</strong>g, analys och konstruktion (ms).<br />

Allwood, J., Andersson, L-G.(1976) Semantik. (Semantics). GULING 1. Göteborg<br />

University, Department of L<strong>in</strong>guistics<br />

Austerlitz, R. (1994) 'F<strong>in</strong>nish and Gilyak sound symbolism – an <strong>in</strong>terplay between<br />

system and history'. <strong>in</strong> L. H<strong>in</strong>ton, J. Nichols, J.J. Ohala (eds) <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism,<br />

Cambridge University Press. Cambridge, pp. 249-260<br />

Bentley, M., Varon, E. (1933) An accessory study of phonetic symbolism Amer. J.<br />

Psychol. 45, 76-86<br />

Bloomfield, L. (1933) Language, Henry Holt. New York<br />

Bol<strong>in</strong>ger, D. (1968) Aspects of language, Harcourt, Brace & World, Inc. New York<br />

Bol<strong>in</strong>ger, D. (1950), Rime, assonance and morpheme analysis, Word 6: 117-136<br />

Bol<strong>in</strong>ger, D. (1965) Forms of English: Accent, morpheme, order, Hokuou Publish<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Company. Tokyo<br />

Br<strong>in</strong>g, S. C. (1930) Svenskt ordförråd, Natur och Kultur. Stockholm<br />

Brown, R. W. (1958) Words and th<strong>in</strong>gs, The Free Press. New York<br />

273


Bühler, K. (1969) "L'onomatopée et la fonction représentative du langage", <strong>in</strong> Essais sur<br />

le langage. J.-P. Pariente (ed.) Les Editions de M<strong>in</strong>uit. Paris (Partial translation of work<br />

orig<strong>in</strong>ally published 1933 as Sprachtheorie ) Fisher. Jena<br />

Chasta<strong>in</strong>g, M. (1958) "Le symbolisme de voyelles: significations des "i" I& II", Journal<br />

de Psychologie 55, 403-423 & 461-481<br />

Chasta<strong>in</strong>g, M. (1965) "Pop - fop - pof - fof", Vie et Langage 159, 311-317<br />

Chasta<strong>in</strong>g, M. (1966) "Si les r étaient des l", (Part 1) Vie et langage 173, 468-472; (Part<br />

2) 174, 502-507<br />

Childs, G.T. (1994) 'African ideophones'. <strong>in</strong> L. H<strong>in</strong>ton, J. Nichols, J.J. Ohala (eds.)<br />

<strong>Sound</strong> symbolism, Cambridge University Press. Cambridge, pp. 178-204<br />

Clark, H., Clark, E. (1977) Psychology and language, An <strong>in</strong>troduction to<br />

psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics, Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich. New York<br />

Cytowic, (1989) Synaesthesia - a union of the senses, Spr<strong>in</strong>ger Verlag. New York<br />

Darw<strong>in</strong>, C. (1872) The expression of emotion <strong>in</strong> man and animals, John Murray. London<br />

Davis, R. (1961) The fitness of names to draw<strong>in</strong>gs: A cross-cultural study <strong>in</strong><br />

Tanganyika, Brit. J. Psychol. 52, 259-268<br />

Diffloth, G. (1976) 'Expressives <strong>in</strong> Semai', Oceanic l<strong>in</strong>guistics special publication no.<br />

13: Austroasiatic <strong>Studies</strong> I, Honolulu<br />

Diffloth, G., (1994) 'i:big, a:small', <strong>in</strong> L. H<strong>in</strong>ton, J. Nichols, J.J. Ohala (eds) <strong>Sound</strong><br />

symbolism, Cambridge University Press. Cambridge<br />

Ekman, P. (1973) 'Cross-cultural studies of facial expressions' <strong>in</strong> P. Ekman (ed.)<br />

Darw<strong>in</strong> and facial expression , Academic Press. New York (pp. 169-222)<br />

Fano, G. (1962) Saggio sulle orig<strong>in</strong>e del l<strong>in</strong>guaggio. Con una storia critica della dottr<strong>in</strong>e<br />

glottogoniche, Guil<strong>in</strong> E<strong>in</strong>audi Editore. Tor<strong>in</strong>o<br />

Fónagy, I. (1963) Die metaphern <strong>in</strong> der phonetik. The Hague<br />

Freud, S. (1891) On aphasia, a critical study, International Univ. Press. New York<br />

Gabelentz, G. v. d. (1891) Die Sprachwissenschaft, ihre Aufgaben, Methoden und<br />

bisherigen Ergebnisse. Leibzig<br />

Garlén, C. (1988) Svenskans fonologi: i kontrastiv och typologisk belysn<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

Studentlitteratur. Lund<br />

Garman, M. (1990) Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics, Cambridge University Press. Cambridge<br />

Grammont, M. (1933) Traité de phonétique. Paris<br />

Hamano, S. (1994) 'Palatalization <strong>in</strong> Japanese sound symbolism', <strong>in</strong> L. H<strong>in</strong>ton, J.<br />

Nichols, J.J. Ohala (eds) <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism, Cambridge University Press. Cambridge,<br />

pp. 148-157<br />

Hellquist, E. (1966) Svensk etymologisk ordbok I-II. Gleerup. Lund<br />

274


Herder, J. G. (1772/1969) Essay on the orig<strong>in</strong> of speech <strong>in</strong> P.H. Salus (ed.) On<br />

language: Plato to von Humboldt, Holt, R<strong>in</strong>ehart and W<strong>in</strong>ston. New York, pp.147-166<br />

Hewes, G. W. (1977) 'Language orig<strong>in</strong> Theories' <strong>in</strong> D. M. Rumbaugh (ed) Language<br />

Learn<strong>in</strong>g by a Chimpanzee, New York Academic Press. New York<br />

H<strong>in</strong>ton, L. (1986) 'Musical diffusion and l<strong>in</strong>guistic diffusion'. <strong>in</strong> C. Frisbee (ed)<br />

Anthropology and Music: Essays <strong>in</strong> Honor of David P. McAllester. Detroit Monographs<br />

<strong>in</strong> Musicology 9. Detroit. Information Coord<strong>in</strong>ators<br />

H<strong>in</strong>ton, L., Nichols, J., Ohala, J.J., (1994) 'Introduction: <strong>Sound</strong> symbolic processes',<br />

<strong>in</strong> L. H<strong>in</strong>ton, J. Nichols, J.J. Ohala (eds) <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism, Cambridge University<br />

Press. Cambridge, pp. 1-12<br />

Holland, M. K., Wertheimer, M. (1964) 'Some physiognomic aspects of nam<strong>in</strong>g, or,<br />

maluma and takete revisited', Percept. Mot. Skills 19, 111-117<br />

Householder, F.W. (1946) On the problem of sound and mean<strong>in</strong>g, An English<br />

phonestheme (discussion <strong>in</strong> Language 23 feb 1946)<br />

Humboldt W. v. (1836/1907) Über die Verschiedenheit des menschlichen Sprachbaues<br />

und ihren E<strong>in</strong>fluss auf die geistige Entwicklung des Menschengeschlechts. Gesammelte<br />

Schriften, Vol. VII, Part 1. Königlich--Preussische Akademie der Wissenschaften. Berl<strong>in</strong><br />

Hörmann, H. (1979) Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics: an <strong>in</strong>troduction to research and theory, Spr<strong>in</strong>ger-<br />

Verlag. New York<br />

Ideforss, H. (1928) De primära <strong>in</strong>terjektionerna i nysvenskan, 1, Primära impulsioner<br />

och imperationer, Carl Bloms boktryckeri. Lund<br />

Jacobsen Jr., W. H. (1994) 'Nootkan vocative vocalism and its implications' <strong>in</strong> L.<br />

H<strong>in</strong>ton, J. Nichols, J.J. Ohala (eds) <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism, Cambridge University Press.<br />

Cambridge, pp. 23-39<br />

Jakobson, R., Fant, G., Halle, M. (1957) Prelim<strong>in</strong>aries to speech analysis, the<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>ctive features and their acoustic correlates, M.I.T. Press. Cambridge, Mass.<br />

Jakobson, R., Waugh, L. (1979) The sound shape of language, Harvester Press, Sussex<br />

Jespersen, O. (1918) "Nogle men-ord", studier tillegnade Esaias Tegner, Lund<br />

Jespersen, O. (1922 a) Language - its nature, development and orig<strong>in</strong>, Allen and Unw<strong>in</strong>.<br />

London<br />

Jespersen, O. (1922 b) "Lydsymbolik", Nordisk tidskrift för vetenskap, konst och<br />

<strong>in</strong>dustri (II), Stockholm<br />

Jesperssen, O. (1933) "Symbolic value of the vowel i" [1922] <strong>in</strong> L<strong>in</strong>guistica, College<br />

Park. Maryland<br />

Jo'hannesson, A. (1949) Orig<strong>in</strong> of language: four essays, Reykjavík, H.F. Leiftur,<br />

Blackwell. Oxford<br />

Katz, J.J., Postal, F. (1964) An <strong>in</strong>tegrated theory of l<strong>in</strong>guistic descriptions, M.I.T.<br />

Press. Cambridge, Mass.<br />

Kots<strong>in</strong>as, U. (1994) Ungdomsspråk, Hallgren & Fallgren. Uppsala<br />

275


Kukkonen, P. (1989) Från konst till vetenskap, PhD Thesis, Hels<strong>in</strong>gfors university<br />

Källström, R. (1988) Morfologi, GULING 9, Göteborg University, Department of<br />

L<strong>in</strong>guistics.<br />

Köhler W. (1930) Gestalt psychology, Bell and Sons Ltd. London<br />

Lakoff, G., Johnsson, M. (1980) Metaphors we live by, The University of Chicago<br />

Press. Chicago and London<br />

Lakoff, G. (1987) Women, fire, and dangerous th<strong>in</strong>gs: what categories reveal about the<br />

m<strong>in</strong>d, University of Chicago Press. Chicago<br />

Lapolla, R.J, (1994) 'An experimental <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>in</strong>to phonetic symbolism as it relates<br />

to Mandar<strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese' <strong>in</strong> L. H<strong>in</strong>ton, J. Nichols, J.J. Ohala (eds.) <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism,<br />

Cambridge University Press. Cambridge<br />

Leech, G. N. (1969) Towards a semantic description of English, Longman. London<br />

Lehrer, A. (1974) Semantic fields and lexical structure, North-Holland. Amsterdam,<br />

London<br />

L<strong>in</strong>dblad, P. (1992) Rösten, Studentlitteratur. Lund<br />

Luria, A. (1977) Mannen med det obegränsade m<strong>in</strong>net, Wahlström & Widstrand.<br />

Stockholm<br />

Lyons, J. (1977) Semantics I-II, Cambridge University Press. Cambridge<br />

Malkiel, Y. (1978) 'From phonosymbolism to morphosymbolism', Fourth LACUS<br />

Forum, Columbia, S.C.<br />

Malkiel, Y. (1994) 'Regular sound development, phonosymbolic orchestration,<br />

disambiguation of homonyms' <strong>in</strong> L. H<strong>in</strong>ton, J. Nichols, J.J. Ohala (eds) <strong>Sound</strong><br />

symbolism, Cambridge University Press. Cambridge<br />

Malmgren, S-G, (1988) 'On regular polysemy <strong>in</strong> Swedish' <strong>in</strong> <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>in</strong> Computer-Aided<br />

Lexicology, Almqvist & Wiksell International. Stockholm<br />

Markell, N.N. & Hamp, E.P. (1960) "Connotative mean<strong>in</strong>gs of certa<strong>in</strong> phoneme<br />

Sequences", <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistics 15, pp. 47-61<br />

Marks L.E. (1990) Synaesthesia: perception and metaphor <strong>in</strong> F. Burwick & W. Pape<br />

(eds) Aesthetic Illusion: theoretical and historical approaches, Berl<strong>in</strong> New York, W. de<br />

Gruyter<br />

Massaro, D. W., Tsuzaki, M., Cohen, M. M:, Gesi, A., & Heredia, R. (1993),<br />

'Bimodal speech perception : An exam<strong>in</strong>ation across languages'. <strong>in</strong> Journal of Phonetics,<br />

21<br />

Meillet, A. (1931) Review of Wahlgren, 1930 Bullet<strong>in</strong> de la Société de l<strong>in</strong>guistique de<br />

Paris 31 (2): 113-114<br />

Miller, G. A., Johnsson-Laird, P.N. (1976) Language and perception, Harward<br />

University Press. Cambridge, Mass.<br />

Müller, A. L., (1960) Experimentelle Untersuchungen zur stimmlichen Darstellung von<br />

Gefühlen, Dissertation. Gött<strong>in</strong>gen<br />

276


Müller, M.F. (1861/1961) Three lectures on the science of language and its place <strong>in</strong><br />

general education, repr. Indological Book House. Benares<br />

Nerman, T. (1954) Troll i ord, <strong>in</strong>rim och uddrim i svensk vers, Tidens förlag. Stockholm<br />

Newman, S. (1933) Further experiments <strong>in</strong> phonetic symbolism, American Journal of<br />

Psychology 45, 53-75<br />

Nordberg, B. (1986) 'The use of onomatopoeia <strong>in</strong> the conversational style of<br />

adolescents', FUMS rapport nr 132, Uppsala University. FUMS. Uppsala<br />

Ohala, J. J., (1994) The frequency code underlies the sound-symbolic use of voice pitch,<br />

<strong>in</strong> L. H<strong>in</strong>ton, J. Nichols, J.J. Ohala (eds.) <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism, Cambridge University<br />

Press. Cambridge<br />

Osgood, C. E. (1962 a) Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic relativity and universality, Proc. 16th Int.<br />

Congr. Psychol. (Bonn 1960) North-Holland. Amsterdam<br />

Osgood, C. E. (1962 b) <strong>Studies</strong> on the generality of affective mean<strong>in</strong>g systems, Amer.<br />

Psychologist 17, 10-28<br />

Osgood, C. E., G. J. Suci, and P. H. Tannenbaum (1957) The measurment of mean<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

University of Ill<strong>in</strong>ois Press. Urbana, Ill<strong>in</strong>ois<br />

Oswalt, R. L. (1994) 'Inanimate imitatives <strong>in</strong> English', <strong>in</strong> L. H<strong>in</strong>ton, J. Nichols, J.J.<br />

Ohala (eds.) <strong>Sound</strong> symbolism, Cambridge University Press. Cambridge, pp. 293-306<br />

The Oxford dictionary of English etymology (1967), ed. by C. T. Onions, University<br />

press. Oxford<br />

Paget, R. (1930) Human Speech, Harcourt, Brace. New York<br />

Peirce, C.S. (1955) 'Logic as semiotic: The theory of signs', <strong>in</strong> Buchler, J. (ed)<br />

Philosophical writ<strong>in</strong>gs of Peirce Dover. New York<br />

Peterfalvi, J.-M. (1965) 'Les recherches expérimentales sur le symbolisme phonétique',<br />

American Journal of Psychology 65, 439-473<br />

Plato (c. 428-348 B.C.) Cratylos dialogue (1962) Loeb Classical Library. Harward<br />

University Press. Cambridge<br />

Porzig, W. (1934/1973) 'Wesenhafte Bedeutungsbeziehungen'. Wortfeldsforschung<br />

1973. 78-103<br />

Révész, G. (1946) Ursprung und Vorgeschichte der Sprache, A. Francke. Bern<br />

Rhodes, R. (1994) 'Aural images', <strong>in</strong> L. H<strong>in</strong>ton, J. Nichols, J.J. Ohala (eds.) <strong>Sound</strong><br />

symbolism, Cambridge University Press. Cambridge, pp. 276-292<br />

Roget's thesaurus of English words and phrases (1977) Pengu<strong>in</strong> Books.<br />

Harmondsworth. Middlesex<br />

Rousseau, J.-J. (1822/1969) Essay on the orig<strong>in</strong> of languages <strong>in</strong> P.H. Salus (ed.) On<br />

Language: Plato to von Humboldt, Holt, R<strong>in</strong>ehart and W<strong>in</strong>ston. New York, pp. 138-146<br />

Samar<strong>in</strong>, W. (1978) 'L<strong>in</strong>guistic adaptation to speech function', <strong>in</strong> W. Mc Cormack, S.A.<br />

Wurm (eds) Approaches to language: Anthropological issues, Mouton. The Hague<br />

277


Sapir, E. (1921) Language, Harcourt Brace. New York<br />

Sapir, E. (1929) 'A study <strong>in</strong> phonetic symbolism', <strong>in</strong> D. Mandelbaum (ed.) Selected<br />

writ<strong>in</strong>gs, Berkeley. California<br />

Saussure, F. de (1916/1983) Course <strong>in</strong> general l<strong>in</strong>guistics, Duckworth. London<br />

Sereno J. A. (1994) 'Phonosyntactics', <strong>in</strong> L. H<strong>in</strong>ton, J. Nichols, J.J. Ohala (eds) <strong>Sound</strong><br />

symbolism, Cambridge University Press. Cambridge<br />

Sigurd, B. (1965) Phonotactic sructures <strong>in</strong> Swedish, Berl<strong>in</strong>gska boktryckeriet. Lund<br />

Sigurd, B. (1993) 'Att tillverka varumärken', Språkvård 3, Svenska språknämnden.<br />

Stockholm, pp. 9-14<br />

Snow, C.E., Ferguson, C.A., (eds), (1977) Talk<strong>in</strong>g to Children: Language Input and<br />

Acquisition, Cambridge University Press. New York<br />

Stenström, C. (1984) Svenskans <strong>in</strong>itiala konsonantkluster - en frekvensstudie, Unpubl.<br />

paper, Dept. of l<strong>in</strong>guistics. Göteborg University, Department of L<strong>in</strong>guistics.<br />

Süssmilch, J.P. (1767), Versuch e<strong>in</strong>es Beweises, dass die erste Sprache ihren Ursprung<br />

nicht vom Menschen, sondern alle<strong>in</strong> vom Schöpfer erhalten habe, Akad. Vorl. Berl<strong>in</strong><br />

Svensk Ordbok (1986) Esselte Studium. Stockholm<br />

Svenska Akademiens ordlista över svenska språket (1974) 10 uppl., P.A. Norstedt &<br />

Söner förlag. Stockholm<br />

Svenska Akademiens ordlista över svenska språket (1998) 12 uppl., Norstedts.<br />

Stockholm<br />

Ordbok över svenska språket, utgiven av Svenska Akademien (SAOB) (1926),<br />

L<strong>in</strong>dstedts universitetsbokhandel. Lund<br />

Thorndike, E. L. (1943) Man and his works, Harvard University Press. Cambridge,<br />

Mass.<br />

Thun, N. (1963) Reduplicative words <strong>in</strong> English: a study of formations of the types ticktock,<br />

hurly-burly, and shilly-shally. PhD Thesis, Stockholm University<br />

Traunmüller, H. (1996) '<strong>Sound</strong> symbolism <strong>in</strong> deictic words', TMH-QPSR 2/1996.<br />

Stockholm<br />

Trier, J. (1934) Das sprachliche Feld, Neue Jahrbucher fur Wissenschaft und<br />

Jugendbildung 10, 428-449<br />

Tsuru, S., Fries, H. S. (1933) 'A problem <strong>in</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>g', Journal of Gen. Psychology 8,<br />

281-284<br />

Ullman, S. (1972) Semantics An <strong>in</strong>troduction to the science of mean<strong>in</strong>g, Blackwell.<br />

Oxford<br />

Ultan, R. (1978) 'Size-sound symbolism', <strong>in</strong> J. Greenberg (ed) Universals of human<br />

language 2,<br />

278


Usnadze, D. (1924) 'E<strong>in</strong> experimenteller Beitrag zum problem der psychologischen<br />

Grundlagen der Namengebung', Psychologische Forschung 5, 24-43.<br />

Werner, H. (1953/1957) Comparative psychology of mental development, International<br />

Universities Press. New York<br />

Werner, H., Kaplan, B. (1963) Symbol formation, Wiley. New York<br />

Wescott, R. (1975) 'Allol<strong>in</strong>guistics: explor<strong>in</strong>g the peripheries of speech', <strong>in</strong> P. Reich<br />

(ed),The Second LACUS Forum, The Hornbeam Press, Columbia, South Carol<strong>in</strong>a, 497-<br />

513<br />

Wescott, R. (1978) 'Lexical polygenesis: Words as resultants of multiple l<strong>in</strong>guistic<br />

pressures' <strong>in</strong> Semantics and Lexicology<br />

Whorf, B. L. (1956/1967) Language, thought and reality: selected writ<strong>in</strong>gs, ed J.B.<br />

Carroll (ed), The MIT paperback series 5, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.<br />

Viberg, Å. (1984) 'The verbs of perception: a typological study', L<strong>in</strong>guistics, 21, 1.<br />

Viberg, Å. (1993) 'Crossl<strong>in</strong>guistic perspectives on lexical organization and lexical<br />

progression' <strong>in</strong> K. Hyltenstam & Å. Viberg (eds), Progression and Regression <strong>in</strong><br />

language, Cambridge University Press. Cambridge, pp. 340-385<br />

Wilde, K. (1958) 'Naive und kunstlerische Formen des graphischen Ausdrucks', Ber.<br />

21. Kongr. Dtsch. Ges. Psychol., Gött<strong>in</strong>gen: Verlag fur Psychologie, Bonn, pp. 157-<br />

159<br />

Wisseman, H. (1954) Untersuchungen zur Onomatopoie, Part I: Die<br />

sprachpsychologischen Versuche, W<strong>in</strong>ter. Heidelberg<br />

279


From SOB<br />

Word: Key words,SOB Category<br />

bjäbba uppnosigt prat pejorative, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

bjäfs överdriven pejorative<br />

bjällra kl<strong>in</strong>gande sound<br />

bjärt lysande light<br />

bjässe mycket stor augmentative<br />

black ljus, färg light<br />

bladdra prata strunt pejorative, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

blaffa större fläck m<strong>in</strong>dr tilltalan pejorative<br />

blaj men<strong>in</strong>gslöst prat pejorative, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

blank glansig yta light<br />

blarr struntprat pejorative, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

blaska röra i vatten plaska dålig pejorative, wetness<br />

blek färg light<br />

blemma blåsa round form<br />

blick seende gaze<br />

bliga titta envetet, dumt, fånstirra pejorative, gaze<br />

bl<strong>in</strong>d se gaze<br />

bl<strong>in</strong>ka ögonen gaze<br />

blixt ljus light<br />

block massivt stycke shortwide form<br />

blod vätska wetness<br />

blond ljus färg light<br />

blossa br<strong>in</strong>na, låga light<br />

bluddra struntprat pejorative, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

blunda ögonen gaze<br />

blunder misstag pejorative<br />

blåsa rundad round form<br />

bläck vätska färgad wetness<br />

bläddra blemma, blåsa round form<br />

blända ljus light<br />

blänga betrakta gaze<br />

blänka ljus light<br />

bläs ljus strimma light<br />

blöt vatten wetness<br />

braka ljud sound<br />

brassa skjuta , stora krafter sound<br />

brista sönder destruction<br />

brum ljud sound<br />

brus ljud sound<br />

bryta kraftig, spricka destruction<br />

bråte förbrukat, oanvändbart destruction<br />

bräck krosskada destruction<br />

bräka ljud sound<br />

bränn<strong>in</strong>g splitras destruction<br />

bröl ljud sound<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 1


drasut nedsättande pejorative<br />

dratta slarv, fumlig fall<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

dravel struntprat talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

dregel saliv, oavsiktligt wetness, pejorative<br />

dribbla rörelser movement<br />

drill ton sound<br />

droppa falla fall<strong>in</strong>g, wetness<br />

drulle ohyfsat, vårdslöst pejorative<br />

drummel klumpigt, vårdslöst pejorative<br />

dråsa falla klumpigt fall<strong>in</strong>g<br />

drägg botten, slödder pejorative<br />

drälla planlöst, vårdslöst, förflytta fall<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

drämma slå hårt beat<br />

dröse osorterad, ostrukturerad mängd pejorative<br />

drösa falla, tätt, okontrollerat fall<strong>in</strong>g<br />

fjant åtlöje pejorative<br />

fjolla saknar stadga i karaktär pejorative<br />

fjompig löjlig pejorative<br />

fjuttig futtig, torftig pejorative<br />

fjärta ljud, ofrivilligt sound<br />

fjäsa göra sig till pejorative<br />

fjäsk överdriven pejorative<br />

flabb skratt sound, pejorative<br />

flacka planlöst fara q or s movement, pejorative<br />

fladdra rörelse q or s movement<br />

flaga tunt stycke th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

flagga vifta q or s movement<br />

flak flat form, större stycke th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

flamma stark eld, orolig, plötslig light, q or s movement<br />

flamsa bullersam, tröttsamt, vårdslöst, stojigt pejorative<br />

flanera vandra utan bestämt mål q or s movement<br />

flarn bräckligt stycke poröst th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

flaxa slå häftigt, flygande q or s movement<br />

flimra dallrande ljus q or s movement, light<br />

fl<strong>in</strong>a m<strong>in</strong>dre vackert, försmädligt pejorative<br />

fl<strong>in</strong>ga litet lätt tunt stycke th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

fl<strong>in</strong>k snabbt quickness<br />

flisa litet tunt vasst stycke th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

flitter oäkta, värdelös pejorative<br />

flopp snöplig, misslyckande pejorative<br />

floskel högtravande <strong>in</strong>tetsäg. löjlig pejorative<br />

fluffa lätt luftig ruska airy consistency<br />

fluga flygande q or s movement<br />

flukta kasta snabb blick gaze<br />

fluktuation ständigt stigande och fallande q or s movement<br />

flummig <strong>in</strong>tellektuellt oredig pejorative<br />

flyga förflytta sig q or s movement<br />

fly hastigt lämna q or s movement<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 2


flyta transporteras q or s movement<br />

flytta till annan plats q or s movement<br />

flåsa tungt snabbt q or s movement<br />

fläkta blåsa q or s movement<br />

flämta snabbt häftigt q or s movement<br />

flänga rusa framochtillbaka hetsigt q or s movement<br />

flöda r<strong>in</strong>na stor mängd q or s movement<br />

flöjel v<strong>in</strong>driktn<strong>in</strong>g lätt skiva th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

flöjt ton luftström skarp kant sound<br />

fnasig fjälla th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

fnask nedsättande pejorative<br />

fnatta spr<strong>in</strong>ga planlöst q or s movement, pejorative<br />

fnissa skratta sound<br />

fnittra skratta sound<br />

fnoskig tokig pejorative<br />

fnurra osams pejorative<br />

fnysa häftigt ljudligt förakt sound<br />

fnöske porös dryness<br />

fradga skummande wetness<br />

frasa ljud sound<br />

frat söndergnagt skräp destruction<br />

frossa skakn<strong>in</strong>g köldkänslor q or s movement<br />

frotté poröst rough surface structure<br />

frusa häftigt välla fram q or s movement<br />

frusta ljud sound<br />

fryna rynka rough surface structure<br />

fräsa ljud sound<br />

fräta upplösa destruction<br />

glacé glänsande light<br />

glací jämn yta smooth surface str<br />

glaciär is smooth surface str<br />

glam högljudd sound<br />

glana stirra gaze<br />

glans jämnt sken blank yta light<br />

glas glänsande smooth surface str<br />

glatt slät blank hal smooth surface str<br />

glaubersalt glas smooth surface str<br />

glaukom syn gaze<br />

gli litet dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

glida jämnt smooth surface str<br />

glimma lysa svagt skiftande light<br />

glimmer glänsande light<br />

glimra lysa svagt skiftande light<br />

glimt blänk ljus light<br />

gl<strong>in</strong>der glänsande light<br />

gl<strong>in</strong>dra glimra light<br />

glipa smalt form, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 3


glisa lysa light<br />

glissando glida ton sound, smooth surface<br />

glittra lysa starkt snabbt light<br />

glo stirra dumt gaze<br />

glob klotformig form<br />

glop slyngel pejorative<br />

glopp snö regn wetness<br />

gloria ljus light<br />

glosögd ögon gaze<br />

glosa glo gaze<br />

glufsa mycket ovårdat pejorative<br />

glugg liten form, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

glunkas sägas skvaller pejorative<br />

glupa glupskt pejorative<br />

glupsk nedsätt pejorative<br />

glutta titta gaze<br />

glytt barnunge dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

glåmig blek light<br />

glåpord förolämpande yttrande pejorative<br />

gläfs svagt hundskall sound<br />

glänsa sken blank light<br />

glänta m<strong>in</strong>dre öppen form, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

glöd sken light<br />

gnabb smågräl talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

gnat småaktig upprepad talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

gneta småsaker, oväsentlig, snål pejorative<br />

gnida upprepade rörelser smooth surface str, q or s mo<br />

gnidare snål pejorative<br />

gnissla ljud sound<br />

gnista br<strong>in</strong>nande glödande light<br />

gnistra korta starka ljusglimtar light<br />

gno små snabba rörelser smooth surface str, q or s mo<br />

gnod<strong>in</strong>g knorrhane sound<br />

gnola sjunga talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

gnugga små korta rörelser smooth surface str, q or s mo<br />

gnutta mycket liten dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

gny ljud talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

gnägga kraftigt läte sound<br />

gnälla läte sound<br />

gnöla klaga talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

gramse arg irriterad bad mood<br />

grav grävd hålighet i marken hollow form<br />

grift grav hollow form<br />

groll ovänskap bad mood<br />

grop djup hålighet hollow form<br />

grotta håltum hollow form<br />

grubba grop hollow form<br />

grubbla dyster bad mood<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 4


gruff gräl bad mood, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

grumlig ogenomsk<strong>in</strong>lig oönskad pejorative<br />

grummel grums pejorative<br />

grums oönskad pejorative<br />

grumsa klagomål pejorative, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

grunka föremål vard slang<br />

grym samvetslöst tillfogar lidande bad mood<br />

grymta ljud talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

gryt jordhåla hollow form<br />

gråta röst talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

gräla högljudd respektlös diskussion bad mood, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

gräll syn alltför starkt light, pejorative<br />

gräma oönskad pejorative<br />

gräslig stark olust pejorative<br />

gräva djupare grop hollow form<br />

gröpa gräva m<strong>in</strong>dre hål hollow form<br />

klabb kort tjockt stycke shortwide form<br />

klabb blöt wetness<br />

klack utskjutande, del shortwide form<br />

kladd blöt vidhäftande wetness<br />

klafs ljud våt klibbig sound, wetness<br />

klaga framföra missnöje talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

klamp grovt stycke shortwide form<br />

klamp ljud sound<br />

klamra gripa adhesion<br />

klandra ogillande yttra talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

klang toner sound<br />

klanka upprepat förebrå småaktigt talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

klanta dum klumpig pejorative<br />

klappa upprepat slå sound<br />

klappra ljud sound<br />

klatsch ljud sound<br />

klena kladdig wetness<br />

kleta blöt vidhäftande wetness, adhesion<br />

klibba kladdig wetness, adhesion<br />

klick klump smetig wetness, shortwide form<br />

klick ljud sound<br />

klifs ljud blött sound, wetness<br />

klimp m<strong>in</strong>dre stycke shortwide form<br />

kl<strong>in</strong>g ljud sound<br />

kl<strong>in</strong>k spel sound<br />

klirra ljud sound<br />

klister klibbig adhesion<br />

klocka klang sound<br />

kloss grovt stycke shortwide form<br />

klot runt shortwide form<br />

klotter slarvig pejorative<br />

klubba kloss shortwide form<br />

klucka ljud sound, wetness<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 5


kludda dålig slarvig pejorative<br />

klump oformligt större stycke shortwide form<br />

klunk vätska wetness<br />

kluns klumpig pejorative<br />

klåpa dålig slarvig pejorative<br />

klämta r<strong>in</strong>ga sound<br />

klänga hålla kvar adhesion<br />

kläpp ljud, klump sound, shortwide form<br />

klätt liten topp shortwide form<br />

klättra fästpunkt adhesion<br />

knacka upprepade slag lystr<strong>in</strong>g sound<br />

knackig dålig pejorative<br />

knagg p<strong>in</strong>ne round form<br />

knaggla röra sig walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

knaka ljud sound<br />

knal svag knapp pejorative<br />

knall ljud sound<br />

knalla gå långsamt vard walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

knalle liten rund höjd round form<br />

knapp skiva, kula round form<br />

knapra knastrande sound<br />

knarr ljud sound<br />

knarrig vresig bad mood<br />

knasig tokigt dum pejorative<br />

knast utväxt round form<br />

knastra ljud sound<br />

knata spr<strong>in</strong>ga vard walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

knatte liten dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

knatter ljud sound<br />

kneg nedsättande pejorative<br />

kneken förfallet pejorative<br />

knick tvär böj round form<br />

knipa ihop putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

knippa hophållna putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

knipsa nypa putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

knirka ljud sound<br />

knirra ljud sound<br />

knistra ljud sound<br />

knittra ljud sound<br />

knixa liten hastig q or s movement<br />

knocka slå beat<br />

knodd nedsättande pejorative<br />

knoga mödosamt gå walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

knoge upphöjn<strong>in</strong>g round form<br />

knollra smålockig round form<br />

knop knut round form<br />

knopp liten kula round form<br />

knorr liten vriden form round form<br />

knorr muttrande kurrande sound<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 6


knorva veckig rynkig pejorative<br />

knot knorrhane sound<br />

knota muttrande klaga talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

knota förtjockad round form<br />

knott liten mygga dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

knottra liten upphöjn<strong>in</strong>g round form<br />

knubbig kort tjock round form<br />

knuff stöt beat<br />

knut sammanfogn<strong>in</strong>g putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

knutte nedsätt pejorative<br />

knyck plötslig rörelse q or s movement<br />

knyckla ihop putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

knyppla flätas putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

knyst ljud sound<br />

knyta samman putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

knåp smått obetydligt dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

knäck brott slag beat<br />

knäppa ljud sound<br />

knödel bulle round form<br />

knöl m<strong>in</strong>dre rundad round form<br />

knöl otrevlig pejorative<br />

knös nedsätt pejorative<br />

krabat litet dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

kracka sönderdela destruction<br />

krackelera f<strong>in</strong>a sprickor destruction<br />

krafs värdelösa småsaker pejorative<br />

krafsa ljud sound<br />

krake beklagansvärd pejorative<br />

krakel högljutt sound<br />

krakmandel tunt bräckligt skal th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

kram värdelösa pejorative<br />

kram våt wetness<br />

krams värdelösa småsaker pejorative<br />

krans r<strong>in</strong>g sammanflätad w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

kras ljud sound<br />

krasch ljud sound<br />

kratsa rörelse q or s movement<br />

kratta fåra rough surface structure<br />

kratta dålig pejorative<br />

kravla mödosam förflytta walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

kraxa ljud sound<br />

kreta karva dålig pejorative<br />

krimskrams värdelösa småsaker pejorative<br />

kr<strong>in</strong>gelikrokar mängd krökar w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

kr<strong>in</strong>gla form av en båge w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

krock häftig sammanstötn<strong>in</strong>g beat<br />

krok långsmalt halvcirkel w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

krokan krusiduller, snirklig w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

krokett frasig th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 7


krossa sönder destruction<br />

kruka cyl<strong>in</strong>drisk bukig round form<br />

krulla små lockar w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

krum böjd w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

krumbukt kraftfull sl<strong>in</strong>grande rörelse w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

krumelur krokig l<strong>in</strong>je w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

krumpen böjd w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

krusa full av små vågor w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

kruserlig tillgjord pejorative<br />

krusiduller form sl<strong>in</strong>grande l<strong>in</strong>jer w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

krusta skal th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

krustad bakverk i form av bägare th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

krycka böjt w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

krylla rörlig walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

krypa förflytta sig walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

krysta samtal, formuler<strong>in</strong>g sound<br />

kråka fågel sound<br />

kråma stolt vrida w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

krångel besvärligt pejorative<br />

krås veckad remsa rough surface str<br />

kräk klandervärd pejorative<br />

kräkla <strong>in</strong>rullat krön w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

kräla förflytta sig sl<strong>in</strong>grande walk<strong>in</strong>g, w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

kräm tjockflytande wetness<br />

krämta harkla sound<br />

kränga vickande rörelser q or s movement<br />

kräpp tunt krusig yta rough surface str<br />

krök kraftig sväng w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

kvacka läte sound<br />

kvadda förstöra destruction<br />

kval lidande bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

kvalm osund äckel bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

kvarka sound<br />

kvarn nedbrytande destruction<br />

kvav kvalmig kväva bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

kverulans klaga talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

kvick små snabba rörelser q or s movement<br />

kvida ljud sound<br />

kvillra ljud sound<br />

kvirra kverulera sound<br />

kvissla liten blåsa dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

kvist liten smal dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

kvitter läte sound<br />

kväda sjunga sound<br />

kväka läte sound<br />

kvälja lukt äckla bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

kväva svårighet att andas bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

mjau läte sound<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 8


mjuk angenäm känsel jämn slät soft consistency<br />

mjäkig alltför vek pejorative<br />

mjäla mjölf<strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong><br />

mjäll f<strong>in</strong> konsistens soft consistency<br />

mjäll fjäll f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong><br />

mjöl f<strong>in</strong>mald f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong><br />

njugg onödigt snål pejorative<br />

pjatt obetydlig hålln<strong>in</strong>gslös pejorative<br />

pjoller men<strong>in</strong>gslöst prat pejorative, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

pjosk överdriven pejorative<br />

pjunk gnäll veklighet pejorative<br />

pjåkig dålig pejorative<br />

pladask klumpigt ljudligt fall sound<br />

pladder oavbrutet <strong>in</strong>nehållslöst prat talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

pladuska stor utbredn<strong>in</strong>g oönskad pejorative<br />

planka fyrsidigt stycke form<br />

plask häftigt vatten ljud sound, wetness<br />

platt ytor form<br />

pligg kort spetsigt stift form, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

pl<strong>in</strong>g ljud sound<br />

pl<strong>in</strong>t lådformigt avsmalnande form<br />

plira titta halvt tillslutna gaze<br />

plissé regelbunden f<strong>in</strong> veckn<strong>in</strong>g rough surface structure<br />

plit starkt vard. slang<br />

ploj skämtsamt spratt slang<br />

plomb bly, fylln<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

plottra spridda oväsentliga smådetaljer pejorative<br />

plufsig slappt fet pejorative<br />

plugg litet cyl<strong>in</strong>driskt form, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

plump fläck pejorative<br />

plump grov ohyfsad pejorative<br />

plums ljud sound, wetness<br />

plundra hänsynslöst pejorative<br />

plunta vard. slang<br />

plur<strong>in</strong>g starkt vard slang<br />

plurr vatten wetness<br />

plussig uppsvälld pejorative<br />

plutt liten dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

plym fjäder soft consistency<br />

plymå dyna soft consistency<br />

plysa luckra upp ull soft consistency<br />

plysch sammetsliknande långhårigt soft consistency<br />

plätt litet runt form, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

plös kilformigt form<br />

pracka lura pejorative<br />

prassel ljud sound<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 9


prat tal talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

prick mycket liten rundad platt form, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

prilla portion slang<br />

prillig småtokig slang<br />

propp föremål täppa hål öppn<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

propsa envist kräva talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

prunka lysande färger light<br />

pruta övertala talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

prutt fjärt sound<br />

pryl grovt nålformigt form<br />

pryl onyttig överflödig pejorative<br />

pryttel slang<br />

prål strålande light<br />

prång liten trång form, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

prångla tvivelaktigt knep pejorative<br />

pröjs vard. slang<br />

pröjsare vard slang<br />

skaka rörelse q or s movement<br />

skakel stång long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

skal hårt tunt hölje hardness<br />

skalk hård kant hardness<br />

skall läte sound<br />

skalla ljuda starkt sound<br />

skalle hårdhet hardness<br />

skallra ljud sound<br />

skalm avlång rörlig long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

skalpell rak long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

skalv darra q or s movement<br />

skare hårt ytskikt hardness<br />

skarp spetsig kant hardness<br />

skava hårt tryck föras upprepat hardness<br />

skavank fel pejorative<br />

skippa hoppa q or s movement<br />

skopa m<strong>in</strong>dre halvklotformigt round form<br />

skorpa hårdnad yta hardness<br />

skorr ljud sound<br />

skorvig yta hård ojämn hardness<br />

skorv gammal förfallen pejorative<br />

skott ljud sound<br />

skovel stor bred rundad round form<br />

skångra skaka starkt ljud sound<br />

skåra långsträckt fördjupn<strong>in</strong>g long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

skrabba gammal utsliten destruction<br />

skraj vard. slang<br />

skral dålig pejorative<br />

skraltig dålig svag destruction<br />

skramla ljud sound<br />

skranglig ostadig destruction<br />

skrap ljud sound<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 10


skratta ljud talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

skri läte starkt talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

skrocka läte talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

skrodera skryta talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

skrot skräp värdelöst destruction<br />

skrovlig grov ojämn yta rough surface str<br />

skrubb förvar<strong>in</strong>gsutrymme pejorative<br />

skrubba gnida hårt grov ojämn rough surface str<br />

skrubbor bannor talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

skrumpen förtorkad rynkig rough surface structure<br />

skrutt dåligt pejorative<br />

skrymsle litet utrymme dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

skrymt spökeri troll pejorative<br />

skrynkla oönskat veck rough surface structure<br />

skryta tala överdrivet talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

skrål pratande talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

skråma m<strong>in</strong>dre ytligt sår rough surface structure<br />

skrälla ljud sound<br />

skrälle gammalt nedslitet destruction<br />

skräna röster talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

skräp värdelös destruction<br />

skräppa skryta talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

skrävla skryta talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

skröna lögnaktig talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

skröplig svag ömtålig destruction<br />

skval ljud vatten sound, wetness<br />

skvaller löst prat talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

skvalp ljud sound<br />

skvalta stänka wetness<br />

skvimpa skvalpa små rörelser movement, wetness<br />

skvätt liten mängd vätska wetness<br />

slabba vätska slarv wetness, pejorative<br />

slabbertacka sladdertacka talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

slack alltför stor slakhet slackness<br />

sladd tamp long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sladda okontrollerat glida q or s movement<br />

sladdra o<strong>in</strong>tressant skvaller talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

sladdrig saknar styvhet slackness<br />

slafs kladdande wetness, pejorative<br />

slag hård träff ljud ton beat<br />

slak mjuk böjlig slackness<br />

slam vätska wetness<br />

slammer starkt buller sound<br />

slampa lösaktig slarvig pejorative<br />

slams värdelöst prat pejorative<br />

slamsa smal sladdrig remsa slackness, long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

slana smal böjlig slackness, long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

slang långt böjligt slackness, long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

slank smal long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 11


slankig alltför mjuk böjlig slackness<br />

slant långt, spö long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

slapp alltför mjuk slackness<br />

slarvig <strong>in</strong>te noggrann och ordentlig pejorative<br />

slas slö slarvig pejorative<br />

slasa gå tungt och hasande pejorative, walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

slask blöt sörja sopor wetness, pejorative<br />

slatt bottenskvätt wetness, pejorative<br />

slattrig slapp, pratig talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

slejf band long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

slem segt sekret wetness<br />

slicka flytande kletigt wetness<br />

slidder o<strong>in</strong>tressant skvaller talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

sliddrig sladdrig slackness<br />

slimmad <strong>in</strong>sydd long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sl<strong>in</strong>ga krök v<strong>in</strong>dl<strong>in</strong>g long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sl<strong>in</strong>ka förflytta sig snabbt q or s movement<br />

sl<strong>in</strong>ka prostituerad pejorative<br />

sl<strong>in</strong>kig sladdrig slackness<br />

sl<strong>in</strong>ta plötsligt glatthet q or s movement<br />

slipa yta slät gnida smooth surface str<br />

slipprig hal kladdig wetness, q or s movement<br />

slips band long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

slira okontrollerat glida q or s movement<br />

slisk äckligt kladdigt wetness, pejorative<br />

slita häftigt rycka q or s movement<br />

slits smal öppen ränna long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

slok odåga pejorative<br />

sloka slapp avlång böjlig slackness<br />

sludder otydligt tal talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

slugga boxas vilt q or s movement<br />

slum förfallet pejorative<br />

slunga kasta stor rörelse q or s movement<br />

slur<strong>in</strong>g soppa wetness<br />

slurk liten vätska wetness, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

slusk mycket smutsig ovårdad pejorative<br />

sluss vatten wetness<br />

slutta luta nedåt yta potential movement<br />

slyna ouppfostrad slarvig pejorative<br />

slyngel ouppfostrad pejorative<br />

slå slag beat<br />

släde medar long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

slägga stor tung hammare beat<br />

slänga kasta vårdslöst häftigt q or s movement<br />

slänt sluttn<strong>in</strong>g potential movement<br />

släntra gå långsamt utan mål walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

slät fri från ojämnhet yta smooth surface str<br />

slätt vidsträckt plant smooth surface str<br />

slö kraftlös håglös pejorative<br />

slödder föraktad ouppfostrad pejorative<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 12


slösa för mycket misshushålla pejorative<br />

smack ljud sound<br />

smal r<strong>in</strong>ga bredd narrow form<br />

smash snabbt hårt slag beat<br />

smask ljud sound<br />

smatt prång dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

smatter ljud sound<br />

smegma sekret wetness<br />

smet kladdig röra wetness<br />

smetana tjock grädde wetness<br />

smicker överdrivet pejorative<br />

sm<strong>in</strong>k krämig konsistens wetness<br />

smisk aga beat<br />

smita avlägsna sig obehagligt secrecy<br />

smock rynkn<strong>in</strong>g i rutmönster rough surface str<br />

smocka hårt slag beat<br />

smolk smutspartikel pejorative<br />

smuggla olovligt secrecy<br />

smula liten partikel torr lös dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

smussel i smyg olovligt secrecy<br />

smuts fläckar oönskade pejorative<br />

smutt liten klunk dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

smutt prång dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

smyga röra sig tyst och smidigt secrecy<br />

små r<strong>in</strong>ga utsträckn<strong>in</strong>g dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

smäck slag med handflata beat<br />

smäcker slank elegant narrow form<br />

smäda omdöme talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

smäkta <strong>in</strong>tensiv längtan mental feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

smäll aga beat<br />

smälta flytande wetness<br />

smärt smal smidig narrow form<br />

smärta plågsam känsla mental feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

smätta ljud sound<br />

smörj stryk beat<br />

smörja mjukt kräm wetness<br />

snabb quickness<br />

snabel mycket lång utdragen long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

snacka prata talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

snafs smuts pejorative<br />

snagga mycket kort small end form<br />

snappa kort snabb rörelse quickness<br />

snar snabb quickness<br />

snarka ljud sound<br />

snarpa läte sound<br />

snarra skorra sound<br />

snaskig sölig kladdig wetness, pejorative<br />

snatta m<strong>in</strong>dre värde pejorative<br />

snattra läte sound<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 13


snava falla snubbla walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

snegla titta ögon gaze<br />

snibb hörn trekantigt small end form<br />

snicksnack struntprat talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

(snigel) lång slemmig wetness, long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sniken girigt orättmätig pejorative<br />

snilja tråd long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

snip spetsig form small end form<br />

snirkel starkt böjd l<strong>in</strong>je spiral w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

snitsel smal remsa long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

snitt liten f<strong>in</strong>are dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

sno långsmalt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sno snabbt quickness<br />

snobb överdrivet fåfängt pejorative<br />

snodd garn long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

snofsa vard. slang<br />

snok långt smalt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

snopen besviken mental feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

snopp smal ända small end form<br />

snor slem wetness<br />

snorkel rör long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

snorkig oartig pejorative<br />

snubba tillrättavisn<strong>in</strong>g talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

snubbe vard. slang<br />

snubbla falla walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

snudig fl<strong>in</strong>k quickness<br />

snugga liten pipa dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

snultra långsträckt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

snurra vrida cirkelformig w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

snusa hörbart andas sound<br />

snusk smuts illaluktande pejorative<br />

snut vard slang<br />

snutt liten kort dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

snuva slem wetness<br />

snyfta stötvisa andetag sound<br />

snylta ytnyttja pejorative<br />

snyta fräsa sound<br />

snål överdrivet pejorative<br />

snäcka spiralvridet w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

snäppa ljud sound<br />

snärj högt tempo quickness<br />

snärp fågelläte sound<br />

snärt ända slag beat<br />

snärta nedsätt pejorative<br />

snäsa irriterad tillrättavisn<strong>in</strong>g talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

snöd fåfäng tarvlig pejorative<br />

snöpa stumpa, vanställa pejorative<br />

snöre garn long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

snörvla ljud sound<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 14


spackel degartat wetness<br />

spad vatten wetness<br />

spade platt th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spagat åt var sitt håll separation<br />

spaljé spjälor long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spalt lodrät long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spana blicken gaze<br />

spant balk long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spark hård stöt beat<br />

sparre bjälke long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sparris stam long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spasm ryckn<strong>in</strong>g q or s movement<br />

spat klyvbart glansigt th<strong>in</strong> form, light<br />

spatel platt avlångt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spatt tokig överdrift jocular<br />

spe illvilligt förlöjligande jocular<br />

spegel plan smooth surface str<br />

speja blicken gaze<br />

spektakel förargelse jocular<br />

spene utväxt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spenslig tunn smal long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sperma vätska wetness<br />

speta litet tunt vasst long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spets tunn ända avsmalnande long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spett lång stång spets long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spex parodisk dråplig jocular<br />

spigg taggar long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spik spetsig p<strong>in</strong>ne long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spila spjäla long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spilkum liten skål dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

spill blir över pejorative<br />

spilla vätska wetness<br />

sp<strong>in</strong>del långa ben long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sp<strong>in</strong>kig mycket tunn long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sp<strong>in</strong>na garn long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sp<strong>in</strong>na ljud sound<br />

spira smal stav long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spiral kurva vriden w<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g form<br />

spola vatten wetness<br />

spole avlångt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spol<strong>in</strong>g ouppfostrad pejorative<br />

spont utstående parti long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spotsk hånfullhet attitude<br />

spott vätska wetness<br />

spurt ökn<strong>in</strong>g av farten q or s movement<br />

sputum slem wetness<br />

spydig elaka kommentarer attitude, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

spån tunn avhyvlad bit th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 15


spång smal long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

späd liten dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

spänta stickor long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spö smal böjlig käpp long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spjut lång smal stång hård spets long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spjuver skämtar luras jocular<br />

spjäla ribba long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spjälka sönderdela destruction<br />

spl<strong>in</strong>ta klyva separation<br />

split oenighet separation<br />

splits ändar av tågvirke separation<br />

splitter små vass slagits sönder destruction, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

spraka knastra gnistra sound, light<br />

spralla kroppsligen livlig q or s movement<br />

spratt skämtsam lura jocular<br />

sprattla rörelser q or s movement<br />

sprej f<strong>in</strong>fördelat vätska wetness, separation<br />

spreta åt olika håll separation<br />

spri smäckert rundhult long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spricka långsmalt brott long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sprida fördela utbredn<strong>in</strong>g separation<br />

spr<strong>in</strong>ga lång mycket smal öppn<strong>in</strong>g long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spr<strong>in</strong>ga förflytta sig walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

spr<strong>in</strong>kler vatten, sprider wetness, separation<br />

spr<strong>in</strong>t p<strong>in</strong>ne long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sprits strut form long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spritta plötsligt rycka q or s movement<br />

sprudla bubblande välla fram q or s movement, wetness<br />

sprund smal öppn<strong>in</strong>g long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spruta häftigt stråle vätska q or s movement, wetness<br />

sprutt fart vard. q or s movement<br />

spränga stor kraft splittras separation<br />

sprätt överdrivet pejorative<br />

sprätta kr<strong>in</strong>gkastande separation<br />

spröjs list long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

spröt långt, smalt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stabbig kort kraftig short form<br />

stake lång käpp long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stamma tala stötigt talk<strong>in</strong>g, iterative<br />

stampa stöta hårt ljudligt sound, iterative<br />

stappla gå ostadig walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

stav långt smalt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stepp ljudliga slag sound, beat<br />

stick smärtsamt vass bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sticka tunn vass flisa long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 16


stift smalt spetsigt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stig smal long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stilett stickvapen smal long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stim ljud röster talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

st<strong>in</strong>g smärtsam vass bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

st<strong>in</strong>t blick gaze<br />

stirra titta gaze<br />

stirrig upprörd virrig mental feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

stock stam long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stoft små partiklar f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong><br />

stoj högljutt prat talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

stolle tok pejorative<br />

stolpe påle long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stubb uppstickande strån short form<br />

stubbe uppstickande rest short form<br />

studsa rörelse q or s movement, iterative<br />

stulta tulta walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

stump återstående bit short form<br />

stumpa liten dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

stura tjura attitude<br />

stursk trotsig fräck attitude<br />

stybb f<strong>in</strong>fördelat spill f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong><br />

stycka hugga i bitar separation<br />

stylta stång long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stympa avhuggn<strong>in</strong>g short form<br />

styng smärtsam vasst bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

styv ej lätt böjs stiffness<br />

stång avlängt rakt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stånka läte talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

stänka vätska wetness<br />

stöddig stor kraftig alltför självsäker attitude, pejorative<br />

stön ljud sound<br />

stöppla stöta q or s movement<br />

stör stång long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stöt rörelse kraftigt q or s movement<br />

stötta stolpe long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stöv hudavlagr<strong>in</strong>g f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong><br />

strak rak stel stiffness<br />

stram spänd stiffness<br />

streamer långsmal remsa long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

streck kortare l<strong>in</strong>je long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stretch töjbar long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

strigel läderrem long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stril ljud av vätska long th<strong>in</strong> form, sound, wetness<br />

strimla liten långsmal bit long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

strimma långsmalt band ljus long th<strong>in</strong> form, light<br />

stripa rak hårtest long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stropp ögleformad round form<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 17


strosa lugnt promenera walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

strul besvärligt pejorative<br />

strunt värdelöst pejorative<br />

strut konformigt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

strutta gå knyckigt walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

strå stjälk long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stråk bandliknande long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

stråle ljus long th<strong>in</strong> form, light<br />

stråt väg bana long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sträcka längd long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sträng spänd tråd long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

sträv yta ojämnhet rough surface str<br />

strö f<strong>in</strong>fördelat material f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong><br />

ström r<strong>in</strong>nande vatten long th<strong>in</strong> form, wetness<br />

strössel avlånga korn f<strong>in</strong>e gra<strong>in</strong>, long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

ströva långsamt gå walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

svabba tvätta långa garnändar wetness, long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

svacka större fördjupn<strong>in</strong>g bent form<br />

svada tala länge talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

svaja svänga fram och tillbaka q or s movement<br />

svalla röra sig häftigt q or s movement<br />

svamla prata strunt talk<strong>in</strong>g. pejorative<br />

svank <strong>in</strong>åtböjt bent form<br />

svans långsmal long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

svassa tillgjort pejorative, walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

svepa rörelse q or s movement<br />

svett vätska wetness<br />

svicka tapp short form<br />

svida brännande smärta bodily feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

svikt böjlighet bent form<br />

sv<strong>in</strong>g rörelse q or s movement<br />

svirvel vridbar q or s movement<br />

svischa ljud sound<br />

svulstig alltför pejorative<br />

svämma vatten q or s movement, wetness<br />

sväng rörelse q or s movement<br />

svära yttra talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

svärm flygande m<strong>in</strong>dre q or s movement<br />

sväva rörelse q or s movement<br />

tradig långtråkig pejorative<br />

traggel ständig tröttsam upprepn<strong>in</strong>g pejorative<br />

trampa rörelse walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

trams dumt prat pejorative, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

trasa sönderrivet destruction<br />

traska gå vårdslöst klumpigt walk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

trassel virrvarr krångel pejorative<br />

tratt strutformig pip tratt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

trava röra sig walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 18


tredsk envis ovillighet bad mood<br />

trilla falla fall<strong>in</strong>g<br />

trilla forma runt round form<br />

trilsk enveten motsträvig bad mood<br />

tr<strong>in</strong>d klotrund form round form<br />

tripp kortare dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

trippa gå små lätta hoppande walk<strong>in</strong>g, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

trips liten dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

trissa liten rund round form, dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

trist enformig nedstämd bad mood<br />

troll ful klumpig enfaldig pejorative<br />

tromb häftig luftvirvel q or s movement<br />

tromla roterande cyl<strong>in</strong>drisk q or s movement<br />

tross l<strong>in</strong>a long h<strong>in</strong> form<br />

trubbel besvär problem bad mood<br />

trubbig <strong>in</strong>te avsmalnande short form<br />

trudelutt glad melodi(stump) short form<br />

trulig trumpen bad mood<br />

truls liten slarvig pejorative<br />

trumla roterande q or s movement<br />

trumma slag<strong>in</strong>strument sound<br />

trumpen butter och missnöjd bad mood<br />

trumpet blås<strong>in</strong>strument sound<br />

trunk stor augmentative<br />

truta skjuta fram läpparna long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

tryne nos long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

tråd tunt utdraget long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

tråg avlångt fyrkantigt long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

tråkig enformig negativ bad mood<br />

trål strutformig long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

tråna känna stark längtan mental feel<strong>in</strong>g<br />

trång m<strong>in</strong>sta dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

träda långsamma steg walk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

träns snöre long th<strong>in</strong> form<br />

träsk vatten wetness<br />

träta irriterad ordväxl<strong>in</strong>g bad mood, talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tv<strong>in</strong>g sammanhålln<strong>in</strong>g putt<strong>in</strong>g together<br />

tv<strong>in</strong>na sno ihop trådar putt<strong>in</strong>g together, long th<strong>in</strong> for<br />

vrak skadat odugligt destruction<br />

vrede stark känsla förorättad bad mood<br />

vrensk själsligt motsträvig bad mood<br />

vresig ovänlig missnöje bad mood<br />

vret liten dim<strong>in</strong>utive<br />

vricka kraftigt vrida q or s movement<br />

vrida runt rörelse q or s movement<br />

vrål skrik talk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

vrång sur motsträvig bad mood<br />

vräka handlöst kasta q or s movement<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 19


vräkig överdrivet pejorative<br />

vränga vända fel pejorative<br />

vrövel struntprat talk<strong>in</strong>g, pejorative<br />

Appendix 1 Sida 20


Appendix 2<br />

Some <strong>in</strong>terjections for 9 different languages. (Swedish and English are written <strong>in</strong> ord<strong>in</strong>ary orthography)<br />

'Pejorative'<br />

Swedish Icelandic English Polish Hungarian F<strong>in</strong>nish Ososo Malagasi Slovenian<br />

usch [Oj] oh [O] [o:] [h¨i] [fjO] [dE] (nasal [fOI]<br />

hu [Ojbjak:] pooh [Oj] [u:] [h¨h¨h] [pçO]<br />

E) [fEI]<br />

"t" [|] [u:] phew [ox] [jOj] [hui] [œh] [ts]<br />

blä<br />

ugh<br />

[juj] [|]<br />

whistle<br />

ha<br />

ooh<br />

[fui]<br />

[uœk]<br />

håhå (jaja)<br />

tut [|]<br />

[|]+gesture whistle<br />

tvi<br />

boo<br />

öh<br />

äh<br />

bah<br />

asch<br />

äsch<br />

isch<br />

uh<br />

urrk<br />

fy<br />

bu<br />

tss


'positive' (appreciation, joy etc)<br />

Swedish Icelandic English Polish Hungarian F<strong>in</strong>nish Ososo Malagasi Slovenian<br />

tjo(hej) [vau] oh [O] [oh] [ah] [ah] - -<br />

ah [jipiO] ah [m:] kiss<strong>in</strong>g [m:] sigh<br />

åh<br />

wow<br />

sound + [jip:i]<br />

haha(ha)<br />

yippee<br />

gesture<br />

hihi(hi)<br />

[|]<br />

m:<br />

'surprise'<br />

Swedish Icelandic English Polish Hungarian F<strong>in</strong>nish Ososo Malagasi Slovenian<br />

oj [ha] oh [O:] [je:] [o:] [Ou] [a] [Oho:]<br />

åh<br />

ah<br />

[o:] [m:] [aj]<br />

(positive)<br />

ä<br />

whoops<br />

[h¨]<br />

[Ox]<br />

åhå<br />

[O:]<br />

hoppsan<br />

[OI]<br />

(negative)<br />

[jOI]<br />

(negative)<br />

'other bodily and mental feel<strong>in</strong>g'<br />

Swedish Icelandic English Polish Hungarian F<strong>in</strong>nish Ososo Malagasi Slovenian<br />

aj [aj] oh [Ox] [au] [aj] [Ou] [aj] [aIs]<br />

('pa<strong>in</strong>') [Ou] ow [O] [jaj]<br />

[euo:]<br />

[ux]<br />

[au] ouch<br />

[juj]


- -<br />

shiver<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sound<br />

[br:] shiver<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sound<br />

[O]<br />

[Oj]<br />

brrh<br />

ugh<br />

[ı]<br />

(voiceless)<br />

[br:]<br />

[hm] - [hm] - - -<br />

hm<br />

[ahem]<br />

hm:<br />

m:<br />

- -<br />

- - - - work<br />

songs<br />

hm<br />

(<strong>in</strong>gressive)<br />

brr<br />

burr<br />

('freez<strong>in</strong>g')<br />

hm<br />

('thoughtfulness')<br />

åhej('åhå')<br />

('jo<strong>in</strong>t<br />

effort')<br />

[nu]<br />

[huhuh] expiration - -<br />

phew [ux] [´:]<br />

[h¨]<br />

[F]<br />

[´f:]<br />

sighs<br />

- -<br />

[ajE] - - - - [euo:]<br />

[Oia]<br />

- [aha:] - [aha:]<br />

- [aha]<br />

[jOj]<br />

- aha<br />

haha<br />

oho<br />

<strong>in</strong>halation - - - [h¨i] silence - [Ox]<br />

[at·Cu:] atishoo [apCik] [haptsi] - [itC·imi] - [a·tCix]<br />

[tCixah]<br />

puh<br />

pust<br />

('exhaustion')<br />

ojojoj<br />

vojvoj<br />

håhåjaja<br />

('compla<strong>in</strong><br />

-<strong>in</strong>g')<br />

aha<br />

('sudden<br />

<strong>in</strong>sight')<br />

iiih<br />

('fear')<br />

atjo<br />

('sneeze')


- [m:] (with<br />

ris<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tonation)<br />

[n´mn´m]<br />

yummy [njamnjam] - [m:] smack<strong>in</strong>g<br />

noise<br />

[m:]<br />

[nam]<br />

[namEnamE]<br />

swear<strong>in</strong>g<br />

- - - - maybe<br />

imitation<br />

of animal<br />

mums<br />

namnam<br />

m:<br />

('good<br />

taste')<br />

grr<br />

('anger')<br />

[gr:]<br />

[Oh] (with<br />

coarse<br />

voice)<br />

'commands to animals'<br />

Swedish Icelandic English Polish Hungarian F<strong>in</strong>nish Ososo Malagasi Slovenian<br />

schas [S:] shoo [S:] [hœS] (to [S:] [Cu:] [uS:] [pC:]<br />

('go +gesture<br />

<strong>in</strong>sects)<br />

(common<br />

away') [tvi:tvi:]<br />

[Sits]<br />

command<br />

[hOuhOu] (to<br />

to several<br />

sheep)<br />

animals<br />

"march")<br />

pull pull<br />

- - - - - - -<br />

(to hens)<br />

('come<br />

here')<br />

[gibagiba gib]<br />

(voiceless<br />

[b]) (to<br />

sheep)<br />

[putaputaput]<br />

(to hens)


'commands to people'<br />

Swedish Icelandic English Polish Hungarian F<strong>in</strong>nish Ososo Malagasi Slovenian<br />

jaja [fOs:] now now [njE] (=no) [œlœ] (verb [kai]<br />

aja(baja) [svei] no no<br />

negation)<br />

ajaj [sveiDr] naughty<br />

(mild<br />

naughty<br />

warn<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to<br />

children)<br />

sch [¨s˘] sh [tC:] [tCit] [S:] gesture [S:] -<br />

(be quiet)<br />

[kuS:] +gesture<br />

pst [hai] here whistl<strong>in</strong>g [he:] [pst] (<strong>in</strong> family [e] -<br />

(soft call [¨u] (=du) hey<br />

play) name<br />

for<br />

[EnE]=you<br />

attention)<br />

pst or<br />

whistl<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(to<br />

prostitute)<br />

bu [ba] boo [bu:] [hu] [b¨] [dja] [a] (coarse -<br />

(to scare<br />

[bux] [bu]<br />

voice)<br />

some-<br />

[´] (coarse<br />

body)<br />

voice)<br />

vyss vyss [bi¨mbi¨m] hushaby - - - [S:] - -<br />

lull lull [s¨s¨˘s¨s¨˘]<br />

(quietly)<br />

(putt<strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sb to<br />

sleep)


_ -<br />

[¨u] (=du) [EwE] (not<br />

to adults)<br />

[Ei]<br />

ohoj<br />

hoho<br />

(louder<br />

call for<br />

attention)

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!