Academia.eduAcademia.edu
T.C. Mersin Oniversitesi Sosyal Bilimler Enstitiisii ingiliz Dili ve Edebiyat1 Anabilim Dah 14 ?-J 2 PSYCH VERBS IN TURKISH: A STRUCTURAL AND SEMANTIC ANALYSIS Ptnar iBE Dam~man Dos;. Dr. Mustafa AKSAN YUKSEK LiSANS TEZi Mersin, 2004 Mersin Universitesi Sosyal Bilimler Enstitusu MudurlUgline, Bu yah~ma jurimiz tarafmdan ingiliz Dili ve Edebiyati Anabilim Dah 'nda YOKSEK LiSANS TEZi olarak kabul edilmi~tir. Ba~kan _ _ _r::J_r~-----Do9. Dr. Ye~im AKSAN Doy. Dr. Omit Deniz TURAN Dye~ :.nr:MU:fuAK.sAN (Dan1~man) Onay Yukandaki imzalarm ad1 geyen ogretim elemanlarma ait olduklanm onaylanm. ~- - ------~ c~ - r-·~-r Prof. Dr. Serra DURUGONUL Enstitu Mud1irii 08/07/2004 11 OZET Bu c;ah~ma, konu rolleri ve sozdizim konumlan arasmda duzenli bir ortii~menin varhgmt onvarsayan baglama/ ortu~me kuramlanna evrensel bir bic;imde aykm davrant~ gosteren bir grup eylem olan ruh durumu eylemlerinin Ttirkc;edeki gorunumlerinin betimsel bir c;ozumlemesini sunar ve bu eylemler ic;in ozgun bir smif1andtrma onerir. <;ah~manm gurubunun, bu anlama ilk boltimu, davranl~lnl ili~kin yakla~tmlan <;ah~manm suad1~1 ac;tklamaya davrant~ c;ah~an, gosteren bu ey lemlerden bir farkh olc;utlere dayandmlmt~ yapt ve ozetler. ikinci boltimu, Turkc;e ruh durumu eylemlerini, uyelerinin konu rolleri, durum ekleri ve sozdizim konumlarmt temel alan, onerilen bir smlf1andtrma c;erc;evesinde inceler. Bu stmflandtrmaya gore, Turkc;ede, ilk ikisi durum ekli gec;i~siz gec;i~li, digerleri farkh dort gurup ruh durumu eyleminden soz edilebilir. Sorunlu ikinci gurup eylemler, gozlemlenen evrensel egilimlere benzerlik gosterir. Yalmzca Deneyimleyen uyesine sahip dorduncu gurup eylemler ise, alanyazmda ruh durumu eylemleri incelenmi~ dillerin hic;birinde yer almamalan sebebiyle Turkc;eye ozgu gorunmektedir. <;ah~ma Turkc;ede son derece verimli bir bic;imde ruh durumu bildirme i~levi ustlenen bile~ik yaptlan, anlambilim ve durum temelli bir yakla~tmla inceler. <;ah~manm son boltimu ise ruh durumu bildiren stfatlann ttiretim ozelliklerini bi<;im ve anlam yonunden ele ahr. 111 Tiirk9edeki ruh durumu eylemlerinin ettirgen, edilgen, yapllarda nas1l bir davram~ sergiledigine onerilen bu smlflandmna olu~turur. ili~kin tiirn donii~lii, i~te~ tartl~malarm ve bile~ik temelini sozi.i edilen IV ABSTRACT This study gives a descriptive account of psychological state verbs in Turkish which seem to be universally problematic for linking/ mapping theories that assume a unified mapping between the thematic roles and the syntactic positions; proposing an original classification. The first section of the study summarizes the structural and semantic approaches which attempt to explain the unexpected behavior of a group of psych verbs based on different criteria. In the second section, Turkish psych verbs are analyzed through the proposed classification based on the thematic roles, the case marking and the syntactic position of the arguments. According to the classification, there are four groups of psych verbs in Turkish; the first and the second are transitive and the others are intransitive with different case marked Theme objects. The problematic second group conforms to the observed universal tendencies. The fourth group with an only Experiencer argument; on the other hand, seems to be the only group which does not exist in other languages in the literature whose psych verbs are analyzed. The compound forms which seem to be a very productive way of conveying psych senses in Turkish are also examined with a semantic and case marking based approach. v Lastly, the derivational properties of the psych adjectives are also examined both morphologically and semantically. The mentioned proposed classification constitutes the basis of all discussions analyzing the behavior of Turkish psych verbs with respect to causativity, passivization, reflexivity, reciprocity and compound forms. VI CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGEMENT ........................................................................... i OZET ................................................................................................... .ii ABSTRACT ........................................................................................... .iv CONTENTS .............. ............................................................................. vi THE LIST OF TABLES ...................................... .. ............ . ..... ........ ........... xi THE LIST OF FIGURES ................................................... ...... .............. xviii INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................... 1 Background Information .............................................................................. 1 The Purpose of the Study .............................................................................. 8 Research Questions ......................................................................... .. ......... 9 Hypotheses ............................................................................................. 9 Data Collection Techniques and Limitations ..................................................... 10 The Method of Analysis ............................................................................. 10 I. REVIEW OF LITERATURE ..................................... ...................... 12 I. 1. Argument Structure ....................................................................... 12 I. 1. 1. Transitivity and the Unaccusative Phenomenon ................................ 23 I. I. 2. Voice Alternations and Causativity .............................................. 27 I. 2. Approaches to Psych Verbs .............................................................. 33 I. 2. 1. Structural Approaches to Psych Verbs ................................................ 34 I. 2. 1. 1. Belletti and Rizzi ( 1998) and the Psych Verbs in Italian ............. 34 I. 2. I. 2. Pesetsky ( 1987) and the Binding Problems with Experiencer Verbs ..................................................................................................... 42 Vll I. 2. 1. 3. Vanhoe (2002) and the Psych Verbs in Spanish ...................... .47 I. 2. 1. 4. Mulder (1992) and the Unergative and Ergative Psych Verbs ..... .48 I. 2. 1. 5. Hale and Keyser (2002) on Psych Verbs .............................. .49 I. 2. 1. 6. Ginnis (nd.) on the Morphological Restrictions in Experiencer Predicates .............................................................................................. 51 I. 2. 1. 7. Kural (1996) on the Elementary Predicates and the Psych Verbs in Turkish ...................................... , .......................................................... 52 I. 2. 2. Semantic and Aspectual Approaches to Psych Verbs ............................... 57 I. 2. 2. 1. Grimshaw (1990) Prominence Theory and Psych Verbs ............. 58 I. 2. 2. 2. Tenny (1994) Aspectual Interface Hypothesis and Psych Verbs .... 59 I. 2. 2. 3. Voorst ( 1992) and the Aspectual Semantics of Psych Verbs ........ 63 I. 2. 2. 4. Filip (1996) on the Psych Verbs in Czech .................... ; ......... 66 I. 2. 2. 5. Arad (nd.) on Psych Verbs ................................................ 68 I. 2. 2. 6. Levin and Hovav (2002) on Psych Verbs .............................. 70 I. 2. 2. 7. Dowty (1991) on Proto Roles and Psych Verbs ....................... 71 I. 2. 2. 8. Hatory (1997) and the LCSs of Psych Verbs .......................... 72 I. 2. 2. 9. Wechsler ( 199 5) Notion Rule and Psych Verbs ....................... 75 I. 2. 2. 10. Krifka (2001) on Psych Verbs ........................................... 77 I. 2. 2. 11. Kordoni (2000 and 2001) on the Psych Verbs in Modem Greek ... 79 I. 2. 2. 12. Dabrowska (1996) on the Dative and Nominative Experiencers in Polish ................................................................................................... 82 II. ANALYSIS OF THE PSYCH VERBS IN TURKISH .............................. 84 II. 1. II. 1. 1. What is Psych? ..................................................................... 84 The Experiencer Argument ............................................... 86 viii II. 1. 2. The Theme Argument ..................................................... 87 II. 1. 3. The Input of Psych Verbalization ....................................... 88 II. 1. 4. Secondary Psych Senses .................................................. 93 II. 2. Psych Verb Types in Turkish .................................................... 94 II. 2. 1. Type 1 Psych Verbs ....................................................... 96 II. 2. 2. Type 2 Psych Verbs ....................................................... 98 II. 2. 3. Type 3 Psych Verbs ...................................................... 100 II. 2. 3. 1. Type 3a Psych Verbs .......................................................... 100 II. 2. 3. 2. Type 3b Psych Verbs ......................................................... 102 Type 4 Psych Verbs ....................................................................... 107 II. 2. 4. II. 3. Voice and Psych Verbs in Turkish ..................................................................... 109 II. 3. 1. Psych Verbs and Transitivity in Turkish .................. 110 II. 3. 2. Psych Verbs and Causativity in Turkish .................. 112 ~L II. 3. 2. 1. Morphologically Causative Psych Verbs ................. .J 14 II. 3. 2. 1. 1. Type 1 Psych Verbs and Morphological Causativity ...... .114 II. 3. 2. 1. 2. Type 2 Psych Verbs and Morphological Causativity ....... 122 II. 3. 2. 1. 3. Type 3a Psych Verbs and Morphological Causativity ...... 126 II. 3. 2. 1. 4. Type 3b Psych Verbs and Morphological Causativity ...... 129 II. 3. 2. 1. 5. Type 4 Psych Verbs and Morphological Causativity ....... 133 II. 3. 2. 2. Periphrastic Causative Psych Verbs ........................ 136 II. 3. 2. 2. 1. Type 1 Psych Verbs and Periphrastic Causativity .......... 136 II. 3. 2. 2. 2. Type 2 Psych Verbs and Periphrastic Causativity .......... 139 II. 3. 2. 2. 3. Type 3a Psych Verbs and Periphrastic Causativity ......... 141 II. 3. 2. 2. 4. Type 3b Psych Verbs and Periphrastic Causativity ......... 142 lX II. 3. 2. 2. 5. II. 3. 2. 3. Causativity and the Permissive Reading .................. 144 II. 3. 2. 4. Control, Causativity and the Psych Events ............... 146 II. 3. 3. Psych Verbs and Passivization in Turkish ........................ .150 II. 3. 3. 1. Type 1 Psych Verbs and Passivization ....................... .151 II. 3. 3. 2. Type 2 Psych Verbs and Passivization ........................ 153 II. 3. 3. 3. Type 3a Psych Verbs and Passivization ...................... 156 II. 3. 3. 4. Type 3b Psych Verbs and Passivization ..................... .158 II. 3. 3. 5. Type 4 Psych Verbs and Passivization ........................ 159 II. 3. 4. Psych Verbs and Reflexivity in Turkish ............................ 160 II. 3. 4. 1. Type 1 Psych Verbs and Reflexivity ......................... .160 II. 3. 4. 2. Type 2 Psych Verbs and Reflexivity .......................... .161 II. 3. 4. 3. Type 3a Psych Verbs and Reflexivity ........................ 164 II. 3. 4. 4. Type 3b Psych Verbs and Reflexivity ........................ 164 II. 3. 4. 5. Type 4 Psych Verbs and Reflexivity ......................... .165 II. 3. 5. II. 4. Type 4 Psych Verbs and Periphrastic Causativity .......... 143 Psych Verbs and Reciprocity ......................................... 166 Compound Psych Use-sin Turkish .... ; ............................................... .167 II. 4. 1. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with a Nominative Psych Nominal .............................................................................................. 169 II. 4. 2. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with an Accusative Psych Nominal .............................................................................................. 175 II. 4. 3. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with a Dative Psych Nominal ............................................................................................. 177 X II. 4. 4. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with a Complex Nucleus ......................................................................................................... 180 II. 4. 5. II. 5. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with an Adjective .......... 180 Psych Adjective Derivation .................................................... 184 II. 5. 1. Psych Adjectives Derived from Verbal Roots ........................ 185 II. 5. 2. Psych Adjectives Derived from Adjectival Roots ................... 191 II. 5. 3. Psych Adjectives Derived from Nominal Roots ..................... 191 II. 5. 4. Non- Derived Psych Adjectives ........................................ 194 II. 5. 5. Compound Psych Adjectives ............................................ 195 CONCLUSION .................................................................................. ... 197 BIBLIOGRAPHY .................................................................................. 202 XI THE LIST OFTABLES I. 1. Table 1. Dowty (1991) Thematic Roles and the Syntactic Realization Rules ......... 19 1.1.1. Table 1. Syntactic Accusativity ............................................................ .25 Table 2. Syntactic Ergativity ................................................................ 25 Table 3. Nakipoglu (1998) Unaccusatives and Unergatives ............................. 27 I. 2. Table 1. Belletti and Rizzi (1988) Psych Verbs in Italian .............................. .41 Table 2. Vanhoe (2002) Psych Verbs in Spanish ........................................ .48 Table 3. Mulder (1992) s- psych verbs and o-psych verbs ............................. .48 Table 4. Kural (1996) Affectedness and Protogonism .................................... 53 Table 5. Kural (1996) Affectedness, Protogonism and Thematic Roles ............... 53 Table 6. Kural (1996) Thematic Combinations with the Experiencer Argument .... 54 Table 7. Dabrowska (1994) The two Aspects of Mental Experiences .................. 82 Table 8. Dabrowska (1994) Nominative and Dative Experiencers ..................... 82 II. 1. Table 1. Psych Noun+ -!An= Psych Verb ................................................. 88 Table 2. Psych! Non-Psych Noun+ -!A= Psych Verb .................................... 89 Table 3. Verb-> Psych Noun-> Psych Verb ............................................. 89 Table 4. Verb -> Psych Adjective -> Psych Verb ......................................... 90 Table 5. Verb+ Seemingly Causative Morpheme= Psych Verb .......................... 90 Table 6. Verb+ Seemingly Passive Morpheme= Psych Verb ............................ 90 Table 7. Verb+ In! 1$= Psych Verb ......................................................... 91 xu lA~= Table 8. Psych Adjective+ Psych Verb .............................................. 91 Table 9. Psych/ Non-Psych Adjective+ -(!An)= Psych Verb ........................... 92 Table 10. Psych/ Non-Psych Adjective+ -sA= Psych Verb ............................... 92 Table 11. Non-Psych Adjective+ -Arl AI= Psych Verb ................................... 92 Table 12. Verbs which have both psych and non psych senses ........................... 94 II. 2. Table 1. Psych Verb Types in Turkish ..................................................... 96 II. 3. Table 1. Voice in Turkish .................................................................. 109 Table 2. Tietze ( 1989) Medioreflexive Verbs ........................................... 110 II. 3. 1. Table 1. Psych Verbs and Other Intransitives ............................................ 110 Table 2. Transitivity in Turkish Psych Verbs ............................................ 110 Table 3. Optional Argument Deletion of Base Type 3a Verbs ........................ 111 Table 4. Optional Argument Deletion of Derived Type 3a Verbs .................... 111 Table 5. Optional Argument Deletion of Type 3b Verbs .............................. 111 II. 3. 2. 1. Table 1. Type 1 Psych Verbs and Causativity ............. ; ............................. 114 Table 2. Type 1 Causative Psych Construction .......................................... 116 Table 3. Type 1 Causative Psych Construction (Revised) .............................. 120 Table 4. Type 2 Psych Verbs and Causativity .......................................... .122 Table 5. Type 2 Causative Psych Construction .......................................... 125 Table 6. Type 3a Psych Verbs and Causativity ......................................... 126 Table 7. Type 1 and Type 3a Causative Psych Construction (Comparison) ......... 126 Xlll Table 8. Type 1 and Type 3a Causative Psych Construction (Differences) .......... 127 Table 9. Type Type 3a Causative Psych Construction .................................. 128 Table 10. Type 3b Psych Verbs and Causativity .......................................... 130 Table 11. Type 3b Causative Psych Construction ........................................ 132 Table 12. Type 4 Psych Verbs and Causativity ........................................... 133 Table 13. Type 4 Causative Psych Construction .......................................... 134 Table 14. Psych Verbs which Behave like the Lexical Causatives of Type 2 ........ 135 II. 3. 3. Table 1. Passivization in Psych Verbs ..................................................... 150 Table 2. Type 1 Psych Verbs with -II ..................................................... 151 Table 3. Type 1 Psych Verbs with -(l)n .................................................. 152 Table 4. Type 1 Passive non psych Verbs ................................................ 152 Table 5. Passive and Active Psych Verbs ................................................ 152 Table 6. Active Type 2- Passive Type 3a Verbs ......................................... 156 Table 7. Type 3b Verbs with Passive Morphemes with a Reflexive Meaning ...... 158 Table 8. Type 4 Passive Verbs with a Reflexive Meaning ............................. 159 II. 3. 4. Table 1. Some Type 1 Verbs and Reflexivity ............................................ 160 Table 2. Type 2 Verbs and Reflexivity ................................................... 162 Table 3. Seemingly Reflexive Psych Verbs ............................................. 166 II. 4. Table 2. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nom. Psych Nominal (2) .......................................................................................................... 168 Table 3. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nom. Psych Nominal (3) .......................................................................................................... 169 XIV Table 4. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nom. Psych Nominal (4) .......................................................................................................... 169 Table 5. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nom. Psych Nominal (5) .......................................................................................................... 170 Table 6. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nom. Psych Nominal (6) ........................................................................................................... 170 Table 7. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nom. Psych Nominal (7) ........................................................................................................... 171 Table 8. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nom. Psych Nominal (8) ......................................................................................................... .171 Table 9. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nom. Psych Nominal (9) .......................................................................................................... 172 Table 10. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nom. Psych Nominal (10) ......................................................................................................... 172 Table 11. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nom. Psych Nominal (11) ... . ························· .................... ··············· ··························· .............. 173 Table 12. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nom. Psych Nominal (12) .......... . .............................................................................................. 174 Table 13. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nom. Psych Nominal (13) .......................................................................................................... 174 Table 14. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with an Ace. Psych Nominal (1) .......................................................................................................... 175 Table 15. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with an Ace. Psych Nominal (2) ....................................................................................... ....... ... . ........ 176 Table 16 . Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with an Ace. Psych Nominal (3) ..................................................................................... . .................... 176 Table 17. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with an Ace. Psych Nominal (4) .......................................................................................................... 176 Table 18. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dat. Psych Nominal (1) . ........................................................................................................ 177 Table 19. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dat. Psych Nominal (2) ..................................... ······ ·········· ................. ················ ······ ............. 177 XV Table 20. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dat. Psych Nominal (3) ......................................................................................................... 177 Table 21. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dat. Psych Nominal (4) ......................................................................................................... 178 Table 22. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dat. Psych Nominal (5) ......................................................................................................... 178 Table 23. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dat. Psych Nominal (6) ... ........ .. ............................................................................................ 179 Table 24 . Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dat. Psych Nominal (7) ......................................................................................................... 179 Table 25. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dat. Psych Nominal (8) ......................................................................................................... 179 Table 26. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Complex Nucleus (1) ............ ........................................................................................... .. 180 Table 27. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with an Adjective (1) ............... 181 Table 28. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with an Adjective (2) ......... ;,: .... 181 Table 29. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with an Adjective (3) ............... 181 Table 30. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with an Adjective (4) ............... 182 Table 31. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with an Adjective (5) .. .. .. .. ...... . 182 Table 32. A New Classification for Turkish Psych Verbs Including Compound Forms ......................................................................................................... 182 II. 5. Table 1. Psych Adjectives Derived with -Gan ............................. ... .......... 185 Table 2. Psych Adjectives Derived with -GI + c/1 III siz .. ............................ 186 Table 3. Psych Adjectives Derived with-gif ............................................ 186 Table 4. Psych Adjectives Derived with -Ik ............................................. 186 Table 5. Psych Adjectives Derived with --Inti+ (l/1 slz) .............................. 186 Table 6. Psych Adjectives Derived with -Inr; + lll siz .................................. 187 Table 7. Psych Adjectives Derived with-/~ + III slz .................................... 187 Table 8. Psych Adjectives Derived with -It+ /II slz .................................... 187 XVI Table 9. Psych Adjectives Derived with -Ar + III slz ................................... 187 Table 10. Psych Adjectives Derived with -eA +III slz .......... ......................... 187 Table 11. Psych Adjectives Derived with -DA + II ....................................... 188 Table 12. Psych Adjectives Derived with -sf ............................................. 188 Table 13. Psych Adjectives Derived with -Ar doner ...................................... 188 Table 14. Psych Adjectives Derived with -kaloz ......................................... 188 Table 15. Psych Adjectives Derived with -DIRI -t + lei ................................. 189 Table 16. Psych Adjectives Derived with- Ill n + mA + dlk ........................... 189 Table 17. Psych Adjectives Derived with -An ............................................. 189 Table 18. Psych Adjectives Derived with -II+ mA + mf$ .... ........................... 189 Table 19. Psych Adjectives Derived with -DIRI -t + Jk + CI .......................... . 189 Table 20. Psych Adjectives Derived with -Im + II+ sfz ................................ .190 Table 21. Psych Adjectives Derived with -Ill n +An/ mAzl mAyan ................... 190 Table 22. Psych Adjectives Derived with -1m/ n/ GI +sAl ............................. 190 Table 23. Psych Adjectives Derived with -msAr ......................................... 191 Table 24. Psych Adjectives Derived with -Ilk ......................................... .... 191 Table 25. Psych Adjectives Derived with -C/ ........... .... :............................ 191 Table 26. Psych Adjectives Derived with -AI+ t +lei .......................... ........ 191 Table 27. Psych Adjectives Derived with -sAk ....... ................................... 191 Table 28. Psych Adjectives Derived with -C/ ............................................. 192 Table 29. Psych Adjectives Derived with -In ............................................. 192 Table 30. Psych Adjectives Derived with -kar ................................... ......... 192 Table 31. Psych Adjectives Derived with -$inas .. ....................................... 192 Table 32. Psych Adjectives Derived with -i ............................................... 192 Table 33. Psych Adjectives Derived with -sAl ............................................ 192 Table 34. Psych Adjectives Derived with -Dan .......................................... 193 Table 35. Psych Adjectives Derived with - !A + lei ................................... .. .193 Table 36. Psych Adjectives Derived with -n{: Table 37. Psych Adjectives Derived with -II .............................................. 194 Table 38. Psych Adjectives Derived with -slz ............................................ 194 Table 39. Non- derived Psych Adjectives ................................................. 194 Table 40. Psych Adjectives Derived with -lA$ ............................................ 195 ............................................. 193 xvu Table 41. Psych Adjectives derived with -!An ............................................ 195 Table 42. Psych Adjectives derived with -lA .............................................. 196 Table 43. Lexical Counterparts of Complex Nucleus Verbs ( 1) ........................ 196 Table 44. Lexical Counterparts of Complex Nucleus Verbs (2) ........................ 196 Summary Tables Table 1. Summary of the Causativity Properties of Turkish Psych Verbs ..... : ..... XIX Table 2. Summary of the Psych and Non-Psych Compound Forms ................ XXII XVlll THE LIST OF FIGURES I. 1. Figure 1. Bresnan (1995) Lexicalist Views on Argument Structure .................... .22 Figure 2, Bresnan (1995) The Argument Structure in LFG .............................. 22 I. 2. Figure 1. Belleti and Rizzi (1988) the temere class ....................................... 35 Figure 2. Belleti and Rizzi ( 1988) the piacere and preoccupare classes .............. 35 Figure 3. Belletti and Rizzi (1988) a Flat Tripartite Structure ........................... 38 Figure 4. Belletti and Rizzi ( 1988) Theme Prominent Structure ........................ 3 8 Figure 5. Belletti and Rizzi (1988) Experiencer Prominent Structure ................... 39 Figure 6. Belletti and Rizzi (1988) Theme and Experiencer as a Small Clause ....... 39 Figure 7. Kural (1996) Experiencer Subject Neutral Object Verbs ..................... 56 Figure 8. Kural (1996) Neutral Subject Experiencer Object Verbs ..................... 56 II. 5. Figure 1. The Experiencer- Stimulus Interaction ....................................... 184 1 INTRODUCTION Background Information The relationship between the syntactic and the semantic components of language has been one of the central issues and a matter of discussion in modem linguistics. The validity and the coverage of the generalizations related to these two components actually depend on a clear identification of the nature of the relationship between the two. In fact, the discussion point is not the question of whether there is a relationship between the two or not but rather the extent of this interconnection. The proposed answers, depending on the theoretical framework within which they are presented, viewed the correlation between the syntactic and semantic realizations sometimes as tendencies and sometimes as rules. With a significant departure from his previous commitment about the autonomy of syntax in GB, Chomsky focused on the relationship between the heads and their syntactic complements which he called the thematic relations. The notion of thematic roles was not something new at that time. It was first introduced by Fillmore and then Gruber and recently by Jackendoffwho all have different claims at some crucial points. Chomsky gives a more central position to thematic roles viewing their function as the derivation of the subcategorization frames. This departure from his prior position as to the autonomy of syntax was interpreted as an inevitable departure claiming that " ... thematic roles are introduced into Chomsky's Government and Binding Theory only 2 because other syntactic conditions fail to block all the ill formed structures generated by the grammar" (Ravin, 1990:12). After his acceptance of thematic relations, thematic roles have become much more popular and appeared in the theories which follow him (such as in the model of Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG)). Under Chomsky's view, deriving syntactic structures from semantics helped the linguistic theory in terms of simplicity and comprehensiveness since it supplied the linguistic theory with no need to formulate two sets of principles; one for syntactic transformations and the other for semantic combinations. In the theory, reference to semantics is limited to Theta roles that are viewed as the only semantic notions which bear syntactic relevance. To mean to be syntactically relevant, a lexical item has to correspond to a syntactic behavior. Theta roles in the theory have an abstract syntactic nature which does not have any semantic content and the semantic information about lexical predicates is considered to be only in the form of thematic roles in the lexicon. The theory assumes that there are two properties in the determination of Theta roles. The first is the properties that the lexical items have, and the second is the grammatical functions which link the Theta role of the head with its argument. That means it is possible to determine the syntactic configuration of a predicate from the thematic roles. This view brings the idea that thematic roles also correspond to syntactic relations between a verb and its complements. One of the current views is that the meaning of a verb completely determines the configuration of the thematic roles. Based on this view, Theta roles which express semantic relations between the predicates and their arguments were regarded as semantic primitives. 3 A Projection Principle was also introduced in order to link the lexical items to syntactic configurations. This principle assumes that once the lexical information is projected onto the sentence, it can not be changed or deleted by any of the transformational processes. Together with the Theta Criterion and the Projection Principle, Chomsky proposes a set of other principles which complement each other. The first one is the Principle of Full Interpretation, when regarded from the point of view of predicate argument structure, states basically that there should not be any superfluous elements i.e. arguments of a predicate, in the interpretation of the sentences. This means the only elements that can possibly exist in a sentence are the elements which have to be there. This entails the Principle of Economy which requires the linguistic structures to be as economical as possible. One ofthecriticisms about the theory was.related to the syntactic and thematic correspondence. Unlike the assumptions of the theory, a thematic role could exist in the meaning of a verb without having a certain syntactic pattern as well as it was possible that there may be some thematic roles which were absent in the meaning of a certain verb though they were reflected in the syntactic structure of the sentence. Another criticism was about the semantic contents of the thematic roles. Although the Theta Theory claimed that Theta roles express the semantic relation between predicates and their arguments, there is not anything about the semantic content of these Theta roles themselves in the theory -since Chomsky uses Theta role labels just for syntactic purposes. The other criticism is about the concept of semantic class which does not have any clear cut definition in the theory. Ravin observes that 4 .... The notion of semantic class is unclear- verbs have any number and kind of semantic markers in common, thus forming a continuum of semantic similarity ranging from complete dissimilarity to synonymy. It is not obvious how to break this continuum into distinct semantic classes (1990: 226). Actually the theory does not offer any criteria to determine the semantic classes. Therefore, it is not obvious what criteria form semantic classes; whether synonymy, near synonymy or some other defined senses which exist in the meaning of some verbs will make lexical items share the same semantic class. In fact, the deliberate omission of semantic discussions in the theory in general is one of the main points of criticism as well as other criticisms such as about the criteria that make pragmatic decisions different from the semantic decisions of native speakers to reflect their intuition about grammaticality and acceptableness. One of the other criticisms about the theory is that it bases itself mainly onto considerations of language acquisition. The first aim of the theory was to represent the universal principles and the nature of the linguistic competence. After Chomsky's shift of attention from the autonomy of the syntax, to the lexicon and the lexical information, during the course of the development of the theory, there has also been a shift of attention in the area of language acquisition. Recently, much of the language acquisition has been regarded as the acquisition of the features of lexical entries. As Chomsky himself points out "language acquisition is in essence a matter of determining lexical idiosyncrasies" (Cook, 1997:283). In fact there are some other influential ideas concerning similar issues before and after Chomsky. It is apparent that discussions after Chomsky are either in the form of agreement or disagreement of his basic ideas. 5 One theory which accounts for the semantic relations between the predicates and their arguments at a syntactic level is Fillmore's Case Theory. He defines six cases in the theory; Agentive, Instrumental, Dative, Factitive, Locative and the Objective. These cases, for him, represent just the concepts which are syntactically relevant, so,_~hey have little to do with the semantics. In his view, the lexicon does not contain any semantic information which inherently exists in the meaning of verbs. There are also some criticisms about Fillmore's Case Theory. First, these cases seem to represent just an arbitrarily chosen set of semantic properties, so, the theory neglects some important meaning differences (such as the grouping inanimate objects and natural forces under the instrumental case). Moreover, ·the cases listed do not cover many semantic concepts inherent in the meanings of some predicates which causes him to ignore some semantic nuances. Therefore, Fillmore's Case Theory was regarded as.an insufficient modal classifying some arbitrary semantic concepts into some other arbitrary categories. The model proposed by Jackendoff differs from both Chomsky's and Fillmore's models in the underlying idea that it assumes two independent levels of autonomous representation; syntactic and semantic. He gives the primacy to motion verbs and sees them as the basis for the representation of all other predicates. He assigns five · thematic roles in the analysis of the meaning of verbs as the; Theme, Source, Goal, Agent and Location and analyzes the semantic functions of all other verbs based on motion verbs. There are also some criticisms and counter ideas related to the model. First, it is claimed that trying to force to capture the meaning of all predicates in terms of motion, restricts the semantic coverage of the theory because there are a number of human activities which do not involve the fundamental concepts of motion. Many lexical items such as the psych predicates express a more abstract kind of motion which has a different 6 semantic nature. The theory is also claimed to misinterpret the semantic facts expressed by the meaning of a number verbs. Secondly, it seems to be arbitrary to choose the motion verbs and positing a GO function as primary, and trying to label all other verbs simply by extending the notion of motion thus by giving the notion of motion some kind of a privileged semantic status. The third criticism is that the theory has accounted for only five thematic relations which are not convenient for the analysis of the meaning of some verbs. To conclude, according to the criticisms, Jackendoffwas claimed to do wrong not only by using insufficient thematic role labels to analyze the verbs but also forcing them into an inconvenient motional framework. As mentioned briefly, one of the criticisms for Chomsky's account related to thematic roles was that the theory did not offer an elaborate semantic description of their nature. The MIT Lexicon Project Theory (MLP) especially with Levin and Ho~~v shaped the discussions about the Theta roles in the Government and Binding framework in a slightly different way. The MLP linguists defend the idea that the semantic class that a verb belongs to determines its thematic relations, in other words, the thematic structure of a verb completely determines its syntactic behavior and the members of similar verb classes show similar argument alternations. The correlations among semantic classes, syntactic realizations of arguments, and the assignment of thematic roles are completely predictable. They propose two levels of lexical representations; the first one is the Predicate Argument Struc;ture {PAS) and the se.cond one is the Lexical Conceptual Structure (LCS). There are linking rules which link these LCSs to the corresponding PASs. PASs are just like subcategorization frames and LCSs are more semantic in their nature. The roles in PASs are not marked as Agents, Patients etc. but they are structurally regarded as external or internal. The Theta marking processes correspond to these arguments are the indirect and 7 the direct Theta marking. When the violations as to the linking of arguments of some intransitive verbs came to be questioned, a distinction between two classes of intransitive verbs which are unergatives and unaccusatives were proposed. In the analysis of the extent of the interaction between the syntactic and semantic components, the syntactic aspect has been a more investigated area in comparison to the semantic component especially in the Generative Grammar Tradition. The idea that the verb meaning is the main determinant in the syntactic structure of sentences has caused the lexical semantics and especially the verb semantics to gain more significance. It has been claimed that the semantic properties of a verb i.e. some semantic concepts which are inherent in the meaning of verbs specify the syntactic structure that the verb necessitates. The most remarkable reflection of the role of semantics on the syntactic structure is observed in verb-argument relationships since the verb is the main determinant of the semantic roles that the arguments in the argument structure can bear. The verb determines both the number and the kind of arguments in a sentence. In this way it is in the central position in both the constitution and the interpretation of sentences. Kageyama (1997) mentions two main lexical approaches which are the predicate-centered and the role-centered ones. Although the role-centered approaches labeled the arguments with such roles as Agent, Patient, Goal and Location etc., they did not say much about the meaning of the verb itself. The predicate centered approaches on the other hand assume that verbs have lexical conceptual meanings. This approach isolates the meaning of verbs from its cognitive aspects focusing on the structural properties. The existence of some linking rules that link certain arguments which bear certain semantic roles onto certain syntactic positions, and the idea that there are regularities (linking regularities) that these rules have to obey, have led linguists to study 8 on possible linking theories. This idea has been claimed sometimes to mean that there is a linking between the semantic and the syntactic components and sometimes to mean that the two are completely interwoven. !he. P~rpose of the Study A group of psychological verbs present a kind of problematic area for the mentioned linking or mapping theories according to the principle that thematically the most prominent or the highest argument should also be syntactically in the most prominent or the highest position. A significant number of the studies about psych verbs are about these linking rules and the problems that a group of verbs of these verbs pose. This unusual behavior of psych verbs were sometimes regarded as a matter of difference in their deep structures and sometimes regarded as a matter of differenee in their aspectual properties. Different approaches presented different explanations related to this so called irregularity or exceptionality. However one of the important questions to be answered is about the syntactic and semantic properties of this group of verbs in languages other than the most thoroughly investigated ones in the literature. Therefore the aim ofthis study is to describe the syntactic and semantic properties of these verbs in Turkish and therefore contribute to the universal linking rules finding out cross linguistic commonalities ifthere are any. Apart from the universal linking rules, it has been expeCted thaftlie present study may contribute to the description of Turkish grammar in some or other way with a clear identification of some of the syntactic and semantic properties of psych verbs which constitute a significant part of all verbs in Turkish that have received little or no attention 9 except for some of their semantic properties in traditional Turkish grammars. Therefore, the study aims at contributing to the literature not only because it investigates a group of verbs which are labeled in the literature as exceptional but also because it examines Turkish whose psych verbs has not been studied so far. Research Questions The syntactic and semantic properties of psych verbs are gomg to be investigated through the following questions: 1. Which verbs constitute the psych verb class in Turkish? 2. What kind of a verb classification can be proposed for Turkish psych verbs when their syntactic and semantic properties have been described? 3. What structural and semantic. properties do the psych verbs in Turkish have in terms of their interaction with voice markers? 4. What are the compound psych verbs in Turkish? 5. What are the structural and semantic derivational properties of the adjectivalizations of psych verbs in Turkish? Hypotheses The following hypotheses are presupposed: 1. An analysis of the Dictionary of Turkish Language Institute will partially provide the data needed to create a database of psych verbs in Turkish. 10 2. There is a psych verb classification with similar properties in Turkish with the ones proposed in the literature for different languages when their syntactic and semantic properties have been described. 3. The exceptional behavior of psych verbs are also observed in their interaction with voice markers in Turkish parallel to the universal tendencies. 4. There are a number of compound psych verbs in Turkish which display similar structural properties with their non- psych counterparts. 5. The derivation of the psych adjectives does not display any idiosyncratic properties which differentiate them from general derivational patterns of Turkish. Data Collection Techniques and Limitations The data for the psych verbs, psych compounds, psych nouns and psych adjectives in Turkish is constituted of and limited to the verbs in the Dictionary of Turkish Language Institution Volume . I and II (1988) and, the natural data needed for the identification of subtle semantic differences is constituted of both the sentences from electronic sources and of native speakers themselves. The Method of Analysis The data for psych verbs in Turkish is going to be analyzed according to their syntactic and semantic properties creating a database to examine through a set of specified criteria. 11 The methods of classification and description are going to be used with comparisons and contrasts with the studies of the psych verbs in different languages in the literature of which only the English translations will be given. 12 I. I. 1. REVIEW OF LITERATURE Argument Structure Why are there a limited number of arguments cross linguistically? What is the restriction on the number of the arguments that a verb possibly can take? Why are there a limited number of theta roles? This section analyzes the proposed possible answers that constitute the fundamental concerns of argument structure theories. The process of lexicalization is a language specific one in a way that it includes historical phenomena as well as cultural ones. In spite of the differences in the lexicons of different languages, there is the chance to express similar or sometimes even equal senses by combining these lexical items with others and forming more complex structures in different languages. Lexical items gain new meanings as a result of combinations of different senses, moreover, new lexical items are often introduced into languages. If the lexical items themselves expressed every aspect of events that they denote, we would have as many lexical items as the number of all events in the world which is not possible. This is also the case with the psych verbs. There are innumerable psychological states of human beings each of which can be gradable in themselves. Thus from the point of view of the nature of human computational system, it seems impossible to denote each of these psychological states with a different lexical item. Rather we combine some lexical items with others or we modify them to convey a number of different senses. Within the GB framework lexical structure has gained a more central role in syntactic description. In this tradition, phrase structure rules became more redundant and 13 started to be eliminated. Movement was started to be assumed simply as the checking of lexical features in syntax. Lexical Inclusiveness in the Minimalist Program proposed that the output representations are just the properties of lexical items in the lexicon. The derivation of sentences starts from these lexical items; which is the process called numeration. These lexical items carry the syntactic, semantic, morphological and phonological information. They merge together and create the constituent trees. When they move to new positions, certain lexical features of them are checked according to the checking requirements encoded in the lexicon. Kageyama (1997) views verbs as the items which play the central role in the composition and interpretation of the sentences since they determine the number, the kind and the semantic properties of arguments that will exist in the sentence. In the analysis of verbs, three levels of lexical representations are proposed up to now; the argument structure, lexical conceptual structure and the event structure. Lexical semantic studies have recently focused on two approach~s about the verb semantics and the syntax. They are the role centered approaches and the predicate centered approaches. In the predicate centered approaches the method of predicate decomposition is employed. Decompositions in Kageyama's terms, are the representations showing only the skeletons of the verbs' conceptual meanings (1997:5). These lexical conceptual representations are linked to syntactic structures by means of linking rules. After the recognition that there are significant regularities between argument realizations of different predicates, the idea that the syntax of sentences is determined by the meaning of predicates has gained popularity. These regularities are called ''linking regularities" and the rules which map semantic roles onto syntactic positions are called "linking rules" (Levin and Hovav, 1996:487). Some approaches assume that these 14 syntactic positions are unpredictable and some assume that they are partly predictable. Levin and Hovav (1996) argue that this mapping between the semantic representation and syntactic expression of the arguments of a predicate is fully predictable. They claim that the best way to find out the syntactically relevant aspects of the meaning of a predicate is to express the lexical semantic representation of predicates with a predicate centered approach. To achieve this, we need to find out the elements of core meaning of predicates which are essential in the linking of their arguments. There are some cross linguistic differences in the mapping of arguments to syntactic positions. According to Levin and Hovav, languages may even differ in the linking of the arguments of two verbs which seem to be the translations of each other in two different languages. Languages may also differ in the weight of the value of some aspects of meaning that they give their arguments i.e. for the same argument, when one language gives weight to one semantic component, another language may give weight to another aspect of it. Levin and Hovav mainly focused on the class-membership r~l~tions of arguments. Under their view, if we group verbs into semantically coherent classes, we can isolate the meaning components of a predicate relevant to syntax. Although some idiosyncracy is allowed, generally, much of the syntactic behavior of verbs can be determined by their membership in a semantic class. The basic suggestion is based on the idea that the meanings of verbs have some kind of an internal structure. These internal structures have some primitive elements. The similarities in the meanings of verbs are the results of these shared elements in the decomposition of primitives. Manning (1996) views the argument structure as a syntactic representation. For him grammatical structure is a result of the grammaticization of discourse roles. For 15 example the notion of subject is the result of the grammaticization of the notion of topic or focus of an event. That means there is an association between the topicality and subjecthood. He states that we need two mappings between the gr-structure and a-structure. The first mapping is the argument projection which is based on the meaning of predicates. The second is the linking which links the argument structure to grammatical structure. In recent years, event structure has also been seen as a modular component of argument structure. It is even claimed to be responsible for the linking of arguments to syntactic positions. Then, event structure was started to be represented in phrase structure. They put the elements of event structure in VPs or in functional phrases. These ideas led to the emergence of more syntactic analyses of the event structure. Another property of event structure related to the argument structure is the distinction between the individual level predicates and stage level predicates. Kratzer argues that the stage level predicates have extra event arguments in their representations (Tenny and Pustejovsky, 1990:20). In the theories of verb meaning it is usually assumed that meanings of verbs are multidimensional and the linking rules are sensitive to these dimensions. In the early theories it was advocated that verbs that refer to the same kind of event with same participants, display the same linking pattern. The verbs that do not conform to the standard linking pattern were regarded as non canonical or marked. They were thought to be rare in languages and difficult to acquire. Gropen and Pinker et al (1995) claim that these so called non canonical verbs are in fact more numerous than canonical ones and both are acquired at the same time. 16 Semantically, exammmg the argument structure of a verb is in a way examining the entities and their relationships of these entities with each other specified in the meaning of a verb. Ravin argues that The arguments are place holders for entities. Since predicates have as many arguments as there are entities represented in their meaning, whether these are syntactically realized or not, there are four types of relations logically possible for arguments and complements .. . (1990:160). He then lists these linking possibilities as; first, there are arguments that inherently exist in the meaning of the verb but never have syntactic realizations, second, there are arguments which are inherent and compulsorily syntactically realized, third, there are arguments which are inherent but optionally realized, and the fourth there are some other syntactic arguments which do not correspond to semantic arguments (1990:168). According to Raves, the syntactic complements do not always correspond to semantic arguments. On the contrary Chomsky has the claim that the grammaticality determines the meaningfulness. What Raves argues is that propositions can be both grammatical and meaningless at the same time or vice versa. In fact, the idea that lies behind all these arguments is the difference between the questions as to whether the syntactic and semantic components are autonomous or they are dependent on each other. The theories of argument structure mainly followed either of these approaches. In Cross-Linguistic Patterns of Linking Valin (nd.) argues that the universal aspects of argument linking exist and the language specific variations are rather limited to a few number of possibilities. In the framework of Role and Reference Grammar, the linking between the syntax and semantics is achieved at two levels. One is the lexical phase level and the other is the syntactic phase level. Valin suggests that the universal phases are found in the syntactic phases of linking but cross-linguistic variations in the lexical phases of linking. ~refound 17 Wunderlich (nd.) argues that contrary to the assumptions, there are more argument linking types than the generalized ones. He claims that neither of these types belongs to UG but they are invented by cultural evolution. Some of these types are: portmanteau type, active type, salience type, positional type and the inverse type. He sees the inverse type as the most complex type since it maps two hierarchies on each other, such as psych verbs. The following section is a brief analysis of the the significant approaches to the argument structure phenomena and the linking between the semantic arguments and the syntactic realizations. " ... what a theory of thematic roles should look like is analogous to that of the blind men examining the elephant, each touching a different part of its body" (1991 :561) says Dowty criticizing the traditional accounts of thematic roles and proposing an original theory of his own called Thematic Proto Roles and Argument Selection. What the earlier accounts on the same matter call; thematic relations (of Gruber); deep structures and transformations (of Chomsky), subject sekctip!J _rules (of Fillmore), template matching (of Stowell), universal alignment principle (of Perlmutter and Postal), and universal theta assignment principle (of Baker), is called as Argument Selectional Principles by Dowty. Dowty claims that semantic distinctions are results of distinctions in the real world. Thus, it is wrong to try to identify clear cut boundaries for these classes and to try to find out the limits of our cognitive ability by refemng to those classes. What he means by argument selection is not a kind of linking between the syntactic andsemantic levels but a kind of constraint only on some lexical predicates out of a great number of others. What he means by prototype is not about individual lexical 18 items but rather a generalization about lexical meanings. The argument selection principles that he defines are only about two place predicates which have a subject and a true direct object. Moreover, he takes only the 'arguments' into consideration but not 'adjuncts'. Dowty sees thematic roles as prototypes or cluster concepts and argues against viewing the thematic generalizations as equal with semantic and syntactic generalizations. He defines five criteria (1990:572,573) for the properties of Agent Proto Role which are volition, sentience/ perception, causing event or change of state of another participant, movement and the independent existence of the event described by the verb. He defines another five criteria for the prop·erties of a Proto Patient Role which are change of state, incremental Theme, causally affectedness, relative stationariness and the dependent existence of the event described by the verb. The principles that he formulated as to the selections of arguments (1991:576) can briefly be explained like this: 1. The argument which has the most proto Agent properties is lexicalized as the subject. 2. The argument which has the most proto Patient properties is lexicalized as the direct object. 3. If there are two arguments which have the same number of proto agent and proto patient properties both of them can be lexicalized as subjects/objects. 4. If the predicate is a three place predicate, the argument which has the most proto patient properties is lexicalized as the direct object, and, the one which has the less proto patient properties is lexicalized as the oblique object or the prepositional object. 19 5. lfthere are two arguments which have the same number of proto patient properties both of them can be lexicalized as direct objects. 6. Some arguments may have none of these roles. 7. Some arguments may share the same role. 8. Some arguments may have the properties ofboth proto roles either in at?- equal or a partial degree. Dowty states that these principles are just strong tendencies rather than clear cut rules. The combinations (1991 :571) for the corresponding roles that he foriritilated can be summarized in the following table: Ag~nt .... '; Experiencer lnstru~ent Theme Patient volition+ causation+ sentience+ movement volition+ causation volition sentience causation+ movement change+ (incremental Theme)+ dependent existence+ (instrumental Theme) change+ (incremental Theme)+ dependent existence+ (cimsally affected) Table L Dowty (1991) Thematic Roles and the Syntactic Reahzatton Rules For him, these roles and their argument selection principles determine the following role hierarchies (Dowty, 1990:578): Agent> Instrument/ Experiencer> Patient > Source/ Goal Causing Event> Caused Event Moving Argument> Source/ Goal Argument On the other hand, Hale and Keyser (2002) define ·argument structure as a lexical item's projection of its syntactic configuration which is limited both in variety and complexity among languages. Similarly the number of thematic roles associated with them is also very limited. They also claim that languages do not differ in their basic elements of argument structure. They define two basic structures as the complements and the specifiers mentioning two argument structure types; lp-monadic (in which "lp" refers to the lexical 20 projection) which does not include a specifier but only a complement. On the other hand, lp-dyadic predicates have a specifier argument. They point out that the subject argument is not a specifier argument but rather an external argument. The specifier argument in their view is internal in the lexical projection and shared by both the transitive and the intransitive alternants since it is internal to the argument structure. Alberti (1997) assumes that the internal structure of argument structures have two substructures. The first one is the relative structure which is about perspective of the speakers and the second one is the absolute structure which is the characteristics of a whole family of related argument structures. For him, the thematic roles are abstractions over semantic structures. The important thing for an argument is not its thematic role but the position it occupies in the internal structure of argument structure. Similar to Grimshaw (1990), he emphasizes the importance of the ordering of arguments. The situation described by the verb is also important. What he calls as the family of an argument structure is different argument structure versions of the same verb. He claims that the internal structure of an argument structure can easily be calculated on the basis of the family of the verb. This rule is valid for canonical cases but there are also non canonical cases like psych verbs. According to Alberti, the thing to do is to sort out the arguments which play the central role in the situation described by the verb. Sometimes each participant in the situation is not syntactically realized. He calls the overtly expressed arguments as explicit arguments and non-overtly expressed arguments as implicit arguments. Other arguments which are not the essential participants in the situation are called as adjuncts. Moreover, some verbs may have more than one argument structures. Alberti assumes that the argument version with a lesser number of arguments is prior to the version which has more 21 arguments. Therefore, speakers of a language have not only this knowledge but also have the knowledge of different argument structure versions and the knowledge of the use of one of these versions in different situations. For Alberti, what Pesetsky's analysis makes clear is that in psychological predicates either the Theme can be superior to the Experiencer or the Experiencer can be superior to the Theme. Therefore, Alberti claims that it is not the thematic hierarchy that we should look for but the hierarchy of arguments in the argument structure family. Alberti adopts the argument hierarchy which was proposed by Larson and Baker: Agent> Theme> Goal/ Benefactive/ Location Baker On the Structural Positions of Themes and Goals points out that in some languages Themes are more prominent than Goals and in some other languages Goals are more prominent than Themes. Therefore it is not something easy to decide which one is derived and which one is basic. Bresnan (1995) in Lexicality and Argument Structure proposes a theory of argument structure in LFG framework as opposed to other lexicalist frameworks such as of Levin and Hovav and Hale and Keyser. Her main argument is that in a scheme including the main components; lexical semantics, argument structure and syntactic structure, the redundant element is not the argument structure · but the initial syntactic structure. That means, in the linking process of the arguments to the final syntactic positions, the underlying syntactic trees do not have any role and therefore they should be eliminated. The following schemes illustrate the status of the mentioned initial syntactic structures in other frameworks. The first one is of Hovav and Levin and the second one is of Hale and Keyser (Bresnan, 1995:4, 5): 22 Lexical Semantics ! Lexicon Argument Structure t t Syn~actic Projection Initial Syntactic Structure Syntactic Transformations Final Syntactic Structure Lexical Primitives t Lexicon Initial Syntactic Structure ! Syntactic Transformations Final Syntactic Structure Figure I. Bresnan (1995) Lexicalist Views on Argument Structure The last schema of Bresnan represents both her LFG framework and other lexicalist views: Lexical Semantics ! Lexico Semantic Projection Argument Structure ! Lexico Syntactic Projection Final Syntactic Structure Figure 2. Bresnan (1995) The Argument Structure in LFG She gives examples from Chichewa and English which support her view that verbal processes for transitivity, intransitivity, causativity or passivization of verbs are lexical morphological processes. Therefore, she claims that syntactic tree structures do not have any functions in linking the arguments of verbs to their syntactic positions. Another phenomenon which receives a great deal of discussion in the domain of syntax semantics interface is the phenomenon of split intransitivity (unaccusative/ unergative distinction). The following section briefly analyzes some basic approaches to unaccusative phenomenon. 23 I. 1. 1. Transitivity and the Unaccusative Phenomenon Unaccusative/ unergative distinction among languages have effects not only on agreement and case marking but also especially on subject object distinctions and therefore, has an important place in theories of argument structure. According to the Unaccusative Hypothesis of Perlmutter (1978), some intransitive verbs have two different classes as unaccusatives and unergatives. Unergative verbs have an actual syntactic subject but unaccusative verbs have a derived subject which is an underlying syntactic object. Burzio states that there is a relationship between the case marking and the argument structure (Hoekstra, 2000:55). There is also a correlation between the existence of accusative case and the external argument. Unergative possesses external arguments but not an internal accusative argument. Conversely, ergatives have internal arguments but not external ones. Alberti (1997) points out that in intransitive verbs the subject is either associated with a patient like or an agent like grammatical function. The corresponding language types are either ergative or accusative. He argues that there is a third type of language (a mixed type) which assigns either an agentlike or a patientlike grammatical function to the subjects of intransitive verbs. Accusative languages mark their intransitive arguments with nominative case; just like the agent arguments of transitive verbs. Again in accusative languages some intransitive subjects behave like agents and some intransitive subjects behave like patients. In some languages unaccusative and unergative verbs select different auxiliaries, so, it is easy to distinguish them from each other. Case assignment properties of 24 these two groups of verbs are also different from each other. Burzio says that unaccusative verbs do not have direct objects since they can not assign case for them but unergative verbs assign accusative case to their _objects (Levin and _Hovav, 199():_492). .L.evtn and Hovav also suggest that telicity as an aspectual property · does not always show unaccusativity since there are verbs which are both atelic and unaccusative. According to Chomsky, there is a differentiation between AGRs and AGRo. The choice between the two is a matter of activation. Nominative- accusative languages activate AGRs but ergative- absolutive languages activate AGRo. AGRs is seen as related to Tense while AGRo is seen as related to V. Tense case is a property of all sentences but verb case is not; that is why accusative case is not always available for all verbs. This is claimed to be just a matter of asymmetry between the nominative and the accusative (Hoekstra, 2000:58). As mentioned, there are also different conceptions as to what the argument structure is and at which level it should be analyzed. Manning ( 1996) argues for a distinction between the grammatical relations and the argument structure prominence. In Government and Binding literature, Grimshaw (1990) claims that argument structures are not sets or lists of arguments of a predicate. They have their own internal organizations, they affect the grammatical behavior of predicates and they are predictable from the key characteristics of the meaning of predicates. Some sort of prominence relations ·a.mong the arguments are determined by the thematic and aspectual properties of each predicate. Manning ( 1996) suggests six classes of treatments of ergativity which are the syntactic accusativity analysis, ergative as passive analysis, absolutive subject as object analysis, the oblique analysis, the inverse analysis and the four relations analysis (37). 25 Manning adopts the inverse analysis and assumes that there is an accusative organization for all languages at the level of argument structure. He argues for the existence of at least three linking possibilities cross linguistically. What he calls syntactic accusativity displays the following mapping possibilities: There are some languages which always use inverse mapping for transitive verbs. These languages are what he calls syntactically ergative: Gr- Structure A- Structure Object A-subject (agent) Subject Patient Table 2. .. Syntactic Ergativity He sees the argument structure as a syntactic level and valence changing operations as operations on this level. In brief, the basic claim of Manning is that the syntactic representation has two levels of information which are the grammatical structure and the argument structure. According to him, there are two different mapping possibilities between these two levels. Manning introduces an Inverse Grammatical Relations Analysis and claims that these two levels pose different sort of constraints on syntax and linking of these two levels differ from one language to another. Dowty (1991) sees the unaccusative/ unergative distinction as a grammatical distinction and deals with syntactic accusativity and semantic accusativity separately. In his analysis of thematic proto roles, Dowty predicted that his argument selection principles do not apply to syntactically ergative languages since they have an inverse correlation between the subject and object and thus between the proto agent and the proto patient. 26 However he claims that these principles can be applied in the same way just by means of a reversal. In ergative languages, the arguments with most proto patient properties are lexicalized as subjects and the arguments with the most proto agent properties are as objects. His observation is that the unaccusative verbs have arguments with ·patientlike meanings while unergative ones have arguments with agentlike meanings. He argues that the most important proto property to distinguish between the unaccusatives and unergatives is volition. In his analysis, the verbs which have the arguments with the properties of sentience and volition are unergative verbs. Examining "variable behavior verbs" which are the verbs that sometimes show unaccusative and sometimes ergative behavior, Levin and Hovav (1996) claim that unaccusative and unergative classes can be predicted semantically. Moreover they observe that these verbs most of the time have more than one meaning i.e. when they display unaccusative behavior, they have a different meaning and when they are unergative, they are associated with another meaning (Levin and Hovav, 1996:490, 491). Chung (nd.) proposes a number of criteria to distinguish between the unaccusative and the unergative verbs. First, in terms of argument structure, unergative verbs take external arguments while unaccusative verbs do not. Second, he claims that in the events described by unaccusative verbs, the notion of intention is not involved; on the other hand, unergative verbs describe a kind of willed intentionality. Third, the nominal suffix -er is attached only to verbs which can take external arguments i.e. unergatives. Fourth, the resultative construction can only be used with unaccusative verbs but not with unergatives (12). 27 Nakipoglu (1998) argues that Turkish is one of the languages which are sensitive to the unaccusative/ unerg<l.tive distinction. She defines three classes of unaccusatives in Turkish which are the endpoint, measure and path unaccusatives. She observes that semantic notions such as agentivity, volition, control and delimitedness are important for the unaccusative/ unergative distinction for Turkish intransitives too. Nakipoglu uses adjectival passives as a diagnostic to distinguish between unaccusatives 'iud unergatives in Turkish. For example, -ml~ is incompatible with transitives and it modifies only the subjects of unaccusatives. Another adjective deriving suffix -Ik is also only compatible with unaccusative verbs. What Nakipoglu (1988) observes for Turkish unaccusatives and unergatives can be summarized in the following table: Unaccusatives Unergatives externally instigated can not be passivized delimited the only argument is affected and changed internally instigated can be passivized non-delimited the only argument is affecting or instigating rather than affected lack a direct internal argument describe activities can never select an external argument describe situations Table 3. Nak1po~lu (1998) Unaccusattves and Unergattves Another grammatical process which constitutes a significant part of the discussions in the psych verb literature is causativity. The following section briefly summarizes some of the basic discussions about causativity. I. I. 2. Voice Alternations and Causativity The idea that the causative meaning has an affect on the realization of the arguments of a predicate supported the claim that the unusual realization of the arguments of the Experiencer object psych predicates is because of their causative semantics. 28 Therefore, psych verbs which constitute a borderline area for lexical semantic and syntactic studies have been analyzed mostly with reference to causativity. Palmer (1994) sees causative constructions as derived from simple non causative sentences just like passive constructions which are derived from their active counterparts. Unlike passive constructions, causative ones add a subject Causer argument to the argument structure. Just like many languages which have morphological passive forms, some languages have morphological causative fonns like Turkish. Other than morphological forms there are periphrastic forms which utilize a specific helping verb for causation. For example English does not have any grarnmaticalized causative morpheme but it rather uses periphrastic verbs. There are four principles for causative constructions; first, there sh~uld be a morphological or periphrastic mark on the verb, second, there should be a Causer addition to the subject position, third, other arguments should be demoted, and fourth, there should of course be a causative meaning. For "demotion" Palmer adopts Comrie's (1976) hierarchy of grammatical relations (Palmer, 1994): Subject> Direct Object> Indirect Object> Oblique Constituent Palmer claims that the accusative case expresses direct causation which he calls coercive causation but the instrumental expresses indirect causation which he calls noncoercive causation. For him, dative case ts used with verbs of experience whose subjects are typically animate non Agents. 29 He defines two types of causation which are the manipulative causation (lexical/ single event causation) and directive causation. Manipulative causation necessitates the physical movement of the Causer while directive causation does not. In the causative constructions in Turkish, as mentioned, there is a demotion in the hierarchy of grammatical relations in which the subject (SU) is the highest, direct object (DO) is the second highest, the indirect object (IO) is the third highest and the oblique object is the least high. Zimmer (1988) argues that in Turkish causative constructions of intransitive verbs, the subject slot is filled and the Causee argument is assigned the next highest case appropriate (which is the case of the DO). This is what Zimmer refers to as "Syntactic Demotion Strategy (SDS)" (1988:217). Turkish is one of the languages that uses this strategy. Zimmer claims that in Turkish this rule does not apply regularly. Turkish transitive verbs take accusative objects. With a small class of verbs which take dative case marked objects, the mentioned rule does not apply. Zimmer calls them as "case switching causatives" (219) exemplifying a similar process in French and emphasizing that case switching is possible in the process of causativization only for this small class of verbs. Therefore, we can neither see it with the verbs which have accusative objects nor change or switch the cases ofNPs for topicalization purposes. The discoursal effect of this process is similar to the passivization which topicalizes the direct object. In these sentences too, the surface DO is more topical, affected or patientlike element than the surface indirect object. Thus, Zimmer claims that Turkish, with these possibilities in case assignment, allows its speakers to convey some differences in the way they regard the roles of the elements in the sentence. 30 In brief, Zimmer's basic underlying claim is that there is a semantic flexibility in the assignment of cases according to the communicative needs of the speakers. Zimmer's analysis of case switching causatives is in a sentential level and there is also a need for an analysis of the semantic concepts inherent in the meaning of such verbs. A detailed analysis of the relationships between the semantic and syntactic properties of these verbs would shed a light on the issue from a lexical point ofvlew. Levin and Hovav (1996) divide verbs into two groups in terms of their causativity as internally caused eventualities and externally caused eventualities. They define a linking rule which relates Causers with the deep structure subject position which they call a Causer Linking Rule (1996:501). For them, Agents and Causers are not the same arguments. However, since an Agent is also responsible for the eventuality of the verb, it is also a kind of Causer argument. In decompositional approaches of verb meaning, Cause is a dyadic predicate which needs a Causer and a Causing Event. According to Levin and Hovav, the Causer argument may be an argument of an intransitive verb or a stative verb. Causer argument can also be an Agent as well as a natural force. Moreover, they identify unergative verbs as externally caused intninsitives and unaccusatives as internally caused intransitives. Levin and Hovav define another rule; the Theme Linking Rule which links the Theme argument to the deep structure argument position and argue that Theme Linking Rule has precedence over Causer Linking Rule (1996:502). Holmes's (nd.) analysis is a Word Grammar (WG) analysis of the syntax and semantics of causative/inchoative alternation. In the WG analysis it is claimed that " ... both causative and inchoative uses must be associated with independent lexical 31 representations since there are cases where both sides of the alternation have idiosyncratic semantic properties" (Holmes, nd.:327). The idea that there are events having complex internal struc;tures divided the Theme argument into two parts as the inner event and the outer event. According to Tenny and Pustejovsky the outer event relates to causation and agency, however, the inner event relates to telicity and the change of state (2000:7). Simply, the outer event causes the inner event. There are different linguistic approaches as to what causation represents. According to an approach, it represents the relationship between the two propositions. On the other hand, some approaches claim that it is the relationship between the two events and some others claim that it is between an Agent and an event. For example, Jackendoff sees it as a relation between an individual and an event or between a thing and an event. Dowty on the other hand, sees it as a relation between two propositions. In Grimshaw (1990) we see the activity as the outer causing event and the state as the inner event. Another issue in question in the literature is the relationship between the Cause and the Agency. Both Causers and Agents are syntactically realized as subjects. However both can occur independently from each other. That means although these two notions often intersect, they should be represented separately. One other argument about agentivity is that most of the time Agents are regarded as volitional unlike Causers. In the psych verb literature first we see Belletti and Rizzi (1988) ·who identify three classes of psych predicates in Italian. According to their classifications Class I verbs have nominative Experiencer subjects and accusative Theme objects (as in John fears bears, Class 2 verbs have nominative Theme subjects and accusative Experiencer objects (as in The bears frightened John), and Class 3 verbs have nominative Theme objects and dative Experiencer subjects (as in Bears appeal to John). 32 The mappings of the arguments of these three classes are; Class 1 verbs link the Experiencer role to the external argument. Class 2 verbs link the external argument to the object position licensing a Causer role with causative morphology as opposed to the Class 1 verbs which are non causative and stative. Nelson (nd.) argues that the difference between the two classes is due to the differences in causation and aspect as Grimshaw (1990) argues. The Class 3 psych verbs differ from the other two classes. They are similar to Class 1 verbs because they are stative too. They are similar to the Class 2 verbs because they assign nominative case to Subject and object case to Theme but they are not causative unlike Class 2 verbs. Grimshaw (1990) argued that in causative psych predicates, the thematic role of the subject is not the Theme but the Causer and the Causers are ranked more highly than the Experiencers in the thematic hierarchy. Fabienne (nd.) in Object Experiencer Psych Verbs (OEPVs) and Causatives with supporting data from French, argues that not all object Experiencer psych verbs are causatives. As known, causative verbs describe two events; namely, a causing event and a caused event. If all OEPVs are causatives they should describe a causing and a caused event but this is not the case for French OEPV s. The caused event is the mental event affecting the Experiencer. This event can be either dynamic or stative. Fabienne claims that some French OEPVs lack a causing event saying that only one class of OEPV s has two eventualities similar to causatives. Pylkkanen (2000) in Stativity and Causation argues against the idea that stativity and causation are incompatible notions with evidence from a class of Finnish psych predicates which are stative both in their causative and non causative uses. Her first argument is that the Experiencer object predicates in Finnish are morphologically causative 33 but aspectually stative, just like non causative Experiencer subject predicates. Her second argument is that morphologically non causative psych verbs refer to simple individual states (i-/evel) but morphologically causative psych verbs refer to complex stage level states (s-Ieve!). She demonstrates that causativity is compatible with stage level stativity but not with individual level stativity in Finnish. Pylkkanen assumes that this type of psych verbs are not derived from non causative psych verbs but rather they have their own external arguments. She adopts Pesetsky's approach that the subjects of frighten verbs have the Causer role but objects of fear verbs have the Target role and the Causer role is received from the causative morpheme not from the predicate itself. I. 2. Approaches to Psych Verbs The domain of emotions is such a rich field that it may even include a number of other disciplines like linguistics, psychology, sociology and even anthropology. Apart from its psychological and cultural aspects, a detailed linguistic analysis of psychological verbs and cross linguistic evidence from a variety of languages will provide a better understanding of the linguistic realizations of emotions and the nature of the lexicon. This section analyzes some of the significant approaches to psych verbs in the psych verb literature. These approaches can be grouped under two general headings; structural and semantic/ aspectual. 34 I. 2. 1. Structural Approaches to Psych Verbs Among the structural approaches, Belletti and Rizzi ( 1988) is the most significant and mostly referred study which explains the exceptional behavior of a group of psych verbs from a syntactic point of view. Pesetsky (1987) focuses on the binding peculiarities observed with a group of psych verbs. Vanhoe (2002) analyzes Spanish psych verbs in LFG framework. Mulder (1992) analyzes ergative and unergative psych verbs in Dutch. Hale and Keyser examine the transitivity alternations of psych verbs in English. Ginnis analyzes the morphological restrictions on causative psych verbs in English and Kural (1996) analyzes Turkish psych verbs through elementary predicates. The following sections briefly summarize the mentioned structural approaches to psych verbs. I. 2. 1. 1. Belletti and Rizzi (1998) and the Psych Verbs in Italian Belletti and Rizzi (1988) analyze the so called problematic area of psych verbs which seem to constitute a resistance to the regular mapping of thematic roles to dstructures and identify three types of psych verbs in Italian which present different syntactic configurations: (1) Gianni fears this. (2) This worries Gianni. (3) a. To Gianni pleases this. b. This pleases to Gianni. They call these three classes as the Iemere, preoccupare and ·the pia cere classes. The piacere class is different from the other two classes. It has a dative 35 Experiencer and a nominative Theme. This class allows both orderings of these two arguments unlike others. Traditional studies view the temere class as the main structure from which other two structures were derived transf()rmationally. Belle~ti and Rizzi p;r_opo&~JP.a~ th~ 4- structure configurations of these three classes are not so much different from each other. The first tree diagram is ofthe example (1) and the second is of(2) and (3): s ~ NP VP A V NP I Gianni teme questo Figure I . Belleti and Rizzi (1988) the Iemere class s v· V e preoccupa piace NP NP questo Gianni a Gianni Figure 2. Belleti and Rizzi (1988) the piacere and preoccupare classes The commonalities between these two diagrams are: • Both verbs directly theta mark the Theme argument • Verb+ Theme theta marks the Experiencer. 36 Fitsr they claim that the subject of the preoccupare class is riot adeep subject but a derived subject. In order to prove this they use the anaphoric cliticization test. The assumption is that the deep subject can bind a reflexive clitic while the derived subjects can not. While the verbs in temere class are perfectly grammatical verbs of preoccupare class are not: (4) Gianni himself fears. (5) *Gianni himself worries. With the addition of a human subject, a reciprocal and an adverb like intentionally or voluntarily, the structure becomes acceptable with a meaning that a human subject is intentionally causes a psychological state in the Experiencer: (6) These two guys frighten each other intentionally every time that they have ADV the opportunity. The conclusion is that if the Theme of a psychological verb is non-agentive then their subjects are derived. Another test to prove the same claim is the causative construction test. Basing themselves on Burzio's ideas, Belletti a_nd Rizzi claim that in Italian, whenever ..there is a derived subject, the structure can not be embedded under a causative construction. While it is possible for temere verbs to have acceptable counterparts embedded with a causative verb (7), it is not the case with the preoccupare class (8): (7) This caused that Mario him feared even more. (8) *This caused that Mario him worried even more. Moreover, causative verbs can also have their VP complements. The observation is that temere verbs are possible with the infinitival verbs however, not all but most of the verbs of preoccupare class are not: 37 (9) This will make (one) fear the president even more. (10) *This will make (one) attract the president even more. Belletti and Rizzi observe that this behavior of preoccupare verbs with infinitival VPs is similar to unaccusative verbs. Turning to their passivization behavior, Belletti and Rizzi claim that verbs of preoccupare class do not have syntactic passives. The possible passive sentences with these verbs are instances of adjectival passivization: (11) Gianni is disgusted by the corruption of this country. (12) Gianni is fascinated by this perspective. because they: • show typical adjectival morphology • can not bear clitics (since only the verbs and therefore verbal passives can bear clitics) • select the auxiliary essere 'be' (which is compatible with both adjectival and verbal passives) but not venire 'come' (which is compatible only with verbal passives) • do not allow regular participial form: (13) • *I am tired/ excited by his ideas. have irregular adjectival forms: (14) I am tired/ excitedofhis ideas. Belletti and Rizzi see this as a result of a blocking principle which predicts that the existence of an irregular adjectival form blocks the regular adjectival participle formation (1988:313). 38 Another special characteristic of the verbs of preoccupare class is with respect to their behavior in the theory of Binding; the Experiencer argument of these verbs violates the c-command requirement for antecedent-anaphor relationship, binding an anaphor contained in the subject: (27) *The gossips about himself describe Gianni better than any official biography. (28) The gossips about himself worry Gianni more than anything else. While the object of psych verbs can bind an anaphor which is contained in the subject they can not bind an anaphor which is the subject itself: (29) Pictures ofhimselfworry him. (30) *Himself worries him. Belletti and Rizzi claim that the subject of these verbs moves to the subject position from an internal VP position. They propose four different possible internal structures for this VP (1988:320): 1. A "flat triparite" structure: VP V Theme Experiencer Figure 3. Belletti and Rizzi ( 1988) a Flat Tripartite Structure This can not be the right structure because the branching is not "binary". 2. The Theme is more prominent than the Experiencer: VP V' A V Figure 4. Theme Experiencer Belletti and Rizzi ( 1988) Theme Prominent Structure 39 This is not the right structure because the Experiencer can not c-command the Theme. 3. The Experiencer is more prominent than the Theme: VP V' Experiencer ~ V Figure 5. Theme Belletti and Rizzi (1988) Experiencer Prominent Structure This is the only structure that is compatible with the evidence of Belletti and Rizzi. 4. The Theme and the Experiencer form a small clause: VP Theme Experiencer Figure 6. Belletti and Rizzi (1988) Theme and Experiencer as a Smalt Clause This can not be the right structure because there is no theta role for a small clause constituent. The Experiencer argument ofpreoccupare class is in the object position but it: • does not have the properties of canonical objects. • is a kind of secondary object. • is the sister ofV'. • is immediately dominated by VP. • is not transparent to extraction unlike canonical objects: 40 • has some object properties (it receives accusative case) Belletti and Rizzi claim that this accusative case is not the ordinary accusative case which we see in the object arguments of simple transitive verbs. The accusative case for them has two realizations which are structural and inherent. The general rule is this: V is a structural case assigner iff it has an external argument. The verbs of preoccupare class are not structural case assigners since they do not have external arguments. Therefore this accusative case is an inherent case. They observe that the auxiliary selection is also affected by this: • If a verb takes avere, it has an external argument. If it has an external argument, it is a structural case assigner and has a deep subject. • If a verb takes essere, it does not have an external argument. If it does not have an external argument, it is an inherent case assigner and it has a derived subject. The third class is the piacere class. Piacere class has the following properties: • Their Experiencer is in the inherent dative case. • They select the auxiliary essere. • Theme-Verb-Experiencer and Experiencer-Verb-Theme are possible orders. • The most natural word order is the second one which seems to be unmarked. • They are ergative verbs. • Their subject position is non-thematic. • Their Theme and the Experiencer are VP internal. 41 • Their Experiencer is higher than the Theme. (higher is to mean ccommanding) • Either their Themes or their Experiencers should be in preverbal position. Both should not remain in the VP. • Verb-Theme-Experiencer order is ungrammatical (as it is the case with the temere and preoccupare classes) which is fully grammatical with non-psych verbs: (31) *Please/ worry/ fear your ideas to Gianni. (32) Won the elections a student. As a result Belletti and Rizzi draw the following conclusions from the analysis of three types of psych verbs in Italian: • These three classes are identical to each other except for the selection of different inherent cases. • The Experiencer is projected to a higher position than the Theme (1988:344). • The lexical representations of three classes of psych verbs are (Belletti and Rizzi, 1988:344): <Class temere preoccupare piacere • All s-structures which have an inherent case marked NP in their subject positions have a derived subject • If a theta grid has both an Agent and an Experiencer, the external role is always given to the Agent. 42 • If there is no other highly ranked role than the Theme, or if the Theme is the only member in the theta grid, it can be chosen as the external argument. Otherwise a Theme can never be the external argument. • According to these generalizations, the following verb classes would not be possible which: a. are like temere class but with no external argument. b. are like preoccupare class but with a simple transitive structure. c. are like piacere class but with structural accusative case assigned to d. are like piacere class but with avere as an auxiliary. Theme. • According to these generalizations, the following verb classes would be possible which: a. assign inherent case for both the Experiencer and the Theme b. assign inherent case for the Theme and externalize the Experiencer I. 2. 1. 2. Pesetsky (1987) and the Binding Problems with Experiencer Verbs Am_o ng other peculiarities t~at Experiencer verbs pose, Peset~Jcy ( 1.9_87) deals with anaphoric binding problems such as the following: (1) Pictures ofhimselfl know John likes e. (2) Pictures of each other annoy politicians. (3) Each other's health worried the students. 43 The problem is that the anaphors in the sentences (1 ), (2) and (3) are not ccommanded by the antecedents in italics. It is important to note that the violations like these are only seen with the verbs which assign the Experiencer theta role to its object. Pesetsky observes that there is one more unusual property of Experiencer Object psych verbs; these verbs license an infinitival clause with Tough Movement (TM). In these constructions PRO in the infinitival clause is controlled by the Experiencer argument of the matrix verb as in (4) and (5): (4) These pictures(i) annoy mem [ PRO(i) to have to look e(i)]. (5) War(i) frightens meu> [PROm to think about e(i)]. but with non-Experiencer predicates it is ungrammatical: (6) *Bill(i) kicked mem [PROm to have to look at e(i)]. Pesetsky observes another peculiarity related to the meaning of sentences with Experiencer arguments. In order to interpret a sentence like John kissed Mary, we just need to know if John kissed Mary or not. We do not need to know other circumstances -aboutthe mentioned event. However, in order to interpret such a sentence like The article angered John we need to know what exactly is the thing that angered him. He calls this as "expressively incompleteness" (1987:130). Moving on from this point, Pesetsky predicts that there are deleted infinitival clauses in these sentences which have undergone TM. For him, the clause deletion is due to the discoursal factors. Therefore, the sentences in (7) and (9) are clause deleted counterparts of (8) and (1 0): (7) Pictures of each other annoy the politicians. (8) [Pictures of each other (i) ]G) annoy the politicians(i) fPRO(ifto look at [ecD]]. (9) Stories about herself generally pleased Mary. 44 (10) [Stories about herse(fii>] generally pleased Mary(i) [PRO(i) to hear [eJ (j) ]. The anaphor inside the subjects is bound by the Experiencer object which is non c-commanding. Pesetsky's assumptions related to this phenomenon are: • Experiencer Object psych predicates take an infinitive clause which undergoes TM. • This infinitive clause can be omitted. • PRO in the tough infinitive clause is controlled by the Experiencer. • e in the tough infinitive clause is c-commanded by PRO. • TM causes some connectivity effects. • As a result of this connectivity effect, the subject NP which includes the anaphor acts as if it is in the position of e. • The immediate antecedent of the anaphor is the PRO. Pesetsky claims that Experiencer predicates have always Agent-Patient counterparts and that these Agent-Patient usages exclude TM. Thus, in non Experiencer sentences we neither see TMs nor peculiar binding effects: (11) Bill deliberately annoyed me (*to talk to). (12) The actor deliberately frightens the children (*to look at). (13) *Each other's friends deliberately annoyed/ frightened the party- goers by blowing smoke in their faces. Pesetsky's claim that Experiencer psych predicates contain a deleted infinitival TM is only about the Experiencer predicates whose Experiencers surface as s-structure objects and does not include all Experiencer predicates. 45 Another claim IS that if the sentence involves TM, the nominalization IS ungrammatical: (14) *the book's annoyance of John. (15) *the book's amusement of children. Nominalization is possible with the Agent-Patient versions of these verbs when they do not contain an infinitival clause: (16) Mary's (deliberate) annoyance ofJohn. (17) Mary's (deliberate) amusement of children. Another observation is that the tough infinitive clause excludes reflexives as in: (18) ?I disgusted myself. (19) ?l(i) disgusted myself(i) [PRO to think about e(i)]. Pesetsky identifies two approaches as to the question of how many arguments these Experiencer verbs can have. According to the first approach, there are three arguments which are; an external NP, an Experiencer and an infinitive clause. The second approach does not regard the surface subject as an argument of the verb and identifies two arguments which are an Experiencer and an infinitive clause. Pesetsky notes that there are some verbs like amuse which require the existence of an infinitive clause "independent of TM": (20) It amuses Mary to read this book. (21) This book amuses Mary to read e. (22) *This book amuses Mary to read War and Peace. These are similar to raising structures. The subject position may also be occupied with an expletive element: (23) It seems that book has been read. 46 (24) That book seems to have been read. (25) *That book seems that War and Peace has been read. When passivization is considered, Pesetsky observes that some nominalizations seem better than others with passives: (26) ??Bill was delighted by Fred. (27) Bill was delighted by Fred's visit. (28) ??Mary was amazed by my shoes. (29) Mary was amazed by my choice of shoes. According to Pesetsky, (27) and (29) can be derived without TM. The reason of (26) and (28)'s seeming odd is because of the Causer argument. This argument can either refer to an action or an event. But here, a shoe is neither an event nor an action therefore, it can not be a theta marked argument of the verb but the choice of shoe can be. Pesetsky notes some thematic and selectional differences betwe-en verbal, adjectival and nominal examples. Therefore, there should be a distinction between the Cause of Emotion and the Target of Emotion: (30) Bill was angry at the article in the Times. (31) The article in the Times angered Bill. The sentence (31) differs from (30) in that the anger of Bill is not directed at the article but something in the article. However, when we use the adjectival form (30), we mean that the article itself is the object of his anger. Pesetsky claims that such pairs like anger-angry, fear-frighten are not simple lexical variants. The non Experiencer NP is sometimes the Object of Emotion and sometimes the Cause although it is possible to think of a Cause also as the Object of Emotion as in (32): 47 (32) Bill's behavior embarrassed Sue. Pesetsky's prediction is that " ... the noun and the adjective do not assign the Cause role because the related verb is a lexical causative ... " (1987:137). He points out that the causative verb can not assign the role of the Object of Emotion as, the ungrammaticality of (33) shows: (33) *The article in the Times angered Bill at the government. To conclude, Pesetsky's main argument is that it is possible to explain the peculiar binding properties of Experiencer object psych verbs with connectivity effects and with the existence of TM in these verbs. I. 2. 1. 3. Vanhoe (2002) and the Psych Verbs in Spanish Vanhoe (2002) analyzes the syntactic properties of psychological verbs m Spanish in the framework of LFG. He distinguishes three classes of psych verbs m Spanish. Each of these types presents a different correspondence pattern according to their thematic roles and syntactic positions. The first group is the temer class which is similar to the fear class in English. The second group is asustar or preoccupar class which is similar to frighten class in English and the third class is gus tar class with a dative Experiencer. In three of these classes, there is an Experiencer who reacts to a Theme emotionally. Vanhoe argues that aspectually, the temer and the gustar classes are states but preoccupar 'Class is achievement. States also differ in themselves; temer class behaves like ordinary transitive verbs but gustar class shows unaccusative behavior. He modifies Dowty's proto -Agent and proto Patient properties and analyzes three groups of psych verbs in Spanish according to them: 48 V anhoe discusses another phenomenon called "leismo" which means that a direct object may sometimes be marked with DAT. Most of the time it is optional, pragmatic or contextual in Spanish. However there are also some verbs in Spanish whose direct objects are obligatorily marked with DAT such as interesar. evidence I. 2. 1. 4. Mulder (1992) and the Unergative and Ergative Psych Verbs According to Belletti and Rizzi's analysis, the well known three types of psych verbs have similar underlying structural representations. Mulder (1992) states that these three sentences have radically different deep structures. The only similarity between them is that they all have an Experiencer argument. Other than this feature, .th~se y~rb~ have very little things in common. He refers to the second type (frighten type) of verbs as s-psych verbs and the third group as o-psych verbs which have the following properties: - 8-;' P,SY~H (]ONSTRUCJJQNS~'i'>; . . . ·E:&i)i.tit,z,: ~Q-l(:f,§,¥ClJ~"Q~~IRQg,!J~:n~.~~·~.'·t}Y'MAJ::£u·ii&'t':. -~ . The Experiencer is the subject of the state of mind. The Experiencer (the dative NP) is the subject of an inalienable possession construction and it functions as the possessor with a covert possessee. The Theme is the causal subject. The nominative Theme is the subject of an embedded small clause. There is a similarity between the synthetic and There is a variation between the synthetic and periphrastic forms. periphrastic forms. Inversion with these verbs is ungrammatical except They are morphologically complex since they for topicalization purposes. consist of an ergative matrix V, ~ posse~~ive empty verb and a small clause complement of a verb. Table 3. Mulder (1992) s- psych verbs and a-psych verbs Mulder argues that all s-psych verbs are VPs which are multi headed. He observes in Dutch that s-psych verbs have a reflexive counterpart, they can be agentive as in (1), reflexive as in (2) and causative as in (3): 49 (1) Teun interested the student for English. (agentive in Dutch) (2) The student interested for English. (reflexive in Dutch) (3) Linguistics interested the student the most. (causative in Dutch) Mulder claims that in constructions like (1) there is the corporation of either a psych N or an ADJ into a causative V. In (2) the Experiencer argument is a "weak reflexive" and in (3) there is a reflexive interpretation of a PP (see that in this case the PP can not be realized overtly). According to Mulder, the Experiencer argument is an argument who .either possesses a mental state or is the subject of a psychological state (1992:122). However, this possession that we see between an Experiencer and a psych noun in psych verbs is inalienable possession. The Experiencer possesses the mental state which is represented by the psych N. It is in fact an obligatory complement but since it is an inalienable possession it is non-overt. I. 2. 1. 5. Hale and Keyser (2002) on Psych Verbs Hale and Keyser argue that, as it is the case with many transitive verbs, Experiencer Subject psych verbs can not participate in middle constructions. The following are their examples for that (14): (1) *John's talent envies easily. (Everyone envies John's talent.) (2) *French films love easily. (My kids love French films easily.) On the other hand Experiencer Object psych verbs can form middle constructions as in: (3) Politicians anger easily. (The truth anger politicians.) 50 (4) This colt frightens easily. (Loud noises frighten this colt.) (5) I worry easily. (Economic down-turns worry me.) (6) Children bore easily. (Adult talk bores children.) One of the reasons for Experiencer subject psych verbs' not forming middles may be that the Experiencer is not affected by the action denoted by these verbs. In Experiencer Object verbs it is affected. Therefore the first group does not meet the Affectedness Requirement. Another explanation for these structures would be hidden in the semantic connection between the internal and external arguments. In the following examples, the psych nominals contain genitive nominals which refer to the Experiencer: (7) Mary has my respect. (I respect Mary.) (8) She has the boss's esteem. (The boss esteems her.) (9) He has his children's love. (His children love him.) Hale and Keyser claim that without these genitives, the Experiencer disappears as in Mary has the respect or He has love. The paraphrases of the sentences above would be: (10) I give my respect to Mary. (11) The boss gives her his esteem. (12) His children give him their love. Hale and Keyser assume that psych nouns are bare nouns which have a part relation to some whole entity. The following paraphrased examples show that the emotion (here love) is possessed by the external argument (Mary) not by the internal argument (the children) as it is the case with all Experiencer psych v~rbs: ( 13) Mary loves her children. (14) ~gives her children her love. 51 This anaphoric properties of psych nouns are regarded by Hale and Keyser as the reason of the failure of Experiencer subject psych verbs to form middles because middle forms lack an external argument. I. 2. 1. 6. Ginnis (nd.) on the Morphological Restrictions in Experiencer Predicates Basing herself on Pesetsky (1990) Causer Target/ Subject Matter distinction, Ginnis gives examples from psych predicates which allow either an Experiencer or a Suggestor subject. She describes the Experiencer as the argument which undergoes the psychological state and the Suggestor as the argument which refers to the behavior described by the psychological state and claims that a predicate can not have both a Suggestor and T/SM arguments at the same time: (1) Carol was fearful of earthquakes. Expei'iencer T/SM (2) Carol's expression was fearful. Suggestor (3) * Carol's expression was fearful of earthquakes. Suggestor T/SM Ginnis explains the ill formedness of (1) with synthetic causatives like frighten with a morphological restriction on causative elements. In English, if possible, lexical causatives are inserted but if the verb has no synthetic causative, the default form is used. Ginnis states that the T/SM violation with synthetic causatives in English can be eliminated by using an "analytic default". These processes that she mentions can be summarized for English like this: 52 (Causative) <-> -ify in env. Root+ ASP+ X: terr-, horr- (Causative) <-> -en in env. Root+ASP+_ _ Y: fright ... (Causative) <-> (Causative) <-> in env. Root+ASP+_ _ Z: please, disgust, anger ... make elsewhere The aspectual head (ASP) in her analysis is not recursive but rather the VP is recursive; one causative VP may take another causative VP as its complement, so, many languages like Turkish have multiple causative constructions. I. 2. 1. 7. Kural (1996) on Elementary Predicates and Psych Verbs in Turkish Kural ( 1996) identifies seven abstract elementary predicates for the syntactic representation which are: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. Actor Causative Passive Volitionality Control Inchoative Beneficiary ACT CAUSE PASS VOL CONT INCH BEN Kural argues that there is a correlation between the Beneficiary and the Experiencer roles. He sees the Experiencer role as an "instantiation" of the Beneficiary role. He accepts the for PP in the specifier position of BEN as the Experiencer argument which experiences the mentioned benefit. He states that John in the following sentences is the Experiencer argument: (1) The recent changes in the regulations benefited John. (2) John benefited from the recent changes in the regulations. 53 The alternation above can also be seen m pairs like fear/frighten and like/ please: (3) The recent changes in the regulations worried John. (4) John worried about the recent changes in the regulations. Kurallists four basic thematic roles: 1. Actor: manipulates the event 2. Experiencer: internally responds to the event. 3. Neutral: remains outside the event. 4. Patient: undergoes a change of state as a result of the event. He introduces two other phenomena inherent in these roles; affectedness and protagonism: Affectedness related to the change of state actors and Neutrals are unaffected Experiencers and Patients are affected created arguments are not affected destructed arguments are affected only the affected objects can possessivize affected arguments are allowed as the derived subjects of middles but unaffected arguments do not inflectional materials like modality and negation are irrelevant for affectedness contextual information IS irrelevant for affectedness Table 4. .Protagonism related to active participation protagonists actively determines the course of the events actors and Experiencers are protagonists Neutrals and Patients are nonprotagonists Kural (1996) Affectedness and Protogomsm These two properties can be combined with four basic thematic roles in the following way: Actor Experiencer Neutral Patient Unaffected Affected Unaffected Affected Table 5. Protagonist Protagonist Non-protagonist Non-protagonist Kural (1996) Affectedness, Protogomsm and Thematic roles 54 Kural mentions two hierarchies about the mapping of these thematic arguments on to syntactic positions (1996:39): 1. Affectedness Hierarchy: An unaffected role is mapped onto a higher argument. 2. Protagonism Hierarchy: A protagonist role is mapped onto a higher argument. He identifies ten possible thematic combinations for transitive dyadic verbs (Kural, 1996:40). Among these, the ones that include the Experiencer argument can be summarized in the following chart: Few in number Unaffected subject and affected object -··· Both are protagonists With verbs like con, influence, irritate, ridicule, stimulate, train etc. NeutralCauses a clash between the A and P hierarchies. Experlencer The choice of subject depends on alternating individual verbs admire, enry, fear, hate, like, pity, resent, believe, hope, think, want, understand, need, taste take Experiencer subjects amuse, confuse, disgust, frighten, offend, please, satisfy take Neutral subjects Mostly take Experiencer subjects ~ ~-E±0 Experience~;;;;:. ; ;~ A gap in the paradigm Experiencer ;. · Two participants can not internally respond to the event Lack an independent event which would be responded. · Experiencer-; ~ ··· ~ A gap in the paradigm Patient An Experiencer subject can not change the Patient's state ActorExpe_r.ien~~r -· .. 4 Table 6. Kural (1996) Thematic Combmatlons Wlth the Expenencer Argument Kural states that having a desire or thought is also a kind of change of state because state is not only something physical but also something mental or social. An Experiencer is an affected argument because it changes its mental attributes in some or other way. An Experiencer is a protagonist at the same time because " ... the person who is enjoying or wanting something is engaged in a mental activity that determines the enjoying or the wanting" and an Experiencer mentally processes the event " ... a person who is reminiscing or fantasizing about some situation must also be mentally constructing that situation" (Kural, 1996:38). 55 This assumption of Kural is criticized by Dikken ( 1996) claiming that such a sentence like: (5) Sue believes in Santa Claus. " .. .is perfectly true even Sue has always believed Santa Claus and will continue to believe in Santa Claus till the end of time" (Dikken, 1996: 12) implying no change of state of mind of Sue at all. Kural's reason for the absence of Experiencer- Experiencer verbs is that the Experiencer Subject makes the verb stative but Experiencer Object forces the verb to be eventive and the result is a clash between the two. Neutral- Experiencer verbs are said to cause a clash between the two hierarchies because affectedness hierarchy says that the Neutral role should be mapped onto the higher argument, on the other hand, protagonism hierarchy says that the Experiencer role should be mapped onto the higher argument. His observation is that in most of the world's languages the Experiencer argument occupies the subject position. Therefore we can say that there is a precedence relationship between the two hierarchies. Kural says that in this relationship the protagonism hierarchy takes the precedence. It links the Experiencer to the subject position. Only when protagonism can not determine, the affectedness hierarchy applies. Kural shows this precedence rule as: Protagonism Hierarchy> Affectedness Hierarchy He claims that Experiencer Subject Neutral Object verbs like fear can be analyzed with a simple VP structure like: 56 VP A DP V' /"'.... V Experiencer like DP NEUTRAL Figure 7. Kural (1996) Experiencer Subject Neutral Object Verbs Neutral Subject Experiencer Object verbs have a different representation. Kural argues that they are triadic verbs and there is a CAUSE which adds a Neutral argument: VP pp V' A NEUTRAL v VP ~ CAUSE DP V' A v pp I I please NEUTRAL Figure 8. Kural (1996) Neutral Subject Experiencer Object Verbs Here the Neutral argument in the specifier position of CAUSE initiates the mental state of the Experiencer just like Pesetsky's Subject Matter and the Neutral argument in the specifier position of the verb is just like Pesetsky's Target of Emotion. Kural quotes Pesetsky's claim that the Causer argument is the Target when there is not a Neutral argument as in: (6) Suna Mehmet'i bahktan igrendirdi. 57 If there was not an internal argument like balzktan the Causer in this sentence would be the Target Suna. Kural takes all these as the evidence that this constraint is just related to surface structure because these two arguments can co occur when they are kept apart in the surface structure as in the periphrastic causatives in (7) and in verb particle constructions as in (8): (7) Bill made Mary worry about his progress in the class. (8) The lectures turned Bill on to classical music. Kural explains the difference between the ungrammatical (1 0) and the grammatical ( 11) as the type of emotion that they denote: (9) Mehmet Suna'nm kopeginden korktu. (10) *Suna Mehmet'i kopeginden korkuttu. (11) Ahmet Ay~e'ye caz1 sevdirdi. His generalization is that psych verbs which refer to temporary emotions (such as kederlen-, kzz-, sevin-, ~a~zr-, utan-, iirk- etc.) obey this restriction while verbs which refer to permanent emotions (such as bez-, bunal-, gocun-, igren-, imren-, ku~kulan-, oykiin-, sev- etc.) do not. In Koral's analysis the verb fear has a different status among these verbs because fear can be both a permanent property towards an object or it may be a temporary state "aroused anew in each situation" (1996:133). I. 2. 2. Semantic and Aspectual Approaches to Psych Verbs In this section, first the aspectual approaches to psych verbs will be examined through the analyses of Grimshaw (1990), Tenny (1994), Voorst (1992), Filip (1996), Arad (nd.) and Levin and Hovav (2002). Then, other significant semantic approaches based on 58 non aspectual criteria will be overviewed through the analyses of Dowty (1991) and Hatory (1997). Wechsler (1995), Krifka (2001) and Kordoni (2000 and 2001) focus on the semantic concepts of notion and underlying, and, Dabrowska (1994) examines the case marking of Experiencer arguments in Polish. I. 2. 2. 1. Grimshaw (1990) Prominence Theory and Psych Verbs Grimshaw (1990) assumes that the argument structure has its own internal structure or organization and this organization is a reflection of the lexical semantics of the verb. Thus, the argument structure of a verb can be predictable from the general characteristics of the meaning of a verb. This assumption commits itself to the idea that the syntactic representation can be derived from the semantic representation. She claims that there are prominence relations among the arguments of a given verb and, the argument structure is a representation of these prominence relations. These prominence relations are determined by two properties of verbs which are the thematic properties and the aspectual properties. The theory presents the ex~ernal argument as the most prominent argument in thematic and aspectual dimensions which is equal to the dstructure subject. An argument is accepted to be either external or internal according to its relation to other arguments in the argument structure. The theory assumes the following hierarchy for the arguments: (Agent (Experiencer (Goal/Source/Location (Theme)))) The hierarchy indicates that the Agent is the highest argument, and then comes the Experiencer, Goal, Source, Location and lastly the Theme. The most prominent 59 argument in the argument structure always occupies syntactically the most prominent position. In this theory, the special status of psychological verbs is due to the mismatch between their thematic and aspectual properties. Fear class verbs and their prominence relations are like other agentive verbs. Frighten class verbs on the other hand do not meet expectations since their thematically the most prominent argument (Experiencer) does not occupy the syntactic subject position: Fear (x Exp. (y)) Theme Frighten (x (y)) Theme Exp. Unlike Belletti and Rizzi (1988), Grimshaw ( 1990) argues that the Theme of a frighten verb is a d-structure subject. The idea defended in the theory is that these two classes of verbs have the same thematic prominence relations although they differ in the dstructure representation of their arguments. The mismatch is because of the difference in their aspectual properties. More specifically, frighten class verbs are causative but not stative while fear class verbs are stative, and do not have an event reading. The result is that the causal status of an argument plays a role for this argument's syntactic realization either as a subject or as an object. Grimshaw regards this dimension as autonomous from the thematic dimension. She mentions two different kinds of dimensions; one thematic, the other causal. "Each of the two hierarchies imposes its own set of prominence relations on this collection of arguments ... " (1990:24). The causal hierarchy is: 60 (Cause (Other (. .. ))) For frighten verbs, the first element in the thematic hierarchy corresponds to the second position in the causal hierarchy and vice versa. For the fear class, the Experiencer argument, both thematically and aspectually the most prominent one but for frighten class the Theme is the aspectually the most prominent. Grimshaw states that frighten type verbs cross linguistically seem to be more stable than the fear type. She defines another class of psych verbs whose semantics are similar to fear type verbs which can be regarded as unaccusatives (such as concern and please). The underlying idea of the theory is that every argument has a prominence status relative to the other arguments in the mentioned two dimensions. The external argument is the one which is the most prominent in two dimensions. A verb can only have one external argument since only one argument can have maximal prominence. Frighten verbs are different from other causatives in that they have no external arguments. Therefore, they can not undergo such processes like nominalization or passivization which require the suppression of the external argument. Here, Grimshaw distinguishes between the two notions; the d-structure subject and the external argument. Frighten verbs have d- · structure objects but not external arguments. She also differentiates between causatives and unaccusatives. She states that frighten verbs have an argument which has a maximal ~pectual prominence but not an external argument which has a maximal thematic prominence. The unaccusatives too, do not have an external argument since they do not have a first sub event. She claims that the only argument of unaccusative verbs is in the d-structure object not a subject. Although 61 both classes of verbs i.e. unaccusatives and frighten type verbs have internal arguments, Grimshaw says that the aspectual status of these arguments are different from each other. I. 2. 2. 2. Tenny (1994) Aspectual Interface Hypothesis and Psych Verbs According to Tenny (1994), the intersection or the visible side of the lexical semantics is the side of the aspectual properties which regulate much of the linking. These aspectual properties which are related to syntax are of a very limited kind. Specifically she sees delimitedness as the only aspectual property which plays a role in the syntax semantics interface. For her, delimitedness refers to an inherent endpoint of events. It depends not only on the verb but also the other elements in the verb phrase. She argues that the internal arguments of stative verbs do not undergo any change or motion. They do not also have an internal temporal structure. They just describe states. ., She demonstrates the difference between the fear and the frighten type verbs with the differences between their entailments as in (1) and (2): (1)a. The truth frightened John in five minutes. Entails that b. It took five minutes for John to become frightened. c. John was frightened at the end of five minutes. (2) a. ?John feared the truth in five minutes. Does not entail b. It took five minutes for John to fear the truth. c. John was feared at the end of five minutes. 62 She gives further evidence for the differences between these two classes by using rate adverbials to modify the argument that undergoes a change of state. In this way we can measure out the change of the changed argument as in: (3) The truth interested John only slowly, since he was not imaginative by nature and was slow to see its implications. A delimiting expression can also be used with a causative change of state verb to refer to the ends tate of the internal argument as in: (4) The movie frightened the children to death. (5) *The movie feared the children to death. Tenny describes the constraint related to the example (9) as the Measure-Out Constraint which states that only the direct internal arguments (not external arguments) can undergo a change of state and only they are able to measure out an event. The verbs whose Experiencers are the internal arguments can express a change of state but verbs whose Themes are the internal arguments can not. Only the verbs which have Theme subjects can be causative while Experiencer subject verbs can not (Tenny, 1994:67). Tenny's argument related to psych verbs is that they conform to the aspectual restrictions on the internal and external arguments in the Aspectual Interface Hypothesis. Their internal arguments are also constrained by the Measuring-Out Constraint. She simply sees the Experiencer argument of a psych verb as an event participant which sometimes measure out and sometimes does not measure out events. As an internal argument, it measures out the event, as an external argument, it does not. The Theme argument of a psych verb, on the other hand, undergoes a change of state. If the Theme is the internal argument, the verb (Experiencer- Subject Theme- Object verb) is not a change of state verb; otherwise it is (67). 63 I. 2. 2. 3. Voorst (1992) and the Aspectual Semantics of Psych Verbs Most of the theories about the aspectual properties of psych verbs including Grimshaw and Tenny claim that the differences between the classes of psych verbs and peculiar properties of the verbs offrighten class are due to the differences in their aspectual properties. Grimshaw sees fear type verbs as stative and frighten type verbs as causative. Unlike this common assumption, Voorst (1992) argues that there are not aspectual differences in the deep structures of these classes and therefore, the thing to do is to find out descriptions other than aspectual and thematic ones. Voorst identifies four classes of psychological verbs in English and claim that even though there are some semantic differences among these classes; all of them behave similarly in the aspectual tests. His classification of psych verbs ( 1992:66, 67) can be summarized like this: 1. Class 1 Psych Verbs: Psychological uses of action verbs: He struck me as rather odd. 2. Class 2 Psych Verbs: Psychological verbs with an intentional subject: The clown tried to amuse me. 3. Class 3 Psych Verbs: Psychological verbs with a non-intentional subject: These experiences amused me tremendously. , 4. Class 4 Psych Verbs: Psychological verbs of dislike type in which the subject is the centre of the psychological experience. These art-connoisseurs admire Van Gogh. Voorst in the analysis of activity and accomplishment verbs reaches the following conclusions about psych verbs: 64 • They do not allow end reading. Begin reading is also sometimes unacceptable since it is difficult to predict the exact moment of the occurrence of any kind of feelings. • They allow the adverb for X minutes similar to activities and states. • With the use of the adverb almost, all classes of psych verbs have the interpretation that the event has failed to occur. • Unlike activities and accomplishments, individuation of the direct object of psych verbs does not affect their aspectual semantics. • They do not imply a process leading to a change of state. Voorst then, analyzes the behavior of psych verbs comparing them to the achievement and state verbs and observes that they (just like achievements and states) refer to the beginning of the state or event. With the adverb in X minutes, only a begin reading is possible for them. Voorst distinguishes between the states and events analyzing the behavior of psych verbs and observes that: • They describe a more permanent feeling without the progressive form. On the contrary, when the verb is in progressive, a less permanent feeling is .. conveyed. • When their subjects can not be interpreted as agentive, these verbs are states. • Their imperative form is better with an optative meaning and with an implied agentive subject rather than giving an order. • They allow passivization unlike states. • Unlike states, they allow adverbs which specify the intensity. 65 As a result, psychological verbs are not like accomplishment verbs because they do not imply a process. They are not also like activities because the individuation does not transform them into accomplishments. Lastly, they are not like states because they take place or happen unlike them. These all leads him to conclude that psychological verbs are achievements. Another observation of Voorst is that manner adverbs such as carefully and slowly can be used with psych verbs with perfect grammaticality but these adverbs do not modify the psychological process. For example when we say: (1) Peter carefully frightened the grizzly bear to chase it out of its backyard. carefully does not refer to the performing the process of frightening. Another important distinction is between subjects denoting individuals and subjects denoting events. In (2) the subject is the individual himself while in (3) the subject is not the individuals but something about the individuals: (2) He demonstrated that the situation was getting worse each day. (3) These kids demonstrated that the situation was getting worse each day. (the kids' starving appearance etc.) The measurability that Voorst mentions is not the kind of measurability that of Tenny's. Voorst puts forward that psychological verbs are unmeasurable events since we can not talk about the existence of such a process which leads to final state. That means, just like achievement verbs, psych verbs do not have a culminationpoint. Voorst adapts Ryle's (1949) classification of emotions expressed by psych constructions. He mentions three types of emotions: 1. Inclinations: They refer to permanent emotional dispositions or traits of character. 66 Moods: They are less permanent dispositions or "short term 2. tendency words". Agitations: They are more or less lasting dispositions. 3. Voorst claims that "Constructions with psychological verbs are centered around moods and those containing psychological uses of accomplishments around inclinations underlying moods" (1992:90). For him, the reason for psychological verbs' not being accomplishments is that they are centered around moods which can be manipulated. I. 2. 2. 4. Filip (1996) on the Psych Verbs in Czech Filip challenges the principles in the Aspectual Interface Hypothesis giving evidence from psych verbs in Czech and English that telicity or measuring out the events can not be the basis for the syntax semantics interface. Filip argues that only the psychological predicates which are achievements (the ones which refer to spontaneous changes of mental state) are telic. He gives examples from some psychological causatives (like calm, disillusion, sadden, soothe, disarm etc.) which do not entail a clear cut final stage. These verbs do not co occur with adverbs like halfway and therefore they are atelic as in: (1) ?The music halfway saddened John. (2) *The high-pitched noise halfway distracted her. Filip argues against Tenny's test with incremental expressions like a little bit, saying that this expression relates only events as a whole not their increments or parts. For example when we say: (3) The music saddened John a little bit. 67 we do not mean the part of a larger event but we mean a kind of lower intensity of the whole psychological event. In Czech Filip identifies three types of psych verbs: 1. Nominative Experiencer 2. Accusative Experiencer 3. Dative Experiencer According to Filip there 1s a correlation between the case assignment and thematic roles: • Nominative case of the Experiencer is associated with the existence of sentience and volition • Accusative or dative case of the Experiencer is associated with the existence of a lack or lower degree of control • Nominative Experiencers are either the cause of the event or they are volitional. • Accusative Experiencers are either causally affected or undergo a change of state. Accusative Experiencer verbs can undergo passivization. • Dative Experiencer verbs can not undergo passivization. • Dative Experiencers are generally beneficiaries. • Dative Experiencer verbs can not be modified with manner adverbs like in a pleasant manner, passionately, bitterly but they can be modified with degree or intensity adverbials like a lot and a little. To conclude, Filip's main argument is that telicity or any other semantic property is not useful in explaining the linking properties of psych verbs in Czech. 68 I. 2. 2. 5. Arad (nd.) on Psych Verbs Arad (nd.) has two basic claims about the syntactic and semantic properties of psych verbs. The first one is that the different features that belong to Object-Experiencer (OE) verbs are only seen in their stative reading i.e. with an agentive reading these features do no exist. Her second claim is that OE psych verbs are not inherently psych but they are either formed from normal predicates or they do have also non-psych senses. The main argument is that the peculiar features of OE psych verbs belong to their stativity. Arad shows that OE psych verbs have three readings which associate with different syntactic constructions. This shows that a great number of verbs have both a psych and a non-psych sense and even these senses have different readings in themselves. The mentioned three readings are: 1. Agentive reading: There is an intentional Agent and a change of state in the Experiencer: (1) Nina frightened Laura (to make her go away). 2. Eventive reading~ There is an unintentional Agent and a change of state: (2) The explosion/ noise/ thunderstorm frightened Laura. 3. Stative reading: - There is neither an Agent nor a change of state in the Experiencer. - The Experiencer experiences this mental state as long as she perceives the Stimulus. - There is a kind of stative causation (unlike the other two readings in which there is an active causation). - The stative Causer is not an external argument 69 -The stative Causer is not an affected argument. -The stative Causer is an "external internal, argument (Arad, nd: 16). -The stative Causer can only be generated under the specifier position ofVP. Arad notes that there are some verbs which allow only one of these readings. For example while the verb frighten allows three readings, verbs like concern and worry allow only stative readings. Arad's main prediction is that all of the psych properties exist only in the stative reading of OE verbs. In the agentive reading these verbs are like normal transitive verbs. Another claim is that almost every argument position can be interpreted as an Experiencer. It can be a direct object, an indirect object or a PP. What determines the interpretation of an argument as an Experiencer or a Goal etc. is the verb and its arguments (Arad, nd: 13- 20). Arad adopts Bouchard's (1995) assumption that all verbs can be interpreted as a psych verb if some conditions are met. For this, first there should be an animate argument which can be interpreted as an Experiencer and second, there should be an internal argument which can be interpreted as an emotion or a mental state (Arad, nd: 14). I. 2. 2. 6. Levin and Hovav (2002) on Psych Verbs Levin and Hovav (2002) claim that to decide whether an event with two sub events is a complex event or not, we should look at the temporal relations between the subevents. Therefore the internal temporal constitution of an event which is an aspectual feature is relevant to argument expression. 70 They claim that mental verbs like read, copy, memorize, translate, study, recite and perform etc. are like accomplishments but they are not complex events. The object that is read is not affected by the reading activity. The reader in a way internalizes the text and gains a mental representation from it. They argue that these verbs have incremental objects like verbs of consumption rather than verbs of change of state. Their objects are measurable NPs, thus they are telic. They analyze the mental verb read and say that it has two sub events. The first sub event is scanning the text and the second sub event is forming a mental representation. The point is that these two sub events may be temporally dependant because while the text is scanned, a mental representation is formed at the same time. Thus they do not regard these verbs as complex events (Levin and Hovav, 2002:15). They further point out that incremental Themes of these verbs do not have to be syntactically expressed as the direct object as in (1): (1) Kelly read/studied. (2) Kelly read/studied from the textbook. I. 2. 2. 7. Dowty (1991) on Proto Roles and Psych Verbs Dowty ( 1991) mentions about an indeterminacy in argument selection with some verbs which he calls lexical doublets. These verbs have different argument configurations but they express the same relation. For example, verbs like buy and sell, borrow and lend do not distinguish between their buyer and seller, borrower and lender with respect to the properties of proto roles oftheir arguments. 71 Under his view, psychological predicates are also different kinds of doublets. He classifies the following verbs(Dowty, 1991:579) into two groups as: Experiencer-Subject Stimulus-Subject x likes y x fears y x supposes (that) S x regards y (as) VP x is surprised at y x is disturbed at y y pleases x y frightens x (it) seems (to) x (that) S y strikes x (as) VP y surprises x y disturbs x In the first group of verbs, Experiencer percepts the Stimulus therefore the Experiencer is "sentient/ perceiving" but in the second group Stimulus argument is the Causer of the "emotional reaction or cognitive judgment" of the Experiencer. Dowty argues that both of these arguments have " ... a weak but apparently equal claim to subjecthood" (1991 :579). The first group of verbs are stative verbs but verbs in the second group are sometimes stative sometimes inchoative. The Experiencer has the proto patient properties when there is an inchoative interpretation but this is not the case for the statives. Therefore, according to Dowty's argument selection principles, this argument has the most proto patient properties (although both arguments have the same amount of proto agent properties) and it becomes the direct object. Dowty in his proto role analysis of psychological verbs claims that the Experiencer Object verbs have a Stimulus/Causer argument which causes a cognitive judgment or emotional reaction in the Experiencer. The Experiencer argument is seen as the participant who undergoes a kind of change of state. He notes that the Experiencer argument has the perception of the Stimulus. This property is a proto Agent property for the Experiencer. The Stimulus on the other hand causes the Experiencer to perceive it 72 (which is also a proto Agent property for the Stimulus). This is which causes a conflicting mappmg. I. 2. 2. 8. Hatory (1997) and the LCSs of Psych Verbs Hatori (1997) views psych verbs as ordinary transitive verbs in terms of -able, -er and middle formation as in the following examples: (1) Sue frightened easily. (middle formation) (2) excitable, irritable, upsettable (-able adjective formation) (3) disturber, enchanter, flatterer, startler (-er noun formation) Hatori observes that some verbs of change of location can be extended to more abstract semantic fields such as a change of state; therefore they both have the same LCSs such as the following ones: (4) a. John drove his mother to the station. b. John drove his mother mad. (5) a. The breeze moved the leaves slightly. b. The story of their sufferings moved us deeply. (6) a. The branches touch the roof. b. Her kindness touched me profoundly. Hatori observes in these examples that when these verbs are used with their psychological meaning they co-occur with such adverbs like deeply, profoundly etc. and they are also able to derive psych adjectives with -ing as in moving and touching. 73 Hatori suggests two approaches to analyze psych verbs. The first approach sees the Experiencer argument of these verbs as the Location or the Goal. The second approach sees it as the Theme. According to the first approach the Experiencer can be either in the dative or in the oblique case. The examples in which the preposition to coming after some psych adjectives are presented as the evidence for that: (7) The point was clear to me. (8) They are interesting to the students of comparative literature. The second approach that regards the Experiencer argument as the Theme sees us as the Theme and film and the director as the Agent: (9) The film frightened us. ( 10) The director sent us. Another observation of Hatori is that psych verbs can co-occur with resultative expresswns: (11) He awed them into obedience. (12) He was shamed out ofbad habits. (13) His suffering distressed him into committing suicide. Hatori's argument is that the verbs that are compatible with path expressions have the LCS with either GO or MOVE function. However, some other psych verbs can not co-occur with path expressions: (14) * The storyteller's jokes amused the children into giggling. Hatori claims that in the LCS of these verbs, there is not a GO or MOVE function but a BE function, therefore, they do not refer to an emotional motion or an Experiencer that undergoes it. They have either a stative or an inchoative meaning. With 74 these verbs the Theme argument can be realized as a with phrase mostly in passive constructions: (15) The Chinese dinner satisfied Bill. (16) Bill was satisfied with the Chinese dinner. He also gives examples from a few number of psych verbs which can be followed by both a with phrase and a path phrase. One of them is the verb bore and the verb worry: (17) ... a handsome engineer who bored me to tears with his tales of motorway maintenance. ( 18) You worry me to death with your chatting. To sum up, Hatory's analysis of LCSs of EO psych verbs classifies them into two as one with a GO/ Move function and the other with a BE function. There are some other psych verbs which are used with body parts. Here, the use of body part expressions (like head, mind, nerve, head, soul, stomach and chest etc.) gives a kind of metaphorical meaning. The observation is that identical body part nouns are used across languages. In a number of languages these body part expressions can be used as the Experiencer argument. This should be because humans conceptualize emotions very similarly cross linguistically. I. 2. 2. 9. Wechsler (1995) Notion Rule and Psych Verbs Wechsler (1995) introduces a concept called notion to define the semantic constraints on argument structure. He claims that this concept underlies the semantic 75 constraints even of psychological verbs which constitute a problematic class. He defines notions as mental entities or concrete cognitive structures (Wechsler, 1995:32). In order to explain the semantic constraints on argument structure, he defines a set of notion rules. He analyzes the verbs in the following sentences (1), (2), (3) and (4): (1) a. John wants the cat. b. John has a notion of the cat. c. * The cat has a notion of John. (2) a. John likes Mary. b. John has a notion of Mary. c. * Mary has a notion of John. (3) a. John fears Mary. b. John has a notion of Mary. c. * Mary has a notion of John. (4) a. John is expecting Fred. b. John has a notion of Fred. c. * Fred has a notion of John. He formulates the following Notion Rule for the psych verbs want, like, fear and expect in (1), (2), (3) and (4); The individual (here John) has a notion of X (the object I content of his mental state) but this X does not have to have a notion of John necessarily. In each of the sentences the (b)s are entailed but the (c)s are not. Thus reverse entailments are not true. For example in (1), in order to want the cat, John should have a notion of the cat but that does not mean that the cat should also have the notion of John. Wechsler claims that this notion rule is also valid for the verbs of perception: 76 (5) John saw the cat. a. John has a notion of cat. b. *The cat has a notion of John. Wechsler claims that frighten type verbs seem to violate the notion rule but they do not. They, like the fear type, are consistent with the notion rule. He emphasizes that frighten type verbs are eventive. They describe the causation of the mental state but they do not describe anything about the intentionality of the result state. He distinguishes between Cause and Content saying that they are distinct concepts. However they may sometimes refer to the same entity as in: (6) The dog frightened the baby. Wechsler accepts the existence of some psych verbs as counter examples to the notion rule such as concern and preoccupy: (7) *Toxic waste concerns the Senator deeply- he just happens to be unaware of its existence. Wechsler says that for an X to concern or preoccupy a Y, Y must have some notion of x. He argues that these verbs are few in number, their number does not seem to increase and they are problematic for language learning. I. 2. 2. 10. Kritka (2001) on Psych Verbs According to Croft (1991) one of the tests to distinguish between the Experiencer- Subject and Stimulus- Subject verbs is that only the second type of verbs allows by phrases. However Experiencer- Subject verbs use causal clauses instead of byphrases as in (Krifka, 2001:11): 77 (1) John pleased his boss by coming in early everyday. (2) a. *John was liked by his boss by coming in early everyday. b. John was liked by his boss because he came in early everyday. Other than the known two groups, Krifka identifies two more classes of psychological verbs. The first group does not have a direct object but a Stimulus subject: (3) The painting appealed to Mary. The second group does not have a direct object too but has an Experiencer subject: (4) The soldiers rejoiced about the victory. Krifka claims that Experiencer- Stimulus verbs should also include perception verbs such as see verbs like feel, hear, smell, taste, notice etc. , sight verbs like discover, examine, inspect, perceive and verbs that refer to propositional attitudes like know, believe, doubt etc. Krifka exemplifies that although Stimulus- Subject verbs refer to punctual changes, they can be used with the progressive tense: (5) The storm was frightening the people. but Experiencer-Subject verbs have a stative reading and can not be used with progressive: (6) *The people were fearing the storm. Krifka claims that although most of the Stimulus- Subject verbs in languages are often regarded as causatives, the kind of causation by a Stimulus is not actually causation since it does not undergo causative alternation as non psych verbs do: (7) a. Someone broke the glass. b. The glass broke. (8) a. Someone frightened John. 78 b. *John frightened. According to Dowty's analysis, if the verb refers to a change of state, the Stimulus has a proto Agent property and it is realized as the syntactic subject. Similarly if the Experiencer has a notion of the Stimulus, the Experiencer has a proto Agent property and it is syntactically realized as the subject. What he means by notion is Wechsler's (1995) concept of notion which simply refers to knowing the entity which is the Cause of the experience. Other differences between the Stimulus- Subject and Experiencer-Subject verbs are listed by Krifka (200 1: 13-15) as follows: • Reciprocals reinforce the agentive reading of Stimulus-Subject verbs: The children entertained us. (ambiguous between agentive and non (9) agentive readings) (10) • The children entertained each other. (only the agentive reading) In some languages such as Italian, Stimulus- Subject verbs can not be reflexive (since their subjects are derived subjects as argued by Belletti and Rizzi). • The object of Stimulus- Subject verbs can bind anaphors within the subject. • The subject has fewer subject properties than the object in Stimulus-Subject verbs (this pattern assigns some subject properties to the Stimulus such as syntactic position, verb agreement, case marking etc.). • Experiencer-Subject verbs are regarded as the basic form but the number of Stimulus-Subject verbs is greater in (which is not the case in Turkish). 79 I. 2. 2. 11. Kordoni (2000 and 2001) on the Psych Verbs in Modern Greek Kordoni, (2000) provides evidence against the idea that the Experiencer Object psych verbs are causative constructions. Within the framework of Head Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HDPSG), she identifies three classes of psych verbs in Modem Greek (MG). The first class is similar to fear class in English. It includes verbs with an Experiencer Subject and a Theme Object. The second class just like the frighten class in English. It has a Theme or a Cause argument which is in the nominative case and which agrees with the verb. There is one more class of psych verbs in Modem Greek. This class of verbs has dative Experiencers and nominative Theme arguments. Kordoni (2000) analyzes Modem Greek Experiencer- Subject ·psych verb constructions (MG ESPVCs) in the framework of Wechsler's Notion Rule. One of the claims that he proposed is the object arguments of MG ESPVCs are "semantically underspecified" (199). By semantically underspecified he means, in these constructions, the meaning of the verb does not pose any constraints over the object argument and it is nonspecific about the nature of this argument. In the following examples (1) and (2) Gianis has a notion of Mary in order to love or fear from him. However Mary can be either cognitive or non-cognitive and we need to have some other contextual information to be sure about that: ( 1) a. 0 Gianis agap a tin Maria. John loves Mary. b. John has a notion of Mary. c. ?Mary has a notion of John. 80 (2) a. 0 Gianis fovate tin Maria. John fears Mary. b. John has a notion of Mary. c. ?Mary has a notion of John. The conclusion that Kordoni (2000) reaches is the subject NPs of ESPVCs have a notion of the entity denoted by the object verb according to Wechsler's Notion Rule. The non cognitive participants of MG ESPVCs are syntactically realized as the object NP. Dowty in his proto Agent and Patient property analyses also claimed that the experienced (stimulus) argument of ESPVCs has neither proto Agent nor proto Patient properties~ therefore he claimed that they are semantically underspecified. In this way Dowty's and Kordoni's approaches are parallel to each other. Grimshaw (1990) has also claimed that accusative Experiencer- Object psych verb constructions have two different readings; agentive and psychological. Agentive reading is possible when the nominative argument is animate. This animate argument is interpreted as the deliberate and volitional Agent who causes the object Experiencer to experience the mentioned emotional state. With this reading the accusative Experiencer can not be doubled. In Greek, in such a sentence like (3): (3) Giannis upsets Mary. Giannis knows Mary (he has a notion of Mary), and Mary also has a notion of Giannis i.e. the individual denoted by the subject NP has a notion of the individual denoted by the object NP and vice versa. When there is a causative reading the clitic on accusative Experiencer is doubled obligatorily. The interpretation is there is something about Giannis that causes 81 Mary to experience an emotional state. This sort of constructions is similar to what Grimshaw calls causative psych verb constructions as in (2): (4) The thunderstorm frightened the child. In both of these constructions Giannis and the thunderstorm do not have any notion of the argument experiencing the mental state i.e. Mary and the child are neither volitional nor intentional. They also do not participate in the mental state, they just cause it. Thus, in these constructions only one of the participants is cognitive and the other participant is just the Causer. Therefore, these constructions can be regarded as normal causative constructions. Grimshaw claims that ES psych predicates have their external arguments and therefore they can undergo passivization. Kordoni (2001) gives counter examples for this claim from MG ESPVCs. While (5) and (7) are grammatical in MG, (6) and (8) are not: (5) His parents love John. (6)* John is loved by his parents. (7) Mary envied the neighbor's house. (8) *The neighbor's house was envied by Mary. He concludes that MG ESPVCs are neither passivized nor they are the passive forms of the corresponding EOPVCs. The experienced (stimulus) argument of them are realized either as the object ofthe sentence or the complement of a PP. The accusative NP does not behave like a normal object since it can not be passivized. 82 Dabrowska (1996) on the Dative and Nominative I. 2. 2. 12. Experiencers in Polish Dabrowska, (1994) in the analysis of the case marking of Experiencer predicates in Polish argues that mental experiences have two aspects whose properties can be summarized in the following table: objective conscious public active . . . subjective irrational personal passive ... Table 7. Dabrowska (1994) The two Aspects of Mental Experiences Dabrowska claims that we have a tendency to identify ourselves with the first aspect of mind rather than the second. Therefore, we tend to conceptualize ourselves as active, conscious and rational etc. rather than passive, unconscious and irrational with the effect of cultural values. Dabrowska observes that some verbs in Polish have the Experiencer arguments either in nominative or in dative and there are also some verbs which allow both cases. She claims that semantic motivations behind the choice of case marking can be defined with reference to the concept of "sphere of awareness". Some of the basic characteristics which can be associated with these cases in her analysis can be summarized as follows: Is defmite, concrete and active. Is with a participant who has Agentive properties Foregrounds the Experiencer's active role. Backgrounds the sphere of awareness. Is used with predicates which refer to a definite desire, intention, determination or resolution. Emphasizes volitionality and control. Involves effort and action. Table 8. The participant is affected by a state or process occurring in his personal sphere. Foregrounds the sphere of awareness. Depicts the Experiencer as passive. Portrays the Experiencer as the passive receiver of impulses. Describes physiological drives and cravings which come from the instinct. Describes effortless, unplanned, can't help situations. Focuses on the irmer experiences of he Experiencer. Dabrowska (1994) Nominative and Dative Experiencers 83 She accepts that these semantic properties do no not make all of the grammatical properties predictable. She just claims that grammatical forms are motivated by them. For her, the choice between the two cases" ... has the effect of highlighting some aspects of the situation and hiding others" (1994:1047). 84 II. ANALYSIS OF THE PSYCH VERBS IN TURKISH II. 1. What is Psych? This section presents a brief introduction to psych verbs in Turkish to clarify the basic assumptions of the study with an initial analysis of some semantic and morphological aspects of these verbs. In the realm of psych verbs, there is a multitude of terms like mental, cognitive and emotional. The first two terms are commonly used interchangeably to refer to verbs connected with conscious mental processes such as to know, to learn and to understand. The term emotional on the other hand is a quality of verbs connected with feelings such as love, like, hate, fear etc. In some studies we see the verbs of one group under the title of the other group or sometimes it is the case that these verbs are altogether regarded as psych. In fact it is not easy to distinguish between the two since mental and emotional domains seem to be interconnected in our daily lives too. Some activities have emotional bases as a point of initiation as opposed to other activities which have mental bases. Accordingly, some of the verbs that we examined in Turkish belong to mental domain such as algzla-, anzmsa-, anlamlandzr-, ayrzmla-, belle-, bilin9len-, bil-, degerlendir-, dii,Jiin-, kap-, kavra-, kzvzr-, ogren-, yor- while some of them unquestionably correspond to the emotional domain such as acz-, arzula-, bayzl-, begen-, bunal-, 9zldzr-, giiven-, igren-, kaygdan-, kork-, onemse-, iimitlen-, iiz-, zevklen- and so on. There are some other verbs which can be considered either as mental or emotional. Although it is almost for sure that a verb like kavra- belongs to the mental 85 domain and a verb like begen- belongs to the emotional domain, a verb like ilgilen- can either be mental (for example if you are interested in a subject or an idea) or emotional (for example if you are interested in a boy or a girl to mean to be attracted to). It is again hard to say whether benimse- "to adopt" occurs in the emotional or the mental domain. Sa~zr­ "to be surprised" seems to belong to the mental domain if taken as an act of being surprised for something different from your existing thoughts related to this thing. If this act of being surprised is for something that is different from what you emotionally expected then the verb belongs to the emotional domain. The analysis of such verbs which can be regarded as fuzzy cases shows that some of them are semantically closer to the mental domain such as afalla-, aldan-, apl$-, ayart-, kan-, kur-, ku~kulan-, ~ah u9-, yargzla-, zannet- and some of them are semantically closer to the emotional domain such as avun-, bagz~la-, benimse-, ilgilen-, onemse- . The mental and the emotional domains, first, as we stated are indistinguishable and have some intersections, and second, verbs belonging to emotional and mental domains have structural correspondences rather than differences. Therefore we have enough reasons to regard both mental and emotional verbs as psych. None of the studies in the psych verb literature offers a clear cut definition or at least attempt to define what a psych verb is. In their appendices there are neither lists of verbs that they accepted as psych nor a set of criteria specifying the features that these verbs possess or do not possess but it seems that a kind of consensus exists as to what they are. Therefore, before everything, we need to define what a psych verb is or at least what we take it to be in this study. In order to be a psych verb, a verb should: 86 1. be semantically related to either the emotional or the mental domains i.~. it should describe an event occurring in one of these domains. 2. have an animate Experiencer argument who expenences the mentaVemotional state. 3. lead to a change in the psychological state of the Experiencer. II. 1. 1. The Experiencer Argument It is obligatory for a psych verb to have an Experiencer argument. Semantically, this argument experiences the men:taVemotional state that the verb describes. This argument is regarded by Kural (1996) as an instance of the beneficiary argument. The discussions on psych verbs are centered mostly on the structural positions of the thematic arguments Experiencer and the Theme. The syntactic position of the Experiencer argument is either the subject (mostly the subject) or the object position as it is the case in Turkish: (1) Ali Ay~e'yi seviyor. Experiencer (2) Ali Ayse'yi tiztiyor. Experiencer The surface case marking properties of the Experiencer argument may vary according to the structural position that it occupies; it is nominative marked in the subject position and accusative marked in the object position. The Experiencer argument is almost always an animate being. It is hard to think of any situations that an inanimate being experiences one of the mentaVemotional states like aldzr-, r;eldn-, dan!-, duygulan-, hzrslan-, hisset-, hiiziinlen-, kaygzlan-, kopiir-, kiis-, onemse-, pipiriklen-, sez-, szkzl-, iimitlen-, yadzrga-. 87 Therefore the emotional domain is one of the unique belongings of human being and it is at least at that moment, not realistic to think of an inanimate being whose psychological state changes or which understands implied meanings etc. Moreover, even in such cases where the subject is not a nominal referring to an inanimate being, we tend to interpret it as animate. II. 1. 2. The Theme Argument Semantically the Theme role is the most neutral or inactive thematic role among the other thematic roles, although it does not seem to be equally inactive in the following examples (1) and (2) with psych verbs: (1) The thunder frightened the baby. Theme Experiencer (2) The children frightened the baby. Theme Experiencer (3) Ali talked to Ayse. Agent Theme Animacy is also an important quality that affects the degree of activeness but the point of discussion should be whether to name the argument other than the Experiencer as Theme in dyadic psych verbs or not. In (2) the nature of the Theme argument is different from the Theme argument of (3). In (2) the Theme is semantically the cause of the fear of the baby. Therefore this type of psych verbs are regarded as different from the Experiencer Subject type psych verbs in having a Causer argument. Pesetsky (1996) decomposes the Theme role into two as the Target of Emotion and the Subject Matter which is an essential distinction to differentiate between the Theme arguments of the Experiencer subject and the Experiencer object verbs. 88 In Turkish, the Theme argument, parallel with the Experiencer argument is either in the subject or in the object position. The surface case marking properties of the Theme argument vary according to the structural position that it occupies; it is nominative in the subject position and either accusative or oblique case marked in the object position. II. 1. 3. The Input of Psych Verbalization This section gives a brief morphological analysis of psych verbs in Turkish. Morphologically, psych verbs in Turkish are of three types: 1. non-derived simple base psych verbs 2. derived psych verbs a. from nouns b. from verbs c. from adjectives d. compound frozen forms 3. compound psych verbs The majority of psych verbs are non- derived simple base verbs (the etymological analysis of each of which is not the concern here) such as avun-, az-, bzk-, bil-, co~-, dii~iin-, igren-, imren-, inan-, kzskan-, kzz-, kork-, ~a~-, iirk-, iiz- etc. In the second group, there are derived psych verbs. The psych verbs derived from nouns constitute the biggest group. The majority of them are derived nouns with -/An. Most of the time these verbs are intransitive: Psych Noun+ -/An= Psych Verb PSYCH NOUN, ' . ac1 gurur -Ian -Ian acllan- "to grieve" gururlan- "to be arrogant" from psych 89 h1rs hiddet h\iziin ilgi keyif kibir zevk -Ian -len -len -len -len -len -len Table I. h1rslan- "to get angry" hiddetlen- "to get furious" hiizi.inlen- "to become sad" ilgilen- "to be interested" keyiflen- "to get into a good mood" kibirlen- "to become arrogant" zevklen- "to take pleasure" Psych Noun+ -!An Psych Verb Some psych verbs are derived from nouns with -!A. However, this time the input noun can be either a psych or a non- psych noun: Psych/ Non-Psych Noun+ -lA= Psych Verb PSYCH/NONPSYCH NOUN arzu biiyii etki ip yabanc1 yarg1 Table 2. -lA PSYCH VERB -la -le -le -le -la -la arzula- "to wish for" biiyiile- "to charm" etkile- "to influence" iple- "to mind" yabancila- "to fmd smth. strange" yarg1la- "to judge" Psych/ Non-Psych Noun+ -lA- Psych Verb The third group is the psych verbs derived from verbs. The base verb is either a psych verb as in anla-m-lan-dzr-, co~-ku-lan-, kzz-z~-. san-gz-la-, ~a~-zr- or not a psych verb as in al-dzr-, bur-uk-Iah 9ek-in-, duy-um-sa-, kap-tzr-, oyna-t-. Psych verbs are derived by five means from base verbs. The first way is first to derive a psych noun (which mostly refers to a perception or at least implies it) with one of the following morphemes and then to derive the psych verb from these nouns mostly either with -lA or -!An: Verb-> Psych Noun-> Psych Verb INPUTV al bil co~ duy duy es kur kur san -in9 -ku -gu -urn -m -gu -untu -g1 Table 3. -len -Ian -Ian -sa -len -la -Ian -la bilin9len- "to become conscious" co~kulan- "to get excited" duygulan- "to be affected" duyumsa- "to feel" esinlen- "to be inspired" kurgula- "imagine" kuruntulan- "to worry for no reason" sangila- "to suppose" Verb-> Psych Noun-> Psych Verb 90 The second way is first to derive a psych adjective (all of which are the modifiers of the Experiencer argument) mostly with -Gin and then to derive the psych verb either with -iAn or -lA~: Verb -> Psych Adjective -> Psych Verb The third way to derive psych verbs from base verbs is to use the causative morpheme -(D)Ir, or -t: Verb+ Seemingly Causative Morpheme= Psych Verb INPUTV CAUSATIVE \1\IQR.j)JiE.~ -d1r al kap -tlr -1r -tir kes oyna -t -ir -ur u~ Table 5. kapt1r- "to concentrate" ka~1r- "to lose one's mind" kestir- "to estimate" oynat- "to become insane" ~i~ir- "to embarrass" u~ur- "to lose one's mind" Verb+ Causative Morpheme= Psych Verb The fourth way to derive psych verbs from base verbs is to use the passive morpheme -II deriving a reflexive sense: Verb+ Seemingly Passive Morpheme= Psych Verb 91 Lastly, the following morphemes; reciprocal -1~ and reflexive -In also derive psych verbs from base verbs: Verb+ -In/ -Ip Psych Verb When we examine the sense relation between the base and the derived verb, the question of whether the causative, passive, reflexive and reciprocal morphemes add their original meaning to the input stem or they are just homophonous morphemes arises. Further data is going to show that most of them are seemingly causativization or passivization applications. Therefore, the mentioned suffixes can be regarded as derivational suffixes without any voice effect. In the fourth group there are psych verbs derived from adjectives which do not have any other verbal counterparts, so, there is no other way of verbalizing them. Some of these verbs are derived mostly from borrowed stative psych adjectives with -IA.r: Psych Adjective+ -lA$= Psych Verb PSYCH ADJECTIVE ahmak ahk aptal -lA~ PSYCH VERB -Ia~ ahmakl~- -Ia~ ahkl~- ~Ilgm -I~ -Ia~ hm;m ifrit sakin salak tedirgin uysal "to tum into a fool" "to be astounded" aptalla~- "to become stupid" ~IIgml~- "to become mad" h1r~ml~- "to get ill tempered" ifritle~- "to get angry" sakinle~- "to get calm" salakla~- "to become stupid" tedirginle~- "to become uneasy" uysalla~- "to become complaisant" -Ia~ -le~ -le~ -Ia~ -le~ -Ia~ Table 8. Psych AdJeCtiVe+ lA~ - Psych Verb 92 Some of the verbs derived from adjectives are derived with the morpheme -!An but the input adjective may not be a psych adjective as in the last one: Psych/ Non-Psych Adjective+ -/An= Psych Verb The difference between -!An and -lA~ verbs is that the former may occur with an optional dative marked argument although the latter are truly intransitive (see Kural (1996) for unaccusative vs. unergative distinction). Some of these verbs are derived with -sA and the rest are the change of state verbs derived with -Ar, -AI, or -An: Psych/ Non-Psych Adjective+ -sA= Psych Verb Non-Psych Adjective+ -Arl -AI= Psych Verb Lastly there are some psych verbs which are derived from non native compound bases. These verbs are frozen forms therefore the helping verb and the noun component are merged into one verb form as in hazzet-, hisset-, kahrol-. 93 To conclude, there are 13 means for the derivation of psych verbs in Turkish which we can summarize in the following formulas: 1. Non-Derived Base-> Psych Verb 2. Psych Noun+ -/An= Psych Verb 3. Psych/ Non-Psych Noun+ -/A= Psych Verb 4. Verb-> Psych Noun-> Psych Verb 5. Verb-> Psych Adjective-> Psych Verb 6. Verb+ Causative Morpheme= Psych Verb 7. Verb+ Passive Morpheme= Psych Verb 8. Verb+ ReciprocaV Reflexive Morpheme= Psych Verb 9. Psych Adjective+ -lA$= Psych Verb 10. Psych/ Non-Psych Adjective+ -!An= Psych Verb 11. Psych/ Non-Psych Adjective+ -sA= Psych Verb 12. Non-Psych Adjective+ -Ari-A!= Psych Verb 13. Compound Base -> Psych Verb II. 1. 4. Secondary Psych Senses The analysis has shown that the majority of the psych verbs have psych senses as their primary meaning which we can refer to as true psych verbs which include igren-, imren-, ho$lan-, kork-, ozen-, sinirlen-, §G§-, tiksin-, yzl-. 94 Among the verbs in our data, there are no psych verbs that ca.n be secondarily used in different senses other than their psych senses. On the other hand, psych meanings of some verbs are their secondary meanings. Semantically these verbs primarily belong to other verb classes but extensions of meaning secondarily make them psych verbs. The following are some examples: II. 2. Psych Verb Types in Turkish As discussed in previous chapters psych verbs are problematic for linking/mapping theories proposed for different languages in the literature. The crosslinguistic tendencies show that the unexpected mapping of these verbs is universal. Such differences in their syntactic and semantic behavior lead linguists define distinct classes of psych verbs on different bases. Belletti& Rizzi (1988) focused on different surface markings of thematic roles. Levin (1993) classified these verbs with respect to transitivity 95 alternations. Grimshaw (1990) and Tenny (1994) have used aspectual criteria in order to explain the different syntactic behavior that these verbs display. With the aim of providing a partial description related to their semantic and syntactic properties, first, we will propose a classification for Turkish psych verbs. Our claim is that there are four types of psych verbs in Turkish when we consider the syntactic position and the surface case markings of thematic arguments. The following configurations are based commonly on accepted thematic roles the Experiencer and the Theme. In the first group there is an Experiencer argument in the subject position. As expected, this argument is nominative case marked. In the object position there is a Theme argument and again as expected, this argument is accusative case marked just like ordinary Agent Patient verbs. If we reverse the thematic roles of the first group, we get the second group. These are the verbs whose subjects are nominative case marked Themes and objects are accusative case marked Experiencers. The third group consists of verbs which have a nominative case marked Experiencer in the subject position and a Theme argument in the object position just like the first group. The difference is that the Theme argument here is not accusative but oblique case marked. The verbs of the fourth group are different from the other three groups in that they do not have a Theme argument but just a nominative case marked Experiencer argument in the subject position. From now on, we are going to refer to verbs of these four classes as Type 1, Type 2, Type 3(a/ b) and Type 4 psych verbs in Turkish as the following table illustrates: 96 The following sections analyze each of these types according to their input of verbalization, surface case marking and the syntactic distribution of their non sentential arguments. II. 2. 1. Type 1 Psych Verbs The verbs in this group correspond to commonly called fear typepsyqh verbs of Grimshaw (1990) and Tenny (1994) in English, and temere class ofBelletti& Rizzi (1988) in Italian. The mapping of the thematic roles and syntactic positions do not pose any problems for linking theories since they differ in no way from ordinary transitive dyadic predicates. The Experiencer is in the subject position and the Theme is in the object position. The surface case marking of arguments also corresponds to what is expected from a transitive dyadic predicate; a nominative subject and an accusative object: (1) Ben bu konuyu anladtm. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ACC) I understood this subject. (2) Ay§e bu i§i yok arzuluyor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e wishes for this job very much. 97 (3) Ane Ali'yi begeniyor. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e likes Theme (ACC) Ali. (4) Ay$e Ali'yi istiyor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e wants Ali. (5) Ali kansm1 k1skamyor. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali envies his wife. (6) bgretmen ogrencileri ki.iyi.imsi.iyor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) The teacher looks down on the students. (7) Cocuk annesini ozli.iyor. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ACC) The child misses his mother. (8) Kadm bu adam1 seviyor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) The woman loves this man. (9) Ali ailesini umursam1yor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ali does not care about his family. (10) Ya$hlar genvleri 90k yad1rg1yor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Old people consider the young people strange. The input for the Type 1 psych verbalization is of five types: 1. a non-derived/ simple base: begen-, belle-, bil-, dile-, iste- ... 2. a verb: an-zm-sa-, anla-m-lan-dzr-, kes-tir-, kur-gu-la-, um-ur-sa- ... 3. a noun: arzu-la-, hesap-la-, ip-le-, onem-se--, yargz-la- ... 4. an adjective: agzr-sa-, garip-se-, hafifse- ... 5. a compound base: aff-et-, var-say-, zan-net- ... 98 A Type 1 psych verb can semantically be either mental as in algzla-, ammsa-, kap-, kavra-, ogren- or emotional as in begen, kzskan, ozle, sev, yadzrga. II. 2. 2. Type 2 Psych Verbs These verbs which correspond to frighten type psych verbs of Grimshaw (1990) and Tenny (1994) in English, and preoccupare class of Belletti& Rizzi (1988) in Italian are the problematic group for the linking theories since they map the Experiencer role to the object position and the Theme role to the subject position. Therefore the Theme is nominative and the Experiencer is accusative marked. That Type 2 psych verbs in Turkish map their Experiencer and Theme roles in the same way with the other analyzed languages shows this unexpected behavior to be a universal one. Moreover, in Turkish, (as it is the case with most of the other languages) these verbs are limited in number. Some of the verbs in the following examples require a form of support to emphasize the psych sense such as a degree adverbial (9ok, fena etc.) or an instrumental PP. These verbs are either secondary psych uses such as bog-, boz-, biiyiile-, kzr-, sars- as in p), (4), (5), (8), (9) or a non native compound cezbet- as in (6) but not true lexical psych verbs like iiz- in {10). This shows that most of the Type 2 verbs have somewhat a derived psych sense. (1) Ali Ayseyi ayartti. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali seduced Ay~e. (2) Konu§mact dinleyicileri bayd1. Theme (NOM) Experiencer (ACC) The speaker bored the audience. (3) Ane Ali'yi ticy ytldu ktskancyhgt ile bogdu. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ane made Ali fed up with her jealousy for three years. 99 (4) Ali Ay~e'yi fena bozdu. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali embarrassed Ay~e badly. (5) Kadm adarm tam~t1klan ilk saniyede biiyiiledi. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) The woman charmed the man the ftrst second that they met. (6) Ay~e Ali'yi sakinligi ile cezbetti. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e (7) charmed Ali with her calmness. Ay~e Ali 'yi 90k etkiledi. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e influenced Ali very much. (8) Babam beni 90k k1rd1. Theme (NOM) Experienccr (ACC) My father hurt me very much. (9) Ay§e Ali'yi gidi~iyle sarst1. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e (10) gave Ali a shock with her going. Ay~e Ali'yi uzdu. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e made Ali upset. According to their input of verbalization, similar to Type 1, there are five sources for this process: 1. a non-derived/ simple base: boz-, kzr-, sars-, iiz- ... 2. a verb: ayar-t-, kz~kzr-t- ... 3. a noun: biiyii-le-, etki-le- ... 4. an adjective: ergin-/e-. 5. a compound base: cezb-et-. These verbs too, can be either mental as in aydmlat-, sma- or emotional as in bay-, bog-, boz-, biiyiile-, etkile-. 100 II. -2~3~ - _ Type 3 Psych Verbs ... . ' These are 'intransitive verbs. Similar to Type 1 verbs they have an Experiencer argument as the subjeCt bu~ they have a Theme argument as the indirect object. What is different is the oblique case marki~g of the Theme argument. We can regroup the verbs of Type 3 with respect to their case marking in the following way: 1. Type 3a: For Type 3 verbs with dative marked Themes which are the greatest in number. 2. Type 3b: For Type 3 verbs with ablative marked Themes which are less great in number than Type 3 a. II. 2. 3. 1. Type 3a Psych Verbs These verbs have dative marked Themes. The dative Theme semantically is a kind of goal or cause of the mentioned mental or emotional state. These verbs optionally allow a source or a secondary cause argument: (1) Ane Ali'ye ac1d1. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Ay~e felt pity for Ali. (2) Ay§e Ali 'ye danld1. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Ane put out with Ali. (3) Ali Ay!?e'ye yok giiveniyor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Ali trusts Ay~e very much. (4) Ay§e arkada~ma imreniyor. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e Theme (OAT) longs for her friend. 101 (5) Ay~e Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e kadere inamyor. Theme (OAT) believes the faith. (6) Ali yalamm1za kand1. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (OAT) Ali was fooled with our lie. (7) Ali bize kmld1. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Ali was offended at us. (8) Ali bize k1zd1. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Ali got angry with us. (9) Ali Ay~e'ye kiistii. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Ali put out with (10) Ay~e. Genc;ler ~imdi Tarkan gibi pop ylld1zlanna ozeniyor. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (OAT) Young people consider popstars like Tarkan as a model nowadays. Mostly, the verbs of Type 3a are emotional as in begen-, giicen-, heveslen-, inan-, kzrzl-, tutu!-, vurul- however; there are mental verbs too such as aldan-, kan-, ~a$zr-. When we look at the inputs of the verbalization process we see that the five sources for the other two groups are valid for Type 3a verbs too: 1. a non-derived/ simple base: dal-, imren-, inan-, kan-, kzz-, kiis-, ozen-, yiiksiin- ... 2. a verb: ai-m-, bay-zl-, boz-ul-, kap-tzr-, /ar-zl-, tut-u!- ... 3. a noun: cezbe-len-, dert-len-, ejkar-lan-, hiiziin-len-, odak-lan-, ofke-len-, iimit-len- ... 4. an adjective: aksi-len-, deli-len-, dik-len-, ters-len-, ye$il-len-, zor-sun- ... 5. a compound base: kahr-ol- and sabr-et-. 102 II. 2. 3. 2. Type 3b Psych Verbs These verbs have ablative marked Themes which may be iiitetptefed as lhe source or the cause of the mental or emotional state. ( 1) Adam hakaretlerden bezmi~ti. The~ Experiencer (NOM) (ABL) The man was fed up with the insults. (2) Cocuklar sebze yemeginden b1kt1. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) The children were fed up with vegetable meals. (3) Ali kalabahktan yekindi. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) Ali was ashamed of the crowd. (4) Ane Ali ' den ho~lamyor. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e Theme (ABL) likes Ali. (5) Herkes bu sualar depremden korkuyor. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) Everyone fears the earthquake nowadays. (6) Polis adamm kansmdan ku~kulamyor. The~ Experiencer (NOM) (ABL) The police are suspected of the man's wife. (7) Ogrenciler bu dersten sogumu~tu. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) The students have lost their interest in this course. (8) Ali bahktan tiksinir. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) Ali feels disgust for the fish. (9) Adam kimseden utanm1yor. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) The man is not ashamed of anyone. (10) Ay$e riizgar sesinden bile urkuyordu. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) Ane was scared even of the sound of the wind. 103 Mostly these verbs are emotional such as bzk-, bunal-, ho~lan-, igren-, soguthough they may be mental in a lesser number such as ku~kulan-, ~iiphelen-. These verbs allow only animate beings to be their subjects as m bunal-, kaygzlan-, utan-, iirk-, yzl- but not inanimate ones. The mentioned five inputs are also valid for this class: 1. a non-derived/ simple base: bzk-, bunal-, kork-, tiksin-, usan- ... 2. a verb: t;ek-in-, es-in-len-, szk-zl-, um-ut-lan- ... 3. a noun: huy-lan-, kaygz-lan-, 4. an adjective: dar-a/-, ku~ku-lan-, ~iiphe-len- ho~-lan-, ho~-la~- ... ... 5. a compound base: haz-z-et-. Nilsson ( 1985) uses the term predicates of "mental reaction" for the psych verbs and analyzes them as two groups; one requiring the ablative and the other allative (dative) case. In her analysis, the first group consists of verbs like bzk-, usan-, sogu-, nefret et- and the second group consists of verbs like bayzl-, day- etc. She claims that verbs of mental reaction which take the allative case like dan/-, giiven-, inan- have an object which can be qualified as an abstract goal. These verbs describe an attitude ofX towards Y. The other group which take the ablative like duygulan-, umutlan-, yoru/describe a certain reaction of X due to Y and therefore, the object can be qualified as an abstract source or a point of reference. Nilsson puts forward that there are some other verbs which allow both the allative and the ablative cases to be their objects including dertlen-, emin ol-, giicen-, incin- , kaygzlan-, memnun ol-, razz of-, utan-. 104 Nilsson observes that this choice between the two cases is sensitive to the temporal properties. For her (1) expresses a single future event whereas {2) expresses an enduring certainty (1985:115): (1) Derhal dedigimi yapacagma eminim. (OAT) I am sure that he will immediately do as I say. (2) Derhal toparlanacag1mdan eminim. (ABL) I was sure that I would immediately pull myself together. In such cases where the ablative and the allative cases seem to be the alternatives of each other, Nilsson observes the following differences (1985: 116): 1. Ablative has a wider field of application than the allative (as (3) shows). 2. Ablative relates to continuous events or repeated instances 3. Ablative relates to events whose time component is quite indefinite, therefore, there is a less specified! more diversified relation. 4. Allative refers to a more specified relation. 5. Allative notes afuture action (as (4) and (5) show). 6. Ablative notes either a factual or a potential action (as (6) and (7) show). (3) Ben arkada~1mdan eminim. (ABL) I feel certain about my friend. *Ben arkada~1ma eminim. (OAT) (4) Yanm1za girmeye utan1yor. (OAT) He is bashful to come up to you. (5) Sokaga c1kmaya korkuyor. (OAT) He is afraid to go out. (6) Utamyorum istemekten ama ... {ABL) It embarrasses me to ask for but ... (7) Gelmeyi di.i~i.inmekten (ABL) bile korkuyordu. 105 Even the thought of coming scared him. This alternative choice between the two case markings of the same verb is not just a feature of mental predicates. Nilsson gives examples from other groups of verbs which allow for the same choice (1985: 117). (8) a. Karde~im izmir' de yerle~ti. (ABL) b. Karde~im izmir'e yerle~ti. (DAT) Nilsson (1985) sees this freedom in the choice of case marking as a language specific phenomenon and argues that this may be independent of lexical meaning but it is rather related to the interpretation of the context and the "speaker's perception of the relationship between the entities" (117). In our analysis of Type 3a and 3b psych verbs, we have seen that both of these case markings may imply a kind of cause ofthe mentaV emotional state of the Experiencer. This is the reason why we added the notion of cause as a meaning component of both the dative and the ablative marked arguments. According to our analysis, in fact, the following examples may entail different semantic interpretations with ablative and dative: (9) Ali yurtd1~ma gitmeye korkuyor. (NOM) (1 0) (DAT) Ali yurtd1~ma gitmekten korkuyor. (NOM) (ABL) In (10) Ali has gone to abroad before, has experienced something there and his fear is related to the result ofthe activity that he already knows. However, in (9), this is the first time that he will go to abroad and his fear is not related to the result (which is unknown at that moment) but related to the event itself which may have potential (guessed, expected or heard) unwanted results to be experienced. 106 There are two more intransitive psych verbs but they are not grammatically significant to form a type on their own: There is only one verb in our data that requires for a locative marked Theme argument; yam!-. This is a mental verb whose input is a non-derived/ simple base: (1) Ay$e bu konuda 90k yanthyor. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (LOC) Ane was mistaken about this point. There are seven verbs which have instrumental case marked Themes. All of these verbs are semantically emotional type and all require for animate subjects. (1) Ay$e eski hatualarla avunuyor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (INST) Ay~e consoles herself with the old memories. (2) Adam oglunun ba$ar1Slyla gururlantyordu. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (INST) The man was proud of the success of his son. (3) Ayse Ali ile ilgileniyor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (INST) Ay~e is attracted to Ali. (4) Ktz gUzelligi ile kibirleniyordu. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (INST) The girl was arrogant of her beauty. One of the verbs in this class is derived from a simple base ; avun-, one of them is derived from a verb; ~i~(in)-, four verbs are derived from nouns; alaka(lan)-, gurur(lan)- , ilgi(len)-, kibir(len)- and only one verb is derived from an adjective; ~en(len)-. 107 II. 2. 4. Type 4 Psych Verbs These verbs are distinct from the other intransitive verbs of Type 3 since they do not have a Theme argument. Therefore the only argument that they have is a nominative Experiencer which are mostly expressed in passive in English. This group of psych verbs does not correspond to any of the defined psych verb classes in other languages. Therefore it seems to exist only in Turkish. (1) Ay~e afallad1. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e was bewildered. (2) Kalabahk co~tu. Experiencer (NOM) The crowd cheered. (3) Ay~e son gtinlerde dalgtla~tL Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e became lost in thoughts recently. (4) Dedem ya~land1k9a duruldu. Experiencer (NOM) My grandfather calmed down as he got old. (5) Ben bu ortamda 90k geri·liyorum. Experiencer (NOM) I am tensed up very much in this atmosphere. (6) Bebek huysuzl~t1. Experiencer (NOM) The baby got uncomfortable. (7) Baham 9abuk parlar. Experiencer (NOM) My father gets furious quickly. (8) Bu ktz iyice u9urdu. Experiencer (NOM) This girl really went insane. 108 (9) Kadm bir ttirlii yatt~mtyordu. Experiencer (NOM) The woman could not be calmed down in any way. Annem dfule oranla biraz yumu~adt. (1 0) Experiencer (NOM) My mother became a little more tolerant compared to yesterday. Only a few number of these verbs are mental such as salakla~- as well as alzkla~-. are emotional such as ahmakla~-. aptalla~-. bilin9len-, tozut-, u9-. u9ur- etc. The rest of the Type 4 verbs co~ku/an-, co~-. kudur-, par/a-, uysalla~-. iirkek/e~- etc. The four inputs for the verbalization are as follows: 1. a non-derived base: afalla-, apz~-. az-, bat-, bit-, co~-. 90k-, dol-, sin ... 2. a verb: These verbs are mostly derived with -/An or -/A~ either from nouns such as co~-ku-lan-, bil-in9-len- or from adjectives such as ~a~-km-la~-. dur-gun-la~-. kasz-n/ /-, siiz-iil-, yat-z~- co~-kun-la~-. There are also other verbs like dur-ul-, ger-il-, which directly derive from verbs with seemingly voice suffixes. 3. a noun: These verbs are derived from nouns with -/An such as akli-lan-, dioi-len-, gam-ian-, htrs-lan-, his-len- ... 4. an adjective: These verbs are derived from adjectives with ahmak-laoi-. ftlgm-la§-, hzr9m-/a§-, sakin-le§-, tedirgin-le~- -/A~ such as ... As the above analysis shows there are four basic types of psych verbs with similar derivational patterns in Turkish. The first and the second type are transitive and the others are intransitive; one with a dative, one with an ablative and one with a null Theme argument. 109 II. 3. Voice and Psych Verbs in Turkish Although there are some common logical constraints for voice, there are no universal structures which are uniform among different languages. Voice in Turkish changes the surface case marking of arguments. Traditional grammar books define voice as a property of verbs with respect to their subjects and objects. Thus, voice is analyzed from two different points of view. According to their subjects there are four types of voices and four types of verbs which belong to these types; active, passive, reflexive and reciprocal. According to their objects, verbs are of two types which are transitives and intransitives: . VOICE IN TURKISH According to the properties of the subject According to the properti~s of ~Jle objec~ . Transitive Intransitive Active Passive Reflexive Reciprocal Table I. Votce m Turktsh Voice suffixes in Turkish can also function as derivational suffixes and this sometimes causes confusions. The very often .confusion is among the passive and the reflexive voice markers. This is sometimes regarded as a result of the fact that one suffix has the mentioned both functions in the old Turkish. In some cases only the phonological conditions determine the attachment ofthe suffixes. Tietze (1989) argues that there is an ongoing merge of passive and the reflexive forms in Turkish i.e. some verbs which have the reflexive marking have a passive meaning and vice versa. Therefore overt morphological marking does not always correspond to the semantic functions. His basic claim is that all of the verbs with the morpheme -11 were passive in their origin and some of them acquired a reflexive meaning thereafter. The following psych verbs from his list have the passive morpheme -11 but a "medioreflexive" 110 meaning (1989:287). Nakipoglu (1988) regards them as reflexives. These verbs do not have -(I)n forms; bogul-, bozul-, ezil-, siiziil-, iiziil-, vurul-, yzkzl-.. In the other group in his list, there are verbs possessing the two forms (some of which have psych meanings): II. 3. 1. Psych Verbs and Transitivity in Turkish According to Taylan (nd.), Turkish psych verbs constitute a separate group of intransitive verbs. She claims that psychological verbs are the only intransitive verbs which take complements: Transitive v~rbs + + Subject Direct object complement Other type of complement Adjunct + Table I. ,., > Intransitive verbs .. '. ,, ·' -~ ",, Psychological verbs + Other Intransitive verbs + - - + + .. + Psych Verbs and Other lntraosrtlves In our analysis, of the four types of psych verbs in Turkish, only the Type 1 and Type 2 are transitive and the rest are intransitive: TYPE . 1 2 + + + + + 3a 3b 4 Table 2. Transitivity in Turkish Psych Verbs 111 It is possible in Turkish optionally to delete the arguments of Type 3a and 43b whenever the discoursal factors allow. The following verbs of Type 3a for example allow the deletion of their dative marked Themes requiring mostly (but not always) an adverbial support (iyice, fena etc.): These verbs of Type 3a are most of the time derived with either -/An or -/A~: TYPE NOUN/ ADJECTIVE+- With:(DAT) ()p#()nal.!).~letiqn Ay~e Ay~e duygulandt. (Ayse was affected) of (DAT) (lAn)/-(lA~) 3a duy-gu-lan- filme duygulandt. (Ayse was affected by the film.) 3a 3a 3a huy-suz-lansev-da-lanaksi-len- Bebek gUrilltilye huysuzlandt. Bebek huysuzlandt. (The baby became uncomfortable because of the noise.) Ali Ay~e'ye sevdalandt. (Ali felt in love with Ane.) (The baby became uncomfortable.) Dede torunlanna aksilendi. (The grandfather grandchildren.) 3a 3a 3a 3a 3a deli-lendik-le- became obstinate Ali sevdalandt. (Ali felt in love.) Dede aksilendi. to his (The grandfather became obstinate.) Kocas1 bu sozlere delilendi. Kocas1 delilendi. (Her husband went insane for these words.) (Her husband went insane.) Adam patronuna diklendi. Adam diklendi. (The man got stubborn to his boss.) (The man got stubborn.) heves-len- <;ocuklar tatile heveslendi. <;ocuklar heveslendi. (The children became desirous.) hiiziin-len- (The children desired for the holiday.) Ay~e bu ~ark1lara hUzOnleniyor. (Ane becomes sad because of these songs.) Ay~e Annem mutlulugumuza keyiflendi. Annem keyiflendi. keyif-len- 3a ters-len- 3a duy-ar-siz-la~- hilzOnleniyor. (Ane becomes sad.) (My mother took pleasure of our happiness.) (My mother took pleasure.) Ali babasma terslendi. Ali terslendi. (Ali got befouled to his father.) (Ali got befouled.) <;ocuk hakaretlere duyars~zl~tt. <;ocuk duyars~zl~tJ. (The child was desensitized with the insults.) (The child was desensitized.) . . Table 4. Opttonal Argument Deletion of Denved Type 3a Verbs There are also verbs of Type 3b which allow the optional deletion of their ablative Themes: TYPE VERB 3b endi~e-len- Ailesi hastahgmdan endi~eleniyor. (Her family worries about her illness.) 3b gocun- Ay~e sozlerimden gocundu. (Ane took offense at my words.) endi~eleniyor. (Her family worries.) Ailesi Ay~e gocundu. (Ane took offence.) 112 3b 3b huy-lankaygt-lan- 3b onur-lan- 3b sogu- II. 3. 2. Ali adamm tavtrlanndan huylandt. Ali huyland1. (Ali got uneasy with the man's attitudes.) (Ali got uneasy.) Ailesi hastahgmdan kaygilamyor. Ailesi kaygtlamyor. (Her family worries about her illness.) (Her family worries.) Ali ~ok onurlandt. davrant~tan ~ok onurlandt. (Ali felt honored very much with this behavior.) (Ali felt honored very much.) Arkad~lan ondan sogudu. Arkad~lan sogudu. (His friends lost their love for him.) (His friends lost their love.) Table 5. Opttonal Argument Deletion of Type 3b Verbs Ali bu Psych Verbs and Causativity in Turkish Causativity can be regarded as a transitivizing operation which makes intransitive verbs transitive adding a Causer argument to the argument structure. If the verb is already a transitive verb, the causativity has the function of increasing the degree of the transitivity ofthe verb. There are two types of causativity in Turkish; morphological and periphrastic. Periphrastic causatives are formed with predications like sag/a-, neden ol- etc. Morphological causatives on the other hand uses one of the causative suffixes -(D)lr, and(l)t. The affix to be attached is determined by the phonological conditions. Multiple causation is also possible in Turkish. Theoretically there is no limit to the addition of the Causer arguments but, as the number of the Causers and Causees increase, it gets harder to interpret the causative events semantically. Thus, there is a semantic restriction (though not logical) on the number of the Causers to be added, not just for psych causatives but for all causative events. Therefore, the addition of Causer arguments is limited with the causative events which can be interpretable. It may also have discoursal and stylistic functions but beyond the interpretability boundary, multiple causation is a vacuous application. 113 This section analyzes the relationship between the psych verb types in Turkish and causativity. The analysis shows that morphologically there are five causativity types valid for all psych verb classes: 1. Morphological Causatives: They utilize one of the phonologically determined causative morphemes. 1. 1. There are psych verbs that can be causativized which we will refer to as(+) 1. 2. There are psych verbs only the causative forms of which have psych Caus. meanings which we will refer to as Only Caus. 1. 3. There are verbs that can be causativized but the causative form and the non- causative form of the verb have different meanings which are both psych which we will refer to as Diff Psych. 1. 4. There are psych verbs only the non-causative forms of which have psych meanings which we will refer to as Only Non Caus. 1. 5. There are psych verbs that can not be causativized which we will refer to as (-) Caus. 2. ol-. Periphrastic causatives: They utilize one of the predications like sag/a- and neden 114 II. 3. 2. 1. Morphologically Causative Psych Verbs II. 3. 2. 1. 1. Type 1 Psych Verbs and Morphological Causativity Type 1 has psych verbs which belong to five of the mentioned classes: (+)CAUS •.. affet -(Dir)ammsa-(t)ba~t~la -{t)be~en -{Dir)benimse -(t)dti~Un -{Dir)hattrla -(t)hisset -(Dir)kavra -{t)ktskan -(Dir)sev -(Dir)sez -(Dir)unut -{Dir)- . anlamlan -(Dlr)oyna -{t)ye -(Dir)bat -(lr)c;tk -{lr)- c;ak:t c;ak -(Dir)kap:t kap -(Dir)- Table L anla -{t)iste -{t)ktvlr -(t)- alglla -{t)arzula -{t)dile -(t)garipse -(t)kotiimse -{t)kurgula -{t)onemse -(t)ongor -{Dir)san -{Dir)um -(Dir)varsay -(Dir)yarglla -{t)zannet -{Dir)- Type I Psych Verbs and Causativity In the first class there are psych verbs which can be causativized such as: (1) a. Ali Ay~e'yi E~tperiencer (NOM) Ali forgave b. Ali affetti. Theme (ACC) Ay~e. Ay~eyi Causer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ahmet'e affettirdi. Experiencer (OAT) Ali made Ahmet forgive Ay~e. c. Ali Ahmet'e Ay§e'yi affettirdi. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (OAT) Theme (ACC) Ali made Ahmet forgive Ay~e. d. Ali Ay§e'yi affettirdi. Causer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali made Ay~e be forgiven. e. *Ali Ahmet' e affettirdi. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (OAT) *Ali made Ahrnet forgive. f. Ali kendini Ay§e'ye affettirdi. Causer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Experiencer (OAT) Ali made himself be forgiven by Ay~e. 115 g. Ali Ay~e'ye kendini affettirdi. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (DA T) Theme (ACC) Ali made himself be forgiven by Ane. h. Ali kendini affettirdi. Causer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ali made himself be forgiven. (2) a. Ali bu fikri benimsedi. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ali adopted this idea. b. Ali Ay§e'ye bu fikri benimsetti. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (DA T) Theme (ACC) Ali made Ane adopt this idea. (3) a.~ Turk kahvesini sevdi. Experiencer Theme (NOM) Ay~e loved (ACC) the Turkish coffee. b. Ahmet Ay$e'ye Turk kahvesini sevdirdi. Causer Experiencer (DAT) made Ay~e Theme (NOM) Ahmet (ACC) love the Turkish coffee. c. Ali Ay$e'yi sevdi. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ali loved Ay~e. d.? Ahmet Ali'ye Ay§e'yi sevdirdi. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (DA T) Ahmet made Ali love (ACC) Ay~e. (4) a. Ali kotu haberi sezdi. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ali felt the bad news. b. Ali Ay$e'ye koru haberi sezdirdi. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (DA T) Theme (ACC) Ali implied the bad news to Ay~e. (5) a. Ali eski gunleri unuttu. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ali has forgotten the past. b. Ali Ay~e'ye Causer Experiencer (NOM) (DA T) eski gtinleri unutturdu. Theme (ACC) Ali made Ane forget the past. 116 In (la), the verb affet- has a nominative subject and an accusative object. In the causative form (1 b), a Causer argument is added to the subject position, the Theme is still accusative but the Experiencer is dative. ( 1c) shows that the reversal order of the Theme and the Experiencer is also possible keeping the case marking of the thematic arguments the same. Whichever argument is in the preverbal position, it is the focused one. In (ld), we see that the Causer argument and the accusative marked Theme argument can be used without the Experiencer. This accusative Theme can also be a reflexive pronoun with or without the dative Experiencer as in (1 f) and (I g). However, the Causer and the Experiencer without the Theme is ungrammatical as in (le). The English counterpart missing a Theme argument is also ungrammatical. All these can be formulated as: 'J:'yp~ Typel Psych Construction Experiencer + Theme (NOM) t ~~M~~tiY~,,~,~y~Jt.{;;~tl§t~gcti9~ . ' Causer+ Experiencer+ Theme (ACC) (NOM) (OAT) (ACC) Causer+ Theme+ Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) {OAT) Causer+ Theme (NOM) (ACC) * Causer+ Experiencer (NOM) Table 2. {OAT) Type I Causative Psych Construction (3b) also seems to be more acceptable than (3d). The verb sev1- in (3b) means "changing one's attitude in a more positive way" but sev2- in (3c) and (d) means "to have romantic feelings for someone or to feel attracted to someone". However, there are some exceptions to the causativity rules above. In the following sentences, there are psych verbs which have a nominative Causer and an accusative Theme ((6)- (12) sentences). These sentences are normally interpreted as if there is an implied Experiencer argument. This dative marked Experiencer is optional ((b) examples). 117 (c) and (d) examples suggest the two alternative interpretations of (a)s. In (c)s, there is a nominative Causer and a genitive Theme with a periphrastic causative which nominalizes the verb. In (d)s, there is a nominative Causer and a genitive Theme with an optional Experiencer. Here, similarly, there is a periphrastic causative nominalizing the passivized verb. This kind of a passive interpretation can also be observed in the English translations of the sentences. From (6) to (12) of the following, all sentences normally accept the interpretation (d) rather than (c): (6)a. Ali Ayse'yi affettirdi. Causer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e Ali made b. Ali be forgiven. Ay~e'yi herkese affettirdi. Experiencer (OAT) c. *Ali Ay~e'nin affetmesini sagladt/ neden oldu. d. Ali Ay~e'nin (herkes tarafmdan) affed(il)mesini sagladt/ neden oldu. (7)a. Ali Ayse'yi an1msatt1. Causer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ali made Ay~e be remembered. b. Ali Ay~e'yi herkese ammsatt1. Experiencer (OAT) c. *Ali Ay~e'nin arumsamasm1 sagladtl neden oldu. d. Ali Ay~e'nin (herkes tarafmdan) antmsa(n)masmt sagladtl neden oldu. (8)a. Ali Ane'yi bagt~lattt. Causer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e be Ali be b. Ali forgiven . Ay~e'yi herkese bagt~lattt. Experiencer (OAT) c. *Ali Ay~e'nin bag1~lamasm1 sagladt/ neden oldu.) 118 d. Ali Ane'nin (herkes tarafmdan) bagt~la(n)masmt sagladt/ neden oldu. (9)a. Ali Ayse'yi begendirdi. Causer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali made Ane be liked. b. Ali Ay~e'yi herkese begendirdi. Experiencer (OAT) c. *Ali Ay~e'nin begenmesini sagladt/ neden oldu. d. Ali Ay~e'nin (herkes tarafmdan) begen(il)mesini sagladt/ neden oldu. (lO)a. Ali Ayse'yi benimsetti. Causer (NOM) Ali made b. Ali Theme (ACC) Ay~e be Ay~e'yi accepted. herkese benimsetti. Experiencer (DAT) c. *Ali d. Ali (II) Ay~e'nin Ay~e'nin benimsemesini sagladt/ neden oldu. (herkes tarafmdan) benimse(n)mesini sagladt/ neden oldu. a. Ali Ayse'yi sevdirdi. Causer (NOM) Ali Theme (ACC) made Ay~e be b. Ali Ay~e'yi loved. herkese sevdirdi. Experiencer (DAT) (12) c. *Ali Ay~e'nin d. Ali oldu. Ay~e'nin sevmesini sagladt/ neden oldu. (herkes tarafmdan) sev(il)mesini saglad1/ neden a. Ali Ayse'yi unutturdu. Causer. (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali made Ane be forgotten , b. Ali Ay~e'yi herkese unutturdu. Experiencer (DAT) c. *Ali Ay~e'nin unutmasm1 saglad1/ neden oldu. 119 d. Ali Ane'nin (herkes tarafmdan) unut(ul)masmt sagladt/ neden oldu. However, in (13a) and (14a) Ay~e is an accusative Experiencer not a Theme unlike (6)- (12) above. Therefore (b)s are unacceptable and the normal interpretations are (c)s but not (d)s: a. Ali Ayse'yi du~fuldiirdu. (13) Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e Ali made think. b. *Ali Ayse'yi herkese du~iindiirdu. Causer Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ACC) c. Ali Ane'nin (DA T) dii~unmesini sagladt/ neden oldu. d. *Ali Ane'nin (herkes tarafmdan) oldu. (14) dii~Un(Ul)mesini sagladt/ neden a. Ali Ayse'yi ktskandtrdt. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali made Ay~e envy. b. *Ali Ayse'yi herkese ktskandtrdt. Causer Experiencer (NOM) c. Ali Theme (ACC) Ay~e'nin (DA T) ktskanmasmt sagladt/ neden oldu. d. *Ali Ay~e'nin (herkes tarafmdan) ktskan(tl)masmt sagladt/ neden oldu. The conclusion could be that the verbs in (13) and (14) are not the causative forms of the verbs kzskan- and du~un- rather they are separate lexical entries which belong to Type 2 psych verbs: (15) a. Ali Ayse'yi ktskandt. (Type 1) Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) b. Ali Ayse'yi ktskandtrdt. (Type 2) Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) c. Ali Ayse'yi dii~Undii. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (Type 1) 120 d. Ali Ay§e'yi du~fuldurdu. (Type 2) Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) For these verbs, the rule above can be restated adding the last two formulas: Type 1 Psych Construction Experiencer + Theme (NOM) Type 1 Causative Psych Construction Causer+ Experiencer+ Theme (NOM) (ACC) (OAT) (ACC) Causer + Theme+ Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) (OAT) Causer+ Theme (NOM) (ACC) * Causer+ Experiencer (NOM) (OAT) Causer+ Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) *Causer+ Experiencer+ Theme Table 3. (NOM) (ACC) Type I Causative Psych Construction (Revised) (OAT) There are some verbs of Type 1 which can not be causativized: (1) a.~ Ali'yi arzuladt. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e desired for Ali. b. *Ahmet Ay§e'ye Ali'yi arzulatt1. Causer (NOM) Experiencer Theme (OAT) (ACC) * Ahmet made Ay~e desire for Ali. (2) a. Ay§e evliligi diledi. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e wished for marriage. b. *Ali Ay§e'ye evliligi diletti. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) (ACC) *Ali made Ay~e wish for marriage. (3) a. Ali bu davrant§lart garipsedi. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali found these behaviors strange. b. *Ay§e Ali 'ye bu davram§lart garipsetti. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (OAT) Theme (ACC) * Ay~e made Ali find these behaviors strange. (4) a. Ali bu dersi hafifsedi. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali took this course lightly. 121 b. *Arkadaslan Ali 'ye bu dersi hafifsetti. Causer (NOM) Experiencer Theme (DA T) (ACC) *His friends made Ali take this course lightly. (5) a. Ayse ailesini her zaman onemsedi. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e Theme (ACC) has always cared about his family. b. *Ali Ayse'ye ailesini onemsetti. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (DA T) (ACC) *Ali made Ay~e care about her family. When the Causer is inanimate, causative forms of these verbs especially with a generic Experiencer seem more acceptable (although they are not perfect): (6) a. ?Bu reklam bana kola is;meyi arzulattyor. Causer (NOM) Experiencer Theme (DA T) (ACC) This commercial makes me desire for cola. b. Bu reklam insana kola is;meyi arzulattyor. Causer (NOM) Experiencer Theme (DA T) (ACC) This commercial makes people desire for cola. (7) ?Onlarm mutlulugu Ali 'ye evliligi diletti. Causer (NOM) Experiencer (DA T) Theme (ACC) Their happiness caused Ali to wish for marriage. (8) ?Gordtiklerim ve duydukl<:ll'Im bana da bu davran1slar1 garipsetti. Causer (NOM) Experiencer (DA T) Theme (ACC) What I had seen and heard caused me too to find these behaviors strange. (9) ?Smavda ald1g1 iyi notlar Ali'ye bu dersi hafifsetti. Causer (NOM) Experiencer Theme (DA T) (ACC) Good grades that he got made Ali take this course lightly. (10) ?Ayse'nin telkinleri Ali'ye ailesini onemsetti. Causer (NOM) Ay~e's Experiencer Theme (DAT) (ACC) consolations made Ali care about his own family. 122 II. 3. 2. 1. 2. Type 2 Psych Verbs and Morphological Causativity As we mentioned in the previous chapters, there is a Ut1iversality in the unexpected behavior of Type 2 psych verbs. Sometimes the aspectual properties were seen as the reason for this. The most thoroughly discussed of these properties is the causation which is assumed to be inherent in the meaning of these verbs. Our analysis of Type 2 psych verbs in Turkish shows that none of these verbs can be used causatively: (+) CAUS. DlFF. PSYC_H ···· · •· \ ; ~9N- Cj\l,JS. 0 0 ONLYCAUS. ayar -(t)avu -(t)-kt~ktr -(t)- 0 Table 4. . .' ~ J-)CAUS • All of the Type 2 verbs .. Type 2 Psych Verbs and Causat1v1ty Therefore, the assumption that the semantic notion of causation is inherent in the meaning of these verbs seems to be valid in Turkish too as the ungrammaticality of the following causative counterparts demonstrate: ( 1) a. Konu~mact Theme (NOM) dinleyicileri baydt/ stktt. Experiencer (ACC) The speaker bored the audience. b. *Adam konu$mactya dinleyicileri baydtrdt/ stkttrdt. Causer (NOM) Theme (DA T) Experiencer (ACC) *The man made the speaker bore the audience. (2) a. Ane Ali'yi herkesin oniinde bozdu. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ane humiliated Ali in front of everyone. b. *Ahmet Ali 'ye Ane 'yi herkesin oniinde bozdurdu. Causer (NOM) Theme (DA T) Experiencer (ACC) *Ahmet made Ali humiliate Ay~e in front of everyone. Another use of boz- as a compound with a possessive psych noun like keyfinil moralini etc. in causative is acceptable: 123 c.~ Ali'nin moralini bozdu. Theme (NOM) Ay~e Experiencer (ACC) depressed Ali. d. Ahmet Ane'ye Ali'nin moralini bozdurdu. Causer (NOM) Theme (DA T) Experiencer (ACC) Ahmet made Ay~e depress Ali. (3) a. Ay~e Ali'yi yok etkiledi. Theme (NOM) Ay~e Experiencer (ACC) affected Ali deeply. b. *Ahmet Ay~e'ye Ali 'yi yok etkiletti. Causer (NOM) Theme (DAT) Experiencer (ACC) *Ahmet made Ay~e affect Ali deeply. (4) a.~ Ali'yi k1rd1. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e hurt Ali. b. *Ahmet Ali'ye Ay~e'yi kudud1. Causer (NOM) Theme (DA T) Experiencer (ACC) *Ahmet made Ali hurt Ay~e. (5) a. Ane Ali'yi sarsti. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e gave Ali a shock. b. *Ahmet Ay~e'ye Ali 'yi sarstud1. Causer (NOM) Theme (DA T) Experiencer (ACC) *Ahmet made Ay~e give Ali a shock. (6) a. Ay~e Ali'yi iizdii. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e depressed Ali. b. *Ahmet Ali'ye Ay~e'yi iizdiirdii. Causer (NOM) Theme (DAT) Experiencer (ACC) *Ahmet made Ali depress Ay~e. 124 In fact there is not either a phonological or a semantic restriction to causativize these verbs. A verb like Uzdtir- is both phonologically and semantically possible (as the possibility of a periphrastic causative like iiziilmesini sag/a- (which will be discussed in the related chapter) demonstrates). Thus, the restriction seems to be related to only the morphological causativity. Most of the Type 2 verbs above have primarily a non psych/ physical sense. When these are used in their first sense it is possible to causativize them; like szk- lszk-tzr-, boz- I boz-dur-, kzr- I kzr-dzr- etc. There are only 3 verbs of Type 2 whose bases seem to be inseparable from the causative morpheme: (1) a. *Ali Ay§e'yi ayard1. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) b. *Ahmet Ali 'ye Ay§e 'yi ayartt1. Causer (NOM) Theme (DA T) Ahmet made Ali Expcriencer (ACC) seduce Ay~e. c. Ali Ay§e'yi ayartt1. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali seduced Ay~e. d. Ahmet Ay~e'nin ayarttlmasmt sagladt/ neden oldu. (2) a. *Ali Ay§e'yi aydmladt. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) b. *Ahmet Ali'ye Ay§e'yi aydmlatt1. Causer (NOM) Theme (OAT) Ahmet made Ali Experiencer (ACC) enlighten Ay~e. c. Ali Ay§e'yi aydmlattt. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali enlightened Ay~e. d. Ahrnet Ay~e'nin aydmlanmasmt sagladt/ neden oldu. 125 (3) a. Ali Ayse'yi kt~kudt. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) b.* Ahmet Ali'ye Ane'yi Causer (NOM) Theme (DA T) kt~ktrttt. Experiencer (ACC) Ahmet made Ali incite Ay~e. c. Ali Ane'yi kt~ktrttt. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali incited Ane. d. Ahmet Ane'nin kt~kumasmt sagladt/ neden oldu. In (1), (2) and (3) above the base verbs can not be used without the causative morpheme as in ( 1a), (2a) and (3a). The normal causative construction with the addition of a nominative Causer argument is also ungrammatical as in (lb), (2b) and (3b). The only possibility is the usual Type 2 construction; a nominative Theme and an accusative Experiencer ((lc), (2c) and (3c) with a causative semantic interpretation as in (ld), (2d) and (3d). The grammatical and ungrammatical examples lead to the following rule for Type 2 causative psych constructions: Type 2 Psych Construct!on Theme + Experiencer (ACC) * Causer+ Theme+ Experiencer (NOM) (NOM) (DA T) (ACC) Theme + Experiencer Table 5. (ACC) (NOM) Type 2 Causative Psych ConstructiOn Therefore, the only possibility to express causativity is to use the Type 2 psych verbs in a normal psych construction. Thus we argue that these lexical causatives do not permit for further morphological causativization which means that all of the causativity rules for Type 1 verbs are invalid for Type 2 verbs. 126 II. 3. 2. 1. 3. Type 3a Psych Verbs and Morphological Causativity Type 3a psych verbs have also verbs belonging to five of the classes mentioned above: (+) CAUS. c,:tldtr -(t)danl-(t)duygulan -(Dlr)efkarlan -(Dlr)giicen -(Dlr)heveslen -(Dlr)huysuzlan -(Dlr)inan -(Dir)ktz -(Dlr)kiis -(Dlr)ofkelen -(Dlr)ozen -(Dlr)sabtrstzlan -(Dlr)sinirlen -(Dlr)- ONLYCAUS. DIFF.PSYCH NON-CAUS. (-)CAUS. ai-(Dlr)- 0 dal-(Dlr)kopiir -( t)patla -(t)tellen -(Dlr)- giiven -(Dlr)ktr-(Dlr)- Table 6. ~a~akal-(Dlr)- takll-(Dlr)vurul -(Dlr)- .. Type 3a Psych Verbs and Causattvtty The causativization with Type 3a verbs is the reverse procedure of the causativization with Type 1 verbs: Ty~e 1 causativization: la. Ali Ay$e'yi affetti. Ty~e 3a Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ali forgave Ay~e. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Ane believed Ali. 2a. Ahmet Ay$eyi Ali 'ye affettirdi. 2b. Ahmet Ali 'yi Ay§e'ye inandtrdl. causativization: lb. Ali Ay$e'ye inand1. Causer Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) (OAT) Ahmet made Ali forgive Ay~. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (OAT) Ahmet made Ali believe Ay~. 3a. Ahmet Ali'yeAne'yi affettirdi. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) (ACC) Ahmet made Ali forgive Ane. Table 7. 3b. Ahmet Ane'ye Ali'yi inandud1. Causer Theme Experiencer (NOM) (OAT) (ACC) Ahmet made Ali believe Ane. Type I and Type 3a Causative Psych Constructton (Companson) Recall that the causativization of Type 1 verbs is simply the addition of a nominative Causer argument and the dative marking of the Experiencer argument ((la) and (2a)). If we exchange the order of the Experiencer and the Theme keeping the case marking of the thematic roles the same, the difference is nothing more than the emphasis. This is also valid for Type 3a causatives ((3a) and (3b)). 127 When we causativize Type 3a verbs, similarly a nominative Causer argument is added to the subject position. The dative Theme argument does not change but the Experiencer is accusative marked ((1b) and (2b)). One difference between Type 1 and 3a in terms of causativization is that Type 1 verbs allow the optional deletion of the dative Experiencer argument (4a) but Type 3a verbs do not (4b). Moreover, Type 1 verbs do not allow the deletion of accusative Theme argument (5a) but Type 3a verbs do allow the deletion of the dative Theme (5b): Type 1 causativizatiob 4a. Ahmet Ay~e'yi affettirdi. ,, ~' Type 3a ~ausati"yizalion 4b. *Ahmet Ay~e'ye inand1rd1. (' Causer Theme (NOM) (OAT) *Ahmet made Ay~e be believed, Causer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ahmet made Ay~e be forgiven, Sa. *Ahmet Ali'ye affettirdi. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (OAT) *Ahmet made Ali forgive, Table 8, ,, 5b. Ahmet Ali 'yi inand1rd1. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ahmet made Ali believe, Type I and Type 3a Causat1ve Psych Construction (Differences) As expected, verbs of Type 1 do not allow the deletion of the accusative argument in a causative structure since an accusative marked argument is what these verbs necessitate in non causative structures. The unexpected point is that although a Type 3a verb requires for a dative marked argument in a non causative structure, it is ungrammatical with a dative argument in causative structure and requires for an accusative argument. This shows that causativity is not sensitive to argument structure and that it necessitates the existence of an accusative argument no matter what the verb takes in non causative structure. The following sentences (6) and (7) are the verbs of Type 3a that can be causativized. (6c) and (7c) show that these verbs allow the deletion of dative Theme argument: (6) a. Ali Ay$e'ye danld1/ gticendi/ ktistti. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Ali put out with Ay~e, 128 b. Ahmet Ali'yi Ane'ye danlttl/ gucendirdi/ kusrurdu. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (DA T) Ahmet made Ali. c. Ali Ayse'yi danltti/ gucendirdi/ kustfudu. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) (7) a. Ali Ayse'ye k1zdi/ otkelendi/ sinirlendi. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Ali got angry with Ay~e. b. Ahmet Ali'yi Ayse'ye kizdud1/ otkelendirdi/ sinirlendirdi. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (OAT) Ahmet made Ali angry at Ay~e . c. Ali Ayse'yi kizdud1/ otkelendirdi/ sinirlendirdi. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali made Ay~e angry. There are two ways of looking at the verb forms in (6c), (7c) and (8c). From the first point of view (c) sentences imply a deleted dative reflexive pronoun coindexed with the Causer: (8) Ali Ayse'yi (kendine) danltt1/ gucendirdi/ klistlirdU/ kizdirdi/ otkelendirdi/ Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Theme (DA T) sinirlendirdi/ ozendirdi. From the second point of view, these verbs are lexical causatives of Type 2 where the subject is interpreted either as a Causer or as a Theme: (9) Ali Ayse'yi dariltt1/ gucendirdi/ kustlirdU/ kizd1rdii ofkelendirdi/ Causer/ Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) sinirlendirdi/ ozendirdi. The following rules can be suggested for the causativization of Type 3a psych verbs. The first two rules are invalid for some Type 3a verbs as mentioned above: Type 3a Psych Construction Experiencer + Theme (NOM) (OAT) Type 3a ~ausative Psych Construction Causer+ Experiencer+ Theme (NOM) (ACC) (OAT) Causer+ Theme+ Experiencer (NOM) (OAT) (ACC) 129 *Causer+ Theme (NOM) (OAT) Causer+ Experiencer Table 9. (NOM) (ACC) Type Type 3a Causat1ve Psych Construction II. 3. 2. 1. 4. Type 3b Psych Verbs and Morphological Causativity The most thoroughly discussed phenomenon in the literature of psych verbs is the psychological causativity and the representative verbs fear/ frighten which are kork-1 korkut- in Turkish. Goksel (1993) claims that these verbs are not different from ordinary one place predicates. The only difference is in their causative form which results from the ablative marking in Turkish. She claims that the ungrammaticality of such a sentence like: (1) *Zehra beni Nam1k'tan korkuttu. is because of the impossibility of using a psych causative with an ablative which itself expresses a kind of causality. According to her, not only for psych causatives but also for all causatives, it is ungrammatical to use causal ablative arguments in causative constructions. Therefore she regards (2) as ungrammatical as well as (1 ): (2) *Tetanoz John'u hastahktan oldiirdii. *Tetanus killed John with disease. In our analysis of Type 3b psych verbs, such examples are not ungrammatical since it is not uninterpretable to regard Zehra/ Tetanoz as the Causer, benil John 'u as the Experiencer and the ablative marked Namtk 'tan/ hastalzktan as the Theme. It is possible to construct a causal chain in which one Causer causes the other Causer which causes another event as in the following example: (3) Kirli su Ay:;;e'yi dizanteriden oldiirdii. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (ABL) Dirty water killed Ay~e with dysentery. 130 It is also possible for an animate outside Causer to cause an Experiencer experience a psychological state. In this case, there are both a Causer argument and an ablative Theme argument which is a more direct cause of the emotional state of the Experiencer: (4) Annesi cocugu doktordan korkuttu. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (ABL) Her mother made the child fear the doctor. The preverbal focus makes the sentence sound more acceptable: (5) Cocugu doktordan annesi korkuttu. Experiencer Theme Causer (ACC) (ABL) (NOM) In all such examples in this study, the Theme expresses the direct Causer of the event and the Causer expresses a secondary, indirect or an outside Causer. The majority of the verbs of Type 3b can be causativized: (+) CAUS. bez -(Dir)h1k -(Dir)bunal-(t)c;ek -(Dir)igren -(Dir) kork -(t)ku~kulan -(Dir)sogu -(t)~iiphelen -(Dir)tiksin -(Dir)usan -(Dir)utan -(Dir)- ONLYCAUS. 0 DIFF.PSYCH 0 NON-CAUS. 0 (-)CAUS. slkll -(Dir)- .. Table 10. Type 3b Psych Verbs and CausatlVIty As the following examples (1)- (3) shows, a nominative or accusative Causer is added, Experiencer is accusative marked and the ablative marked Theme stays the same. However, (b) sentences become much more acceptable when the Causer follows the Theme as the Experiencer in the subject position ((c) sentences) due to the focusing of the preverbal element. (d) and (e) alternatives are not ungrammatical but in (d)s, there is not a specific source of the mental/ emotional state of the Experiencer, therefore, there is a 131 generality. In (e)s, there is not a specific Experiencer of the mental! emotional state and, therefore, a generic Experiencer is interpreted, so, the optional deletion does not cause ungrammaticality but only an information gap. Ay~e'den (1) a. Ali Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ABL) btktt/ usand1. Ali was fed up with Ay~e. b. Bu davran1~lar Ali'yi Ay~eden Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (ABL) btktudt/ usand1rd1. Such behaviors ofherselfmade Ali fed up with Ay~e. c. Ali'yi Ay~e'den bu davran1~lar btkttrdt/ usand1rd1. Experiencer Theme (ACC) (ABL) Causer (NOM) d. Bu davrant~lar Ali'yi btktudt. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) (generally) e. Bu davrant~lar Ay~e'den btktlrd1. (everybody) Causer Theme (NOM) (ABL) (2) a. Ali tavuktan igrendi/ tiksindi. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ABL) Ali was disgusted with chicken. b. Bu lokanta Ali'yi tavuktan igrendirdi/ tiksindirdi. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (ABL) This restaurant made Ali disgusted with chicken. Experiencer Theme Causer (ACC) (ABL) (NOM) d. Bu lokanta Ali'yi igrendirdi/ tiksindirdi. (generally) Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) e. Bu lokanta bahktan igrendirdi/ tiksindirdi. Causer Theme (NOM) (ABL) (3) a. Ali Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e'den ku~kulandt/ ~uphelendi. Theme (ABL) Ali was suspected of Ay~e. (everybody) 132 b. Polis Ali'yi Ay~e'den ku~kulandirdi/ ~iiphelendirdi. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (ABL) The police made Ali suspected of Ane. c. Ali'yi Ayse'den polis ku~kulandudii ~iiphelendirdi. Experiencer Theme Causer (ACC) (ABL) (NOM) d. Polis Ali'yi ku~kulandud1. (generally) Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) e. Polis Ay~e'den ku~kulandud1. Causer Theme (NOM) (ABL) (everybody) The (d) examples from (1) to (3) demonstrate a similar case with the cases in other verb types. The causative forms of these verbs can either be regarded as having a deleted optional ablative Theme or they can be regarded as separate entries which belong to Type 2: ( 1) Ali Ayse 'yi bikt1rdi/ igrendirdi/ tiksindirdi/ ku~kulandud1/ ~iiphelendirdi/ korkuttu/ iirkiittii/ utand1rd1. The latter treatment of these verbs (as Type 2 verbs) correspond to the universal treatment since in all of the languages analyzed in the psych verb literature (English, Italian, French, Spanish, Finnish, Polish, Greek etc.), these verbs belong to the second class named as frighten type verbs. In the examples above the ablative expresses the source of the mentaV emotional state. Therefore, the rules for Type 3b causativization can be stated as: 133 II. 3. 2. 1. 5. Type 4 Psych Verbs and Morphological Causativity (+)CAUS. az -(Dir)bilin9len -(Dir)co~ -(Dir)eglen -(Dir)heyecanlan -(Dir)kudur -(t)sakinle~ -(Dir)yatl~ -{Dir)- 0 ka9 -(Ir)- dol-(Dir)don -(Dir)kabar -(t)- ap1~ -(Dir)durul-(t)kubar -(t)siiziil-(t)yirtll-(t)- Table 12. Type 4 Psych Verbs and Causativity The only argument of the Type 4 verbs is the Experiencer. Causativization adds one more argument to the following intransitive sentences which is a nominative Causer. Therefore causativization process turns all of the intransitive psych verbs of Type 4 into transitive Type 2 verbs: ( 1) a. Cocuklar azd1. Experiencer (NOM) The children got wild. b. Cocuklar az1tt1. Experiencer (NOM) The children got wild. c. Ablast cocuklart azdud1. Causer (NOM) Experiencer (ACC) Their sister made the children out of control. (2) a. Kalabahk co~uyor. Experiencer (NOM) The crowd cheers. b. Sarktct kalabahg1 Causer (NOM) co~turuyor. Experiencer (ACC) The singer cheers the crowd. (3) a. Ay:;;e heyecanlamyor. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e gets excited. 134 b. Ali Ayse'yi heyecanlandmyor. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali makes Ane excited. (4) a. Annem sakinle~iyor/ yat1~1yor. Experiencer (NOM) My mother calms down. b. Baham annemi Causer (NOM) sakinle~tiriyor/ yat1~tlnyor. Experiencer (ACC) My father makes my mother calm down. The following formula summarizes the only way of causativizing the Type 4 verbs: Type 4 Psych Construction Experiencer (NOM) Type 4 Causative Psych Construction Causer+ Experiencer (ACC) (NOM) Table 13. Type 4 Causative Psych Construction In this section, we analyzed the causativity properties of Type 1, 2, 3a, 3b and 4 psych verbs in Turkish. The analysis has shown that in a causative construction, some verbs which belong to other verb classes behave like lexical causatives of Type 2. In all of these verbs the subject can either be regarded as a Causer or as a Theme as the Table 14 shows. On the other hand, the following Table 1 on pages XIX and XX summarize the causativity properties of all psych verb types in Turkish: ~ Experiencer + Theme+ Causer (NOM) (ABL) (ACC) TYP~4 Experiencer (NOM) Ali'yi tavuktan bu lokanta igrendirdi. Experiencer Theme Causer (ACC) (ABL) (NOM) (This restaurant made Ali disgusted with chicken.) ' Causer/ Theme+ Experiencer Ahmet Ali'yi utandtrdt. (NOM) (ACC) Causer/ Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) =Type2 (Ahmet embarrassed Ali.) Causer+ Theme Bu lokanta bahktan igrendirdi. (NOM) (ABL) Causer Theme (NOM) (ABL) (This restaurant made Ali disgusted .) Causer+ Experiencer Baham annemi sakinle~tiriyor/ yatt~tmyor. (NOM) (ACC) Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) ==Type 2 (My father makes my mother calm down .) Table I. Summary of the Causativity Properties of Turkish Psych Verbs ' 135 Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) r.:-------1 lctistiirdti/ ktzdudt/ ofkelendirdi/ ozendirdi/ sinirlendirdi/ ~a~trttl. Causer/ Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) ~---,-------1 ku~kulandudt/ ~Uphelendirdi/ tiksindirdi/Urktittti/ r.---:--.--i utandudt. Type4 Babam Causer/ Theme (NOM) annem1 Experiencer (ACC) yatl~tmyor. usandtrdt/ 136 Periphrastic Causative Psych Verbs II. 3.2. 2. In Turkish, causativity can be expressed with periphrastic constructions as well as by lexical means. Morphological causativity is a verbal process though periphrastic causativity is a nominal one. Sag/a- and neden ol- are the most often used predications of these constructions. With non psych verbs the DAT marked Actor/ Doer/ Agent of the non causative construction gets the GEN marking and the causation event with the nominalized verb (with -rnA) takes the accusative case for sagla- (1b) and dative case for neden o (1c): ( 1) a. Ali Ay$e'ye kitab1 okuttu. Causer Causee Theme (NOM) (DA T) (ACC) b. Ali Ay$e'nin kitab1 okumasm1 saglad1. Causer Causee Theme (NOM) (GEN) (ACC) c. Ali Ay§e'nin kitab1 okumasma neden/ neden oldu. Causer Causee Theme (NOM) (GEN) (DA T) Stylistic variations changing the syntactic position of the arguments are also possible. This section briefly analyzes the interrelationships between the psych verb types, morphological causativity and periphrastic causativity in Turkish. II. 3. 2. 2. 1. Type 1 Psych Verbs and Periphrastic Causativity As mentioned, Causer+ Experiencer order was ungrammatical for Type 1 (NOM.) (DA T.) 137 psych verbs without the existence of an accusative Theme . Since a periphrastic causative construction does not alter the thematic roles, the ungrammaticality of (la) can not be ruled out by (lb): (1) a. *Ali Ahmet'e affettirdi. b. *Ali Ahmet'e affetmesine neden oldu. All of the verbs of Type 1 which can be morphologically causativized can also be used in periphrastic constructions. As mentioned in the previous section, except for two verbs, all of the causativizable Type 1 verbs have similar semantic interpretations in the periphrastic causative construction: Ay~e'yi (2) a. Ali Causer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali made Ay~e b. Ali unutturdu. be forgotten. Ay~e'yi herkese unutturdu. Experiencer (OAT) Ay~e'nin c. *Ali d. Ali Ay~e'nin unutmasm1 saglad1/ neden oldu. (herkes tarafmdan) unut(ul)masm1 sagladt/ neden oldu. Unlike the other verbs Type 1, the two exceptional verbs of this type which behave like Type 2 verbs do not have passive interpretations with an optional Experiencer by phrase in their causative forms: (3) a. Ali Ay~e'yi dti~Undtirdli. Causer Experiencer· (NOM) (ACC) Ali made Ay~e think. b. *Ali Ay~e'yi Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) c. Ali herkese dti~Undtirdli. Theme (DA T) Ay~e'nin dti~Unmesini saglad1/ neden oldu. d. *Ali Ay~e'nin (herkes tarafmdan) dli~Un(Ul)mesini saglad1/ neden oldu. 138 (4)a. Ali Ane'yi ktskand1rd1. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali made Ane envy. b. *Ali Ay~e'yi herkese k1skandtrd1. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Theme (DA T) c. Ali Ay~e'nin ktskanmasmt sagladt/ neden oldu. b. *Ali Ay~e'nin (herkes tarafmdan) klskan(ll)masm1 sagladt/ neden oldu. Type 1 verbs which can not be morphologically causativized are acceptable in periphrastic causative constructions (although they are not perfect). The observed tendency is to use mental verbs often with sag/a- as in (Sd): (S)a. Ali bu sorunu ongordli. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali anticipated this problem. b.* Ahmet Ali'ye bu sorunu ongordlirdli. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (DA T) Theme (ACC) Ahmet made Ali anticipate the problem. c. ?Ahmet Ali 'nin bu sorunu ongormesine neden oldu. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (GEN) Theme (ACC) d. Ahmet Ali 'nin bu sorunu ongormesini saglad1. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (GEN) Theme (ACC) On the other hand, emotional verbs tend to be used with neden ol rather than sag/a: (6) a. Ay~e Ali'yi arzulad1. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e desired Ali. b. Ahmet Ay~e'ye Ali'yi arzulatt1. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (DA T) Ahmet made Ay~e Theme (ACC) desire Ali. c.? Ahmet Ay~e'nin Ali'yi arzulamasm1 saglad1. Causer (NOM) Experiencer (GEN) Theme (ACC) 139 c. Ahmet Ay$e'nin Ali'yi arzulamasma neden oldu. Causer (NOM) Experiencer (GEN) Theme (ACC) The underlying semantic reason may be due to the semantic difference between sag/a- and neden ol-. The former implies a conscious effort of the Causer to make the Experiencer experience the psych state. On the other hand the latter implies a nonconscious act (or not an act at all) of the Causer. This argument may be linked to the assumption that emotional activities are less controllable than the mental ones. Therefore, mental leanings may be controllable by outside agents by physically or consciously doing something but it is not easy for an outside Causer to have the control over the emotional inclinations of someone. Only the existence of a Causer or something that it does unconsciously causes the Experiencer to experience an emotional state. II. 3. 2. 2. 2. Type 2 Psych Verbs and Periphrastic Causativity As we mentioned in the previous sections, all of the Type 2 verbs are inherently causative and they can not be further causativized. That means all of the Type 2 verbs are lexical causatives and they can not be causativized morphologically: (1) a. Konu$mact dinleyicileri baydt/ s1kt1. Theme (NOM) Experiencer (ACC) The speaker bored the audience. b. *Adam konu$mactya dinleyicileri baydtrdt/ stktlrdt. Causer (NOM) Theme (DA T) Experiencer (ACC) *The man made the speaker bore the audience. However, it is possible to use all of the Type 2 verbs in periphrastic causative constructions with the verb passivized and with a reflexive interpretation as in (2d) and 140 (3d). That demonstrates our claim that the restriction on the causativity of these verbs is neither phonological nor semantic but a morphological one: (2) a. Ane Ali'yi UzdU. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e depressed Ali. b. *Ahmet Ali'ye Ayse'yi UzdUrdU. Causer (NOM) Theme (DAT) Experiencer (ACC) *Ahmet made Ali depress Ay~e. c. Ahmet Ali'nin Ay~e'yi Uzmesine neden oldu. d. Ayse Ali 'nin Uzlllmesine ned en oldu. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (GEN) (3) a.~ Ali'yi ktrd1. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e hurt Ali. b. Ahmet Ali'ye Ayse'yi ktrdtrdL Causer (NOM) Theme (DA T) Experiencer (ACC) *Ahmet made Ali hurt Ay~e. c. Ahmet Ali'nin Ay~e'yi ktrmasma neden oldu. c. Ali Ay:;;e'nin kmlmasma neden oldu. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (GEN) As mentioned, some of the Type 2 verbs seem to be inseparable from their causative morphemes. If we accept the -t morpheme attached to these verbs as the causative morpheme, the semantic interpretations of the following periphrastic causatives are reflexive: (3) a. *Ali Ay:;;e'yi kt~ktrdt. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali incited Ay~e. b. *Ahmet Ali'ye Ay:;;e'yi kt~ktrttl. Causer (NOM) Theme (DA T) Ahmet made Ali Experiencer (ACC) incite Ay~e. 141 c. Ali Ay~e'yi kt~ktrtti .. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali incited Ay~e. d. Ali Ay~e'nin kt~kmnasma neden oldu. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (GEN) (4) a. *Ali Ay~e'yi aydmlad1. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) b.* Ahmet Ali'ye Ay~e'yi aydmlatt1. Causer (NOM) Theme (DA T) Experiencer (ACC) Ahmet made Ali enlighten c. Ali Ay~e'yi Ay~e. aydmlatt1. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali enlightened Ay~e. d. Ali Ay~e'nin aydmlanmasm1 saglad1. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (GEN) II. 3. 2. 2. 3. Type 3a Psych Verbs and Periphrastic Causativity All of the Type 3a verbs that can be causativized can also be used m periphrastic psych constructions: (1) a. Ali Ay~e'ye danldtl glicendi/ ktistti. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (DAT) Ali put out with Ane. b. Ahmet Ali'yi Ay§e'ye danlttt/ glicendirdi/ ktistlirdli. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (DAT) Ahmet made Ali. c. Ali Ay§e'yi danlttt/ glicendirdi/ klisttirdli.. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) d. Ali Ay§e'nin danlmasmal glicenmesine/ klismesine neden oldu. Causer Expeeriencer (NOM) (GEN) 142 The only Type 3a verb aldzr which is not a psych verb without the causative morpheme can not be used in a periphrastic psych construction. However all of the Type 3a verbs whose non causative forms are psych are acceptable as periphrastic causatives: (1) a. AyO?e bu elbiseye baylld1. (psych) Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e Theme (DAT) liked this dress greatly. b. *Ali AyO?e'yi bu elbiseye baylltt1. Causer Ezxperiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e Theme (DA T) made Ali like this dress. c. Ali AyO?e'nin bu elbise'ye bayllmasma neden oldu. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (GEN) Theme (DAT) There are a few number of verbs of Type 3a which can not be causativized. These verbs can perfectly be causativized in a periphrastic construction: (3) a. *Ahmet Ali'yi AyO?e'ye giivendirdi. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Theme (DA T) Ahmet made Ali trust Ay~e. b. Ahmet Ali'nin AyO?e'ye giivenmesini saglad1. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (GEN) Theme (DA T) (4) a. *Ahmet Ali'yi AyO?e'ye kmldtrdl. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Theme (DA T) b. Ahme! Ali'nin AyO?e'ye kmlmasma neden oldu. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (GEN) II. 3. 2. 2. 4. Theme (DAT) Type 3b Psych Verbs and Periphrastic Causativity All Type 3b verbs can be used in periphrastic psych constructions. Most of the verbs of Type 3b that we examine have some kind of negative semantic content on the part of the Experiencer and they sound much more acceptable with neden ol- which led us to 143 notice that neden ol- is more compatible with negative psych events while sag/a- IS compatible with positive ones: (1) a. Bu davram~lar Ali'yi Ay~eden btktudt/ usandtrdt. Causer (NOM) Experiencer (ACC) Theme (ABL) Such behaviors ofherselfmade Ali fed up with Ay~e. b. Bu davrant~lar Ali'nin Ay~e'den btkmasma neden oldu. Causer (NOM) Experiencer (GEN) Theme (ABL) There is only one Type 3b verb which can not be causativized; szkzl. This verb can also be causativized in a periphrastic construction: (2) a. *Ahmet Ali'yi Ay~e'den Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) sikildirdt. Theme (ABL) b. Ahmet Ali'nin Ay~e'den stkilmasma neden oldu. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (ABL) II. 3. 2. 2. 5. Type 4 Psych Verbs and Periphrastic Causativity Type 4 verbs that can be morphologically causativized can also be causativized periphrastically. Other groups of Type 4 verbs whose either non causative or only the causative forms are psych can also be used periphrastically. The non causativizable verbs of Type 4 can be causativized too if used in periphrastic constructions (although they do not sound perfectly acceptable): (1) a. Ali duruldu. Experiencer (NOM) Ali calmed down. b. *Ay~e Ali 'yi durulttu. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ane made Ali calm down. 144 c. Ay$e Ali 'nin durulmasma neden oldu. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (GEN) This brief analysis shows that first, all types of psych verbs in Turkish can be used in periphrastic constructions no matter whether they are lexically or morphologically causative. Second, it became clear that all of the verbs of Type 1, 2, 3 and 4 which can not be causativized for this or that reason, can be causativized in periphrastic constructions. Third, it seems that sag/a- constructions mostly refer to positive psych events on the part of the Experiencer and neden ol- constructions refer to rather negative ones. Lastly, it is anticipated that sag/a- constructions with the implication of a conscious effort are more compatible with mental events while neden ol- constructions which imply a non conscious/ accidental cause are more compatible with emotional ones. II. 3. 2. 3. Causativity and the Permissive Reading As exemplified in the section above, all types of psych verbs that can be morphologically causativizable and non-causativizable can be used in periphrastic psych constructions. The periphrastic causative is a kind of paraphrase of the morphological counterpart just as it is the case with non-psych causatives. When the morphological counterpart is negated, the periphrastic paraphrase has a permissive reading: (1) a. Ailesi Ay~e'yi okuttu. b. Ailesi Ay~e'yi okutmad1. c. Ailesi Ay~e'nin okumasma izin vermedi. Her parents did not let Ay~e go on her education. 145 With a few number of psych verbs, this permissive reading is more significant than the others. Ignoring the thematic relations, in the following sentences, (c)s are the periphrastic permissive paraphrases of the morphological (b)s. For the purposes of focus and emphasis, the dative argument can move to the preverbal position: (2) a. Ali bize Ay$e'yi unutturdu. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (DA T) (ACC) Ali made us forgive Ay~e. b. Ali bize Ay§e'yi unutturmadt. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (DA T) (ACC) c. Ali Ay§e'nin (bizim taraf1m1zdan) unutulmasma izin vermedi. Causer (NOM) Theme (GEN) Ali did no let Ane to be forgotten (by us). (3) a. Ali Ay§e'yi bize kUstiirdU. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (DA T) Ali made Ay~e put up with us. b. Ali Ay§e'yi bize kUstUrmedi. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (DAT) c. Ali Ayse'nin bize ktismesine izin vermedi. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (GEN) (DA T) Ali did not let Ay~e put up with us. In the following example, a Type 2 verb which is assumed to be an inherently causative verb can not be causativized (4a) but the negative form (4b) seems to be acceptable with a corresponding permissive periphrastic paraphrase (4c): ( 4) a. *Ali Ayse'yi herkese iizdUrdU. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali made Ay~e Theme (OAT) be depressed by everyone. b. Ali Ay§e'yi kimseye Uzdtirmedi. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Theme (DA T) c. Ali Ay§e'nin (kimse tarafmdan) Uztilmesine izin vermedi. Causer Experiencer (NOM) (GEN) Ali did not let anyone depress Ay~e. 146 II. 3. 2. 4. Control, Causativity and the Psych Events As mentioned and tested before, emotional activities seem to be less controllable than the mental ones. Emotional activities are generally regarded vaguely as personal matters which can not be induced or created in other people. Control can be considered as two types with respect to the controller: 1. intemaV self control 2. external/ outer control In fact, the semantic distinction between the two seems not to create a structural distinction. That means if a psych event is not self controllable, it can not be controlled by an outer agent too and vice versa. Therefore, the point is being either controllable or noncontrollable. Imperative test can be used as a diagnostic to determine the controllability of psych events. The analysis shows that all of the psych verbs in their imperative form require either an adverbial or an NP marked with the case that the verb normally requires. However, the test is not sensitive to the type of the psych verb (and therefore not to the case of the complement) but rather sensitive to the meaning of the verb. Since imperative suggests a kind of order/ advice and since an advice generally implies a positive or useful suggestion on the part of the addressee, psych verbs with a positive meaning can be used in an imperative form: (1) iyice ogren. Learn well. (2) Yaptiklarmdan utan. Be ashamed of what you did. (3) Ba~artlannla gururlan. Be proud of your successes. 147 (4) Amlarla avun. Console yourself with the memories. (5) Biraz huslan/ ne~elen. Cheer a little. Most of the other psych verbs with a positive meaning can also be used in the imperative form but with a different meaning (which is more controllable than the original psych meaning): (1) Tannya inan. Believe in God. Accept the existence of the God. (2) Ulkeni sev. Love your country. Be devoted to your country. (3) Olumlu dti~tin. Think positive. Look at it in a more positive way. (4) Once ba~armay1 iste. First, want to succeed. First, intend to succeed. (5) Bize ac1. Feel pity for us. Be good to us. (6) istenmedigini anla. Understand that you are notwanted. Realize that you are not wanted. (7) Sevildigini bil . Know that you are loved. Realize that you are loved. (8) Bu kopekten kork. Be afraid of this dog. Watch out this dog. When the verb has a negative meaning, the imperative should be negated too (which will mean a positive advice): (9) a. ?Kocam k1skan. Envy your husband. 148 b. Kocam ktskanma. Do not envy your husband. (10) a. ?<;ah~maktan b1k. Get sick of studying. b. <;ah~maktan b1kma. Do not get sick of studying. ( 11) a. ?Bahktan igren. Disgust with the fish. b. Bahktan igrenme. Do not disgust with the fish. It may be possible to use some non controllable emotional events in the imperative form with the implication that a physical act or a series of physical acts are done to evoke the emotional state on the Experiencer: (9) Onu Oz. Make him depressed (by doing something/ saying something etc.) In fact iiz- is inherently a causative verb and the controllability phenomenon with this meaning is closely related to causativity. If the psych event is a controllable event, it can be causativized but if the event is a non-controllable one, the causative form seems rather odd. If the event is a non controllable one but there is still a causative form, this implies that there is a physical act or a series of physical acts involving in the causative event to evoke the emotional state on the Experiencer: At this point there is another distinction; an Experiencer can experience the ·emotional state because of either of the following: 1. a conscious or an unconscious activity of someone or something or 2. only the existence of someone or something causes the psych state in the Experiencer 149 In English most of the psych verbs which are stative can not be used progressively and therefore, (10) may have two possible interpretations; (a) referring to a habitual disposition and (b) referring to the moment of speech: (1 0) !_fear the dog. Experiencer Theme a. The existence of a dog frightens me even if it does do anything to me at all/ I always fear the dogs/ I have a phobia of dogs. b. The dog is looking at me and barking and this frightens me (at that moment). However in Turkish, the habitual psych state is used with the aorist and there is not a restriction on the progressiveness of stative psych events: (11) a. Kopekten korkar1m. (habitual psych state) The existence of a dog frightens me even if it does do anything to me at all/ I always fear the dogs/ I have a phobia of dogs. b. Kopekten korkuyorum. (moment of speech) The dog is looking at me and barking and this frightens me (at that moment). In the causative constructions, when there is not an aorist or the progressive to indicate the habituality or the moment of speech, (12) is unacceptable with the interpretation (a) but acceptable with the interpretation (b): (12) Annem beni doktordan korkuttu. Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (ABL) a. *My mother made me feel the emotional state of fearing the doctor. b. My mother said something/ did something (consciol,lsly or unconsciously) to make me fear the doctor. In (10) and (11), the dog is the direct cause of the fear. However, in (12) the mother is the indirect Causer but the doctor is the direct Causer of the fear. It is enough for a direct Causer just to exist to make the Experiencer experience the emotional state. On the 150 other hand, the existence of an indirect Causer is not enough as a condition to make the Experiencer experience the emotional state. He/ she has to do something physically for the psych event to occur as the unacceptability of (12a) indicates. The ambiguity can be resolved if annem is emphasized as a Causer occupying the preverbal position: (13) a. Annem beni doktordan korkuttu. (Ambiguous) Causer Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (ABL) b. Beni doktordan annem korkuttu. (Non-ambiguous) Experiencer (NOM) II. 3. 3. Theme (OAT) Causer (NOM) Psych Verbs and Passivization in Turkish Passivization process in Turkish deletes or absorbs the subject argument which can either be expressed by an optional tarafindan (by phrase) or left unexpressed. Therefore, passive is referred to as a thematic role absorber or intransitivizer. In the case of psych verbs, it is the Experiencer argument that is optionally delete.d and it is only the Theme argument that survives in the surface: DELETED ARGUMENT SURVIVIG . ARGuMENT TRANSITIVE PSYCH VERBS. ' Subject or Object Experiencer Argument " Subject or Object Theme Argument Table I. .. Passtvtzatton m Psych Verbs (1) a. Ali Ayse'yi begendi. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ali liked Ane. b. Ayse (Ali tarafmdan) begenildi. Theme (NOM) Ay~e was Experiencer liked by Ali. OTHER TRANSITIVE VERBS Subject Argument Object Argument 151 As a result of the multi functionality of the inflectional and derivational suffixes in Turkish, there are cases in which some forms do not correspond to specified semantic functions. This is significant especially in the case of reflexive and passive morphemes. As argued by Tietze (1989) one form may have the semantic function of the other. Thus, this two phenomena need to be analyzed with reference to each other. II. 3. 3. 1. Type 1 Psych Verbs and Passivization The phonological conditions determine the attachment of either-(!)/ or -(l)n to give a passive meaning.-(!)/ is attached to the ones which end with a consonant: VERB begendU~Un- ktskansansevurnunutvarsay- MORPHEME FUNCTION -II -II passive passive passive passive passive passive passive -Il -II -II -II -II -II Table 2. passive Type I Psych Verbs With -II and -(l)n is attached to others which end either with a vowel or a lateral: VERB arzulaalgtlabellebilhat1rlakavraozleumursa- MORPHEME FUNCTION -(I)n -(I)n -(I)n -(I)n passive passive passive passive passive passive passive -(l)n -(I)n -(I)n -(I)n Table 3. passive Type I Psych Verbs With (l)n 152 Among these, only the mental verb an/a- is passivized not as anla(n)- but as anla($)(Z/}-: (1) a. Ali Ane'nin niyetini anladt. Experiencer Theme (NOM) Ali (ACC) understood Ay~e's intention. b. *Ay~e'nin niyeti (Ali tarafmdan) anlandt. c. Ayse'nin niyeti (Ali tarafmdan) anla~aldt. Theme Experiencer (NOM) Most of the time it is the case that if the verb has primarily a non psych meaning, all of the voice suffixes give their original meaning to the first sense of the verb. Therefore, the passive counterparts of the following verbs are non psych: VERB 9ak- "to understand" 91kar- "to understand" ktvtr- "to understand" kur- "to imagine" salla- "to care" Table 4. MORPHEME. .. .••.•• .. -11 -11 -11 -11 -(l)n fi PASSIVE 9aktl- "to be nailed" 91kanl- "to be taken out" ktvnl- "to curl" kuru!- "to settle" sallan- "to swing" Type I PasslVe non psych Verbs Sometimes both the active and the passive counterparts have psych meanings but they are different from each other: VERB 96z- "to solve" kap- "to understand" tak- "to care" Table 5. II. 3. 3. 2. PASSIVE 96ziil- "to relax" kaptl- "to give oneself to smth." taktl- "to kid" Type I Passtve and Active Psych Verbs Type 2 Psych Verbs and Passivization Except for two verbs biiyiile- and etkile-, all of the other verbs of Type 2 take the passive morpheme -(I)/ and have a reflexive meaning. However, these reflexive counterparts can also be regarded as passive constructions with deleted by phrases: 153 (1) a. Ay~e beni biiyiiledi. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e charmed me. b. ?Ben (Ay§e tarafmdan) biiyiilendim. Experiencer Theme (NOM) Ay~e. I was charmed by c. *Ben Ayse'ye/ Ayse'den btiyiilendim. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (DA T/ ABL) (2) a. Ayse beni etkiledi. Theme Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e (ACC) attracted me. b. ?Ben (Ay~e tarafmdan) etkilendim. Experiencer Theme (NOM) I was attracted by Ay~e. c. Ben Ay§e'den etkilendim. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ABL) I was attracted by Ay~e . (3) a. Ane beni baydt. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e bored me. b. ?Ben (Ay§e tarafmdan) baytldtm. Experiencer Theme (NOM) ?I was bored by Ay~e. c. Ben Ayse'ye baytldtm. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) I loved Ay~e very much. (4) a. Ayse beni bogdu. Theme Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e (ACC) annoyed me. b. ?Ben (Ay~e tarafmdan) boguldum. Experiencer Theme (NOM) I was annoyed by Ay~e. 154 c. *Ben Ayse'ye/ Ayse'den boguldum. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT/ ABL) (5) a. Ayse beni bozdu. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e embarrassed me. b. ?Ben (Ayse tarafmdan)bozuldum. Experiencer Theme (NOM) I was embarrassed by Ay~e. c. Ben Ayse'ye bozuldum. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) I felt disconcerted because of Ane. (6) a. Ayse beni ktrdt. Theme Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e hurt (ACC) me. b. ?Ben (Ane tarafmdan) kmldtm. Experiencer Theme (NOM) I was hurt by Ay~e. c. Ben Ayse'ye kmldtm. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) I felt offended because of Ay~e. (7) a. Ayse beni sarst1. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ane gave me a shock. b. ?Ben (Ayse tarafmdan) sarstldtm. Experiencer Theme (NOM) I was shocked by Ay~e. c. *Ben Ayse'ye/ Ayse'den sarstldtm. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (DATI ABL) (8) a. Ayse beni iizdti. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e depressed me. b. ?Ben (Ay§e tarafmdan) Uztildtim. Experiencer Theme (NOM) I was depressed by Ay~e. 155 c. Ben Ay$e'ye tiztildtim. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) I felt sorry for Ay~e. (1)- (8) indicates that passive interpretations do not sound as well as reflexive ones (without the parenthesized by phrases). Sentences from (1a) to (8a) imply the direct causation inherent in the meaning of Type 2 verbs. In these sentences, the Theme does something to make the Experiencer experience the emotional/ mental state described by the verb. However in (b) sentences: • the argument indicated with by phrase is also a kind of Causer of the event • but this argument does not actively participate in the event • this argument is just a Stimulus for the Experiencer • therefore, there is a reflexive meaning rather than a passive one • thus, (b )s do not have a high degree of acceptability Some of the Type 2 verbs may be used as Type 3a verbs as in (3c), (5c), (6c) and (8c) and some others may be used as Type 3b verbs as in (2c) when the passive morpheme is attached. Such verbs as buyule-, bog- and sars- can not have dative or ablative arguments in their passivized forms as the ungrammaticality of(1c), (4c) and (7c) shows. Only the ablative (2c) is the exact paraphrase of (2b). This may be the result of the ablative case marking of the by phrase in Turkish. (3b) and (3c), (5b) and (5c), and, (6b) and (6c) are not exact paraphrases of each other. As the English translations indicate, (b) sentences are passive but (c) sentences are not. Therefore, all these verbs have reflexive meanings not passive. 156 Type 3a Psych Verbs and Passivization II. 3. 3. 3. Some verbs of Type 3a take the passive morpheme without any difference in meaning: I VERB . <I MO)lp!JEME l ay- (to be awaken) -11 1 ay1l- (to be awaken ) reflexive 1 Only the passive morpheme attached forms of some Type 2 verbs belong to Type 3a: VERB ·.··.· M;p)lp~ME -11 -11 -II -II -II /' bayboz- kutaktut- Table 6. FU;N(J'[JQ~ ii;;/_\ \1.~. reflexive reflexive reflexive reflexive reflexive bay !Ibozul- kmltakiitutu I- Active Type 2- Pass1ve Type 3a verbs Some Type 3a verbs are not separable from their passive morphemes such as dan/- , and some others have non psych meanings in their passive forms such as yont (to mean to interpret) and yontul- (to mean to be educated). Since subjectless sentences are ungrammatical in English (as EPP states), the Experiencer has to be added to the English translations of (b)s of the following, but in Turkish, leaving only the dative Theme on the surface is grammatical (lb)- (8b): (1) a. Ailesi Ay~e'ye gtivendi. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (OAT) Her family misted Ay~e. b. Ay~e'ye gtivenildi. Theme (OAT) Ay~e was trusted by her family. (2) a. Berkes ona imrendi. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (OAT) Everyone was jealous of her. 157 b. Ona imrenildi. Theme (DAT) She was envied by everyone. (3) a. Halk bu palavralara inandt. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (DAT) People believed these lies. b. Bu palavralara inamld1. Theme (DAT) These lies were believed by the public. (4) a. Ogrenciler bu zorluklara katland1. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (DAT) The students coped with these difficulties. b. Bu zorluklara katlanlld1. Theme (DAT) These difficulties were coped with by the students. (5) a. Cocuklar bu odeve yOk ozendi. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (DAT) The children took great pains with this homework. b. Bu odeve yOk ozenildi. Theme (DAT) This homework was taken great pains by the students. (6) a. Bu cevaba herkes 90k Experiencer (NOM) ~a~ud1. Theme (DAT) Everyone was surprised with this answer. b. Bu cevaba 90k ~a~mld1. Theme (DAT) This answer surprised everyone. (7) a. Karde~leri Experiencer (NOM) hastahgma 90k tela~land1. Theme (DAT) Her brothers worried a lot about her illness. b. Hastahgma 90k tela~lamld1. Theme (DAT) She was worried about a lot because of her illness. (8) a. Halk ekonomideki Experiencer (NOM) geli~melere timitlendi. Theme (DAT) The public became hopeful about the economical improvements. 158 b. Ekonomideki geli§melere Umitlenildi. Theme (DAT) It was the economical improvements that people were hopeful about. II. 3. 3. 4. Type 3b Psych Verbs and Passivization The passive morphemes give the following verb a reflexive meaning: Table 7. Type 3b verbs with Passive Morphemes with a Reflexive Meaning One verb of Type 3b; bun a/- can not be separated from its passive morpheme and there is also one verb of Type 3b which has two different meanings in its -(I)n and -II forms: VERB ~ek- p ASSIYE ' :<.;::,:d::.;. "to suffer from/ to tolerate" ,~ :-zi~1;t:: ·"·~""'to be tolerated" 9ek-il-"to be suffered from/ 9ek-in- "to hesitate" There is a similar case of Type 3b with the Type 3a in terms of the translation of the passive sentences with deleted Experiencer arguments (which are ungrammatical in English but acceptable in Turkish): (1) a. Herkes soguklardan btktt. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) Everyone was fed up with the cold weather. b. Soguklardan btklldt. Theme (NOM) People were fed up with the cold weather. (2) a. Cocuklar doktordan korkar. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) The children fear the doctors. b. Doktordan korkulur. Theme (NOM) People fear the doctors. 159 (3) a. Herkes ondan ku~kuland1. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ABL) Everyone was suspected of him. b. Ondan ku~kulamld1. Theme (ABL) He was being suspected of. (4) a. Cocuklar bizden utand1. Experiencer Theme The children were afraid of us. b. Bizden utanlld1. Theme (ABL) We were being ashamed of. II. 3. 3. 5. Type 4 Psych Verbs and Passivization The following verbs in their psych senses are generally used with the passive morpheme but they have a reflexive meaning: VERB durgerkassiizylrt- MORPHEME -Il -11 -11 -11 -11 Table 8. FUNCTION reflexive reflexive reflexive reflexive reflexive VERB durulgerilkasllsiiziily1rtll- Type 4 Passtve Verbs wtth a Reflextve Meanmg It is not easy to draw a line between the verbs of Type 4 and the verbs which have an optional (mostly the dative and sometimes the ablative) argument. Some of the Type 4 verbs can be used with an optional dative argument. When these verbs are used together with their dative arguments with the meaning of an implied goal or cause, it is acceptable to passivize them. In the other case where the verb is used as a Type 4 verb with only an Experiencer argument, the passivized form is unacceptable. 160 II. 3. 4. Psych Verbs and Reflexivity in Turkish Reflexivity in Turkish is a widely discussed phenomenon since Turkish voice suffixes, can also function as derivational suffixes. This multifunctionality sometimes causes confusions. The very often confusion is among the passive and the reflexive voice markers. In some cases phonological conditions determine the attachment of either of these. II. 3. 4. 1. Type 1 Psych Verbs and Reflexivity Normally the Agent and the Patient arguments of a reflexive verb are coreferential. Therefore, only the reflexive pronoun kendi can be the object of an intransitive reflexive verb. There are four verbs of Type 1 to which the reflexive morpheme -(J)n can be attached. However, these morphemes do not give a reflexive meaning to the original psych meaning but rather they produce separate entries which are sometimes psych and sometimes non psych: Another instance is the attachment of the reflexive morpheme to the ·Type 1 verb sev- which produces another verb sevin- that belongs to Type 3a: (1) Ali Ay$e'yi sevdi. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali loved Ay~e. 161 (2) Ali bu habere sevindi. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (DAT) Ali is pleased with this news. Although sevin- seems to be derived from its Type 1 counterpart with the reflexive -(I)n, it would not be wrong to regard it as a separate entry; sev- with an accusative animate/animate and sevin- with an inanimate optional dative Theme. Similarly iste- can be seen as a transitive verb with an accusative animate/animate Theme and istenas an intransitive verb with a unique Experiencer argument. There are also some psych verbs of Type 1 whose bases seem to be inseparable from the reflexive morpheme such as kzskan-, ogren- etc. As a result, ruling out the only seemingly reflexive verbs of Type 1 shows that there are no originally reflexive psych verbs which belong to Type 1. II. 3. 4. 2. Type 2 Psych Verbs and Reflexivity As the following table exemplifies, some Type 2 verbs allow the reflexive morpheme to be attached to give a reflexive meaning such as buyule-, etkile- etc. However, most often, the passive morpheme gives a reflexive meaning: VERB bliyUle- MORPHE ME -(l)n FUNCTION Ayse reflexive Theme (NOM) (Ay~e Ben Experiencer (ACC) charmed me.) biiyiilendim. Experiencer (NOM) (I was charmed.) etkile- -(I)n reflexive reflexive Ayse Theme (NOM) (Ay~e beni etkiledi. Experiencer (ACC) attracted me.) Ben etkilendim. Experiencer (NOM) (I was attracted.) 162 bay- -Il passive reflexive bog- -Il passive reflexive boz- -Il passive reflexive ktr- -Il passlVe reflexive sars- -Il passive reflexive liz- -Il passtve reflexive .. Table 2. Type 2 Verbs and ReflexiVIty Ben Ayse'den etkilendim. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ABL) (I was attracted by Ane.) ~ beni baydt. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) (Ay~ bored me.) Ben baytldtm. Experiencer (NOM) (I was bored.) Ben~ baytldtm. Experiencer Theme (NOM) .(OAT) (I liked Ay~ very much.) Ayse beni bogdu. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) (Ay~e annoyed me.) Ben boguldum . Experiencer (NOM) (I was annoyed.) Ayse beni bozdu. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) (Ay~e embarrassed me.) Ben bozuldum. Experiencer (NOM) (I was embarrassed.) ~ bozuldum. Ben Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) (I felt dis<.:oncerted because of Ay~e . ) Ayse . . beni ~!!~~.. . Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) (Ay~e hurt me.) Ben kmldtm. Experiencer . (NOM) (I am hurt.) Ben ~ ktnldtm. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) (I felt offended because of Ay~.) Ayse beni sarstt. Theme Experiencer (ACC) (NOM) (Ay~e gave me a shock.) Ben sarstldtm. Experiencer (NOM) (I was shocked.) Ayse beni iizdil. Experiencer Theme (ACC) (NOM) (Ay~e distressed me.) Ben iiziildiim. Experiencer (NOM) (I was disteressed.) Ben A~~e·~e iiziildiim. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) (I was distressed for Ay~e.) 163 Among these verbs, only the verb etkile- can be used as a Type 3b verb when the reflexive morpheme is attached. Boz-, kzr- and iiz- can also be used with optional dative arguments when -II is attached. However -II gives them a reflexive meaning rather than a passive one. All of the other Type 2 verbs tum to be Type 4 verbs with an only Experiencer argument in their reflexive form. This may raise the question of whether -(I)n added to etkile- gives a passive or a reflexive meaning to the verb. (ld) shows that although the Theme is a kind of Causer, the event is not a passive (1 d) but a reflexive one (lb/c): (1) a. Ane Theme (NOM) Ay~e beni etkiledi. Experiencer (ACC) attracted me. b. Ben etkilendim. Experiencer (NOM) I am attracted. c. Ben Experiencer (NOM) Ay§e'den etkilendim. Theme (ABL) I am attracted to Ay~e. d. *Ben Ay~e tarafmdan etkilendim. Experiencer (NOM) Causer *I was attracted by Ay~e. II. 3. 4. 3. Type 3a Psych Verbs and Reflexivity Type 3a verbs which are derived from nouns with -/An have reflexive meanmgs such as alevlen-, ate~len-, deli/en-, dertlen-, diklen-, duygulan-, ejkarlan-, heveslen-, huysuzlan-, hiiziinlen-, keyiflen-, ne~elen-, ojkelen-, sevdalan-, terslen-, zevklen- etc. These verbs may have either optional dative Themes or they can be used only with an Experiencer argument as Type 4 verbs. 164 Other than -/An forms, following forms have also reflexive meanings although the reflexive morpheme seem to be inseparable from the verb root such as aldan-, giicen-, giiven-, imren-, inan-, kan-, ozen-, U.Jen-, yuksiin-, zorsun- etc. II. 3. 4. 4. Type 3b Psych Verbs and Reflexivity If we regard -/An as a morpheme which derives intransitive reflexive verbs, similar to Type 3a, there are also Type 3b verbs of this kind such as endi,Jelen-, esinlen-, ho#an-, ku.~kulan-, onurlan-, ~uphelen-, umutlan- etc. Just like verbs of Type 3a, there a few number of verbs of Type 3b which are inseparable from their seemingly reflexive morphemes such as gocun-, igren-, tiksin-, usan-, utan-, yrpran- etc. There is one verb of Type 3b which have different psych meanings with and without the reflexive morpheme: VERB vek- BARE bu dertten yok vekti. -Ali Experiencer Theme (ABL) (NOM) Ali suffered from this problem a lot. II. 3. 4. 5. · REFLEXIVE ' Ali Ayse'den yok yekindi. - Experiencer Theme (ABL) (NOM) Ali was really ashamed of Ay~e. Type 4 Psych Verbs and Reflexivity All of the intransitive verbs of Type 4 derived with -/An have reflexive meanmgs like aklllan-, bilin9len-, biiyiiklen-, co.Jkulan-, di#en-, gam/an-, heyecanlan-, hr~rmlan-, kurt/an- etc. There is one verb of this type which has different psych meanings with and without the reflexive morpheme: 165 VERB BARE kas Ali kendini bu i~ REFLEXIVE Ali 90k kasmd1. (bobtirlendi) i9in 90k kast1. Experiencer (NOM) Ali forced himself very much for this work. Experiencer (NOM) Ali was very full of self importance. As the reflexive pronoun indicates, there is a reflexive meaning even in the case where there is not a reflexive morpheme. Other than these, there are some verbs of Type 4 like eglen-, tiiken- etc. whose bases can not be separable from the reflexive morpheme. As we mentioned above there are a number of verbs of this kind which can not be separated from their seemingly reflexive morphemes, the etymological reason of which is not the concern here: TYPE Type 1 Type 1 Type 3a Type 3a Type 3a Type 3a Type 3a Type 3a Type3a Type3a Type 3a Type 3a Type 3b Type 3b Type 3b Type 3b Type 3b Type 3b Type4 Type4 Table 3. VERB k1skan- - ~gren- aldangiicengiivenimreninankanozenU~en- yiiksiinzorsungocunigrentiksinus anutany1praneglentiikenSeemmgly Reflex1ve Psych Verbs 166 II. 3. 5. Psych Verbs and Reciprocity The data shows that although a few number of psych verbs in the data allow the reciprocal morpheme to be attached, in general, these verbs do not have a reciprocal meaning. Semantically this may be due to the reason that psych activities are personal matters which can not be done reciprocally or cooperatively: ( 1) a. Ali ile Ay!}e birbirlerini ozlediler. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali and Ane missed each other. b. ?Ali ile Ay!}e ozle~tiler. Experiencer (NOM) Some psych verbs can be used with the reciprocal morpheme but the meaning is not reciprocal. Therefore (2c) seems more acceptable than (2b): (2) a. Ali Aylle'ye k1zd1. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) b. ?Ali ile Ayse birbirlerine klZl~tllar. Ali Experiencer (NOM) and Ay~e got c. Ali Ay$e'ye Theme (OAT) angry with each other. klZl~ti. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Some other verbs have different meanings with and without the reciprocal: (3) a. Ali ile Ay!}e birbirlerini anladllar. Experiencer (NOM) Ali and Ay~e Theme (ACC) understood each other. b. Ali ile Ay!}e Ali Experiencer (NOM) and Ay~e got anla~tllar. along well with each other. (4) a. Ali ile Ay!}e birbirlerini seviyorlar. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali and Ane love each other. 167 b. Ali ile Ayse sevi~iyorlar. Experiencer (NOM) Ali and Ane are making love. There are some reciprocal psych forms in the Old Turkish like sav- i~'- to mean to like to love one another. In all of the examples, the reciprocal pronoun birbiri is the Theme argument and the two participants are regarded as one Experiencer argument. Regarding them as separate Experiencers is ungrammatical. Kural's reason for the absence of ExperiencerExperiencer verbs is that the Experiencer subject makes the verb stative but Experiencer object forces the verb to be eventive and the result is a clash between the two. Moreover, if we regard them as separate arguments, it causes the violation of the Theta Criterion: anla~tllar. Experiencer Experiencer (5) *Ali ile Ane (NOM) (NOM) Ali and Ane underdstood each other. II. 4. Compound Psych Uses in Turkish There are two ways of expressing psych events in Turkish; first by lexical means and second by compound constructions. There are 31 helping verbs which are the components of these compound constructions; a/zn-, a/-, at-, bag/a-, besle-, davran-, don-, dur-, duy-, du~-. bo~alt-, r;ek-, edin-, et-, ger;ir-, gel-, getir-, git-, gar-, gaster-, kal-, kapzl-, kesil-, ol-, tut-, tutu/-, ugra, uyandzr-, var-, ver-, yap-. These helping verbs can either be combined with an adjective, a noun or a complex nucleus. 168 II. 4. 1. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with a Nominative Psych Nominal Most of the compound constructions are combined with a nominative psych nominal and a helping verb. b~gla- A psych nominal together with the helping verb produces a Type 3a compound psych verb: NOMINAIJ CASE ·HELPING VERB SENSE · · iimit nominative ba~la- psych . • v TYPE ... ...... ·...• ~' Type 3a ( 1) Ali bu i§e limit baglad1. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Ali hopes for this job. A psych nominal with besle produces a Type 3a psych verb too: (2) Ay;;e Ali 'ye derin bir sevgi besliyor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Ay~e loves Ali deeply. .~Jdf!.NQ ~R.B ·· nefret sevgi umut iimit Table 2. nominative nominative nominative nominative beslebeslebeslebesle- psych psych psych psych Type Type Type Type 3a 3a 3a 3a Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nominative Psych Nommal (2) The helping verb 9ek~ can be compounded with a nominative nominal becoming a Type 4 psych verb which can be used with an optional purpose clause: (3) a. Ali ryok act ryekiyor. Experiencer (NOM) Ali feels great pain. 169 b. Ali Ayse icin c;ok act c;ekti. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (PURP.) Ali felt great pains for Ay~e. The most productive helping verb which can be compounded with almost all psych nouns is duy-. There is not a significant meaning difference between the psych compounds with duy- and their lexical counterparts: NOMINAL · CME aCI arzu gurur heyecan ilgi istek ozlem utan9 nominative nominative nominative nominative nominative nominative nominative nominative HEI.iP,;JN(; SENSE TYPE duyduyduyduyduyduyduyduy- psych psych psych psych psych psych psych psych Type4 Type 3a Type 3b Type4 Type 3a Type 3a Type 3a Type 3b viRti Compound Psych Verbs Whtch Denve wtth a Nommattve Psych Nommal (4) Table 4. (4) Ayse c;ok heyecan duydu/ heyecanlandt. Experiencer (NOM) Ali felt a great excitement. (5) Ayse dilbilime ilgi duyuyor/ ile ilgileniyor. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e (6) Theme (DAT) is interested in linguistics. !sJ.3.e soylediklerinden utanc; duyuyor/ utamyor. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e Theme (ABL) feels ashamed for what she says. The helping verb et- produces four different types of compound psych verbs either from bare nominals: 170 or from the ones derived with -llk: NOMINAL ' ' CASE aksilik arstzhk cadlltk .-.:. . HELPING ,,pRB · ·. . ; densizlik mizmizhk .,.. .;·~. nominative nominative nominative nominative nominative nominative ~ocukluk Table 6. ". '~ etetetetetet- . ,. " SEN~E . .. . ' ,-;, ' psych psych psych psych psych psych l£5L . . ~ _::: Type 4 Type 4 Type 4 Type 4 Type 4 Type 4 . •. __;,___ Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nominative Psych Nominal (6) (7) Ali bu giinleri umut etti. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali hoped for these days. (8) Cocuklar dondurmaya heves etti. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (DAT) The children had a fancy for the ice cream. (9) Doktorlar kanamasmdan endi~e etti. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) The doctors worried about her bleeding. ( 10) Cocuklar kilosuyla alay etti. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (INST) The children made fun of her weight. (11) Bebek m1zn11zhk ediyor. Experiencer (NOM) The baby is grousing. The following psych nominals with ge9ir- derive Type 4 compound psych verbs: ( 11) Kadm sonunda cinnet ge9irmi~. Experiencer (NOM) The woman got out of her mind at the end. 171 There are some compound psych verbs with gel-+ nominative psych nouns. This combination produces Type 4 verbs however; the Experiencer argument of these verbs is not nominative but dative: (12) Bana fenahk geldi. Experiencer (OAT) I feel bored/ fainted. These verbs show that our classification of psych verbs in Turkish should be revised to include the compound uses. Therefore, there is. another class which we will call as Type 4a. Type 4a is valid only for a group of compound verbs. These verbs have only an Experiencer argument just like Type 4 verbs but different from them, their Expeliencer is not nominative but dative marked just like the dative argument ofType 3a verbs: TYPE SUBJECT OBJECT 4a Experiencer (DAT) 0 Gaster- can also be combined with a psych noun but this time a different situation emerges as to the type of the compound psych verb. In these cases the dative marked object argument is not a Theme but an Experiencer and the nominative marked subject is a Theme. Therefore, another type should be added to our classification of psych verbs. This type is also valid for a number of compound uses whose case marking is just like Type 3a verbs whereas the order of the Theme and the Experiencer is just like Type 2 verbs. This will be referred as Type 2a verbs: TYPE 2a SUBJECT .OBJECT Theme(NOM) Experiencer (DA T) 172 nominative nominative sevgi yakmhk Table 9. gostergoster- psych psych Type 2a Type 2a Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nominative Psych Nominal (9) Kadm bu cocuklara yeterince sevgi gostermemi~. (13) Theme (NOM) Experiencer (OAT) The woman did not love these children enough. (14) Kom$ular bize yok yakmhk gosterdi. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (OAT) The neighbors showed us a great hospitality. Similarly, ver- with a psych noun sometimes produces Type 3a verbs in terms of dative case marking while they are Type 2 verbs in terms of the position of the Experiencer argument. Although they are Type 2a verbs, some other compound psych · verbs with ver- are Type 3a verbs: NOMINAL ·· ~.A~F;. i btkkmhk daral heyecan onem tepki ' nominative nominative nominative nominative nominative HE.L~IN9 .•. VERB.· ververververver- ·. t; 1$F,;JY~~ ·, · ·· . TYPE ' psych psych psych psych psych . Type 2a Type 2a Type 2a Type 3a Type 3a Table 10. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Nominative Psych Nominal (10) (15) Ali Ay$e'ye daral/ heyecan/ btkkmhk verdi. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (OAT) Ali bored (16) Ay~e. Ali Ay§e'ye onem verdi. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (OAT) Ali cared about Ay~e. A psych nominal with uyandzr- produces a similar case which we will call as Type 2b psych verbs such as the following ones which have locative Experiencers and nominative Themes just like the order in Type 2 as (17a) and (18a) shows. Uyandzr- can also be used without the causative morpheme appearing either with a dative Theme or a purpose clause as in (17b), (17c) and (18b), (18c): 173 (17) a. Bu tip konular bende a~m ilgi uyandmyor. Theme (NOM) Experiencer (LOC) This kind of subjects interest me a lot. b. Bu tip konulara (kar~1) bende ~m bir ilgi uyantyor. Theme (OAT) Experiencer (LOC) c. Bu tip konular igin bende a~m bir ilgi uyan1yor. PURP ~ (18) CL. Experiencer (LOC) a. Sokak. s:ocuklan kiminde merhamet kiminde nefret uyandmyor. Theme (NOM) Experiencer (LOC) Homeless children make some feel pity and some others feel hate. b. Sokak s:ocuklarma kar~1 insanlarda merhamet uyamyor. Theme (OAT) Ex.periencer (LOC) c. Sokak. c;ocuklan ic;in insanlarda merhamet uyan1yor. PURP. CL. Ex.periencer (LOC) The helping verb yap- can be combined with either bare psych nouns: or with nouns derived with -Ilk. When it is used with a psych noun derived with -Ilk, it produces Type 4 psych verbs: NOMINAL CASE HELPING VERB ·SENSE ahmakhk nominative nominative yapyap- psych psych ~apkmhk Type4 Type4 ~ocukluk serserilik sululuk ~tmankhk terbiyesizlik yapyapyapyapyap- nominative nominative nominative nominative nominative psych psych psych psych psych Type4 Type4 Type4 Type4 Type4 Table 13. Compound Psych Verbs Whtch Denve wtth a Nommattve Psych Nommal (13) (19) Ktz hazen 90k ~tmankhk yaptyor. Experiencer (NOM) The girl sometimes spoils a lot. However when it is used with bare psych nominals it produces either a Type 3a as in (20) or a Type 2a psych verb as in (21): (20) Ali Ayse'ye cilve yaptyor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) · Ali flirts with Ay~e. (21) Ali Ayse'ye baskt yaptyor. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (OAT) Ali pressures on Ay~e. The analyses in this section have shown that most of the compound psych verbs in Turkish are constructed by combining a nominative psych nominal with such helping verbs as bag/a-, besle-, f;ek-, duy-, et-, gef;ir-, gaster-, uyand1r-, ver- and yap-: The output compound psych verb can belong to one of the specified psych verb classes; Type 1, 3a, 3b, 4 or to three new classes; 2a, 2b and 4a (as variants of Type 2 and 4) which should be added to the existing four psych verb types. 175 II. 4. 2. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with an Accusative Psych Nominal Compared to the compound psych verbs derived with nominative nominals, accusative ones are lesser in number. For example ~ek- can be used with a genitive+ accusative marked psych noun similar to-a Type 1 psych verb: NOMINAL CASE HELPING SENSE lfYPE actsmt hasretini ozlernini accusative accusative accusative ~ek- psych psych psych Type 1 Type 1 Type 1 VEfia ~ek- ~ekTable 14. Compound Psych Verbs Wh1ch Denve With an Accusai:Jve Psych Nommal (I) (1) Ali ytllarca Ay~e'nin hasretini ~ekti. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali yearned for Ay~e for long years. Without genitive marking, these helping verbs can be used with .nominative psych nominals as acz ~ek-, ozlem ~ek-, hasret 9ek- etc. The following genitive+ accusative psych constructions are used with yerine; they become Type 4 compound psych verbs with gel and they can also be used with getir(which is inherently causative) together withyerine as in (3): (2) Ali 'nin keyfi yerine geldi. Theme (NOM) Ali cheered again. (3) Ay§e Ali'nin keyfini yerine getirdi. Causer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e made Ali cheer again. NOMINAL CASE HELPING SENSE VERB keyfi(ni)/ ne~esi(ni) yerine dative gel-/ getir- psych Table 15 . Compound Psych Verbs Wh1ch Denve w1th an Accusative Psych Nommal (2) 176 Kes- can also be used with an accusative psych noun as a Type 3b verb: (4) Ali Ayse'den funidi kesti. Experiencer The~ (NOM) (ABL) Ali gave up his hopes for Ay~e. The following accusative nominals can be combined with the verb tut- which produces the following expressions that require a genitive Experiencer. This is a variant of Type 4 which will be called as Type 4b thereafter: I NOMINAL ~tocuklugu iyiligi SUBJECT . ·. . ·· . I~BJECT CASE HELPING SENSE TYPE accusative accusative accusative VERB tuttuttut- psych psych psych Type4 Type4 Type4 ';. •' li·.;:.· yardtmseverligi Table 17. Compound Psych Verbs Wh1ch Denve w1th an Accusative Psych Nommal (4) (5) Ali'nin iyiligi tuttu. Experiencer (GEN) Ali became such a kind hearted person. Therefore, there are only three helping verbs; rek-, kes-, gel-, getir- and tutwhich can be used with an accusative nominal to produce a compound -psych verb. The output verbs belong to Type 1, Type 3b or a proposed new class called Type 4b. 177 II. 4. 3. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with a Dative Psych Nominal AI- as a helping verb produces non psych compound constructions when combined with a nominative or accusative nominal. When it is used with a dative psych nominal it either produces Type 1 or Type 2 compound psych verbs: hedef onlem nasibini alaya iistiine ciddiye nominative nominative accusative dative dative dative alalalalalal- non psych non psych non psych psych psych psych Type 2 Type 1 Type 1 Table 18. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dative Psych Nominal (I) (1) Ali Ay;;e'yi ciddiye almad1. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ali did not take Ane seriously. (2) Ali Ayse'yi alaya aldt. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali made fun of Ay~e. AI- can produce another Type 1 verb when the reflexive morpheme is attached to the verb: ·HELPING · · :VER!J.·_··.·. .. Table 19. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dative Psych Nominal (2) (3) Ayse herseyi Ustline ahruyor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e takes everything as offence. The compound of dative psych nouns with dii,J- results in Type 4 compound psych verbs: NOMII'l'AL ··-·-·- --- ---· -.. --- a~ka CASE dative dative HELPING -...Yi.R:B' . .dii~- SENSE · ' -- ,_ -·. psych psych TYPE - "' ---~ . .•---·----·-· Type 4 dli~Type 4 Table 20. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dative Psych Nominal (3) bunahma 178 (4) Ay~e son zamanlarda iyice bunahma dii~ti.i. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e has been in a real depression recently. There are other instances where the Experiencer is in the object position. The difference from the Type 2 is in the case marking. When the following dative psych nominals are used with gel, the Experiencer is either a genitive marked nominal (Sa) or an overt possessive adjective. This type of compound psych verbs will be named as Type 2c verbs which are another instantiation of Type 2 verbs: TYPE 2c ~a~kmltgma dative gel-/ getirpsych TYJ>e 2c Table 21. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dative Psych Nominal (4) ~a~kmhg1ma (5) a. Sordugu soru geldi. Theme (NOM) I was in a rather dumfounded mood when she asked the question. b. Sordugu soru benim ~a~kmhg1ma geldi. Theme (NOM) Experiencer (GEN) With getir- which is causative itself, there is a Causer, an accusative Theme and again a genitive Experiencer: (6) Ali bu soruyu ~a~kmhg1ma getirdi. Causer (NOM) Theme (ACC) Ali deliberately asked this question when I was in such a dumfounded mood. The following genitive+ dative psych nouns with git- produce Type 2c psych verbs too: giiciine ho~una tuhafma dative dative dative gitgitgit- psych psych psych Type 2c Type 2c Type 2c Table 22. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dative Psych Nominal (5) 179 (7) Ali'nin sozleri Ay~e'nin ho~una gitti. Theme (NOM) Ay~e liked Experiencer (GEN) what Ali said. Kapzl- and ugra- with dative psych nouns produce Type 4 compound psych verbs: (8) Ay~e birden tela~a kaptldt. Experiencer (NOM) Ane suddenly panicked. HELPING NOMINAL' \TER.B ~a~kmhga dative ugrapsych Type 4 Table 24. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dative Psych Nominal (7) (9) ~bu haberi duyunca dumura ugradt. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e became stupefied when heard the news. The following compound psych .verbs with var- require genitive marked Themes. This presents another instance which will be regarded as Type 5 that does not correspond any of the proposed types: NOMINAL. Table 25. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Dative Psych Nominal (8) 180 (10) Ali gerceklerin farkma vard1. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (GEN) Ali realized the truths. (11) Ali bu olaylann aynmma vard1. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (GEN) Ali figured out these events. II. 4. 4. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with a Complex Nucleus The following compound psych verbs consist of two verbs one of which is a psych verb derived with -Ip and the other is one of the helping verbs at-, git- and kal-. These helping verbs reinforce the meaning of the psych component: dahp unutup gitgitkatkalkal- apt~tp donup ~a~mp psych psych psych psych psych Type 3a Type l Type 4 Type 4 Type 3a Table 26. Compound Psych Verbs Which Derive with a Complex Nucleus (I) (1) Ay~e eski gtinleri unutup gitti. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e (2) has already forgotten the old days. Ay~e uzaklara dahp gitti. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e Theme (ACC) Theme (OAT) become lost in thoughts looking far away. (3) Ay§e donup kald1. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e II. 4. 5. was paralyzed. Compound Psych Verbs which Derive with an Adjective The helping verb can be combined with a psych adjective as well as a psych noun. The resulting compound verb can be a Type 1 verb as in the following: 181 biiyiik gorgorgortut- hor kiiyiik yeg psych psych psych psych Type Type Type Type 1 1 1 1 Table 27. Compound Psych Vetbs which Derive wtth an Adjective (I) (1) Ali kendini (herkesten)bUyUk gorliyor. Experiencer (NOM) Ali is full of self conceit. (2) Ali Ay§e'yi hor gorliyor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ali looks down upon Ay~e. or it can be a Type 2 verb with an accusative Experiencer: ADJECTIVE alakadar mutlu HELPING VERB etet- SENSE · .'· TyPE· • psych psych Type2 Type2 Table 28. CqmpoundPsych Verbs whtch Denve wtth an AdJeCtive (2) (3) Bu konu beni alakadar ediyor. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) This subject interests me. (4) Ay§e Ali'yi mutluediyor. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ay~e makes Ali happy. The following are Type 3a compound psych verbs derived with adjectives: (5) Ali Ay§e'ye a~1k oldu. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (DAT) There are also compound psych verbs which belong to Type 3b in which the ablative expresses the cause/ reason of the psych event: 182 (6) Ayse otkeden deliye don<;lii. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ABL) Ay~e got mad wih anger. (7) Ayse herseyden mutlu olur. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) Everything makes Ay~e happy. (8) Ayse bu sesten tedirgin oldu. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) This sound made Ay~e restless. Lastly, there are Type 4 compound psych verbs derived with adjectives: ADJECTIVE . 9ekingen utangac; aktlh uslu bozum sa mimi tuhaf HELPING ··· ,;- . .. _,._ :VERB davrandavrandurdurololol__ .. ; ... > SENSE . ',. ~; ' • psych psych psych psych psych psych psych 1 ·~~~ ... .;'i:l~ Z!\t';.: Type4 Type4 Type4 Type4 Type4 Type4 Type4 Table 31. Compound Psych Verbs whtch Denve wtth an Adjective (5) (9) Ktz utangay davrandt. Experiencer (NOM) The girl behaved shamefully. (10) Cocuklar uslu durdu. Experiencer (NOM) The children stayed quiet. (11) Ayse bozum oldu. Experiencer (NOM) Ay~e was embarrassed. The analysis of compound .psych verbs has shown that the proposed classification for lexical psych verbs in Turkish does not cover compound configurations 183 properly. Therefore the classification mles for psych verb types in Turkish should be rewritten for compound psych verbs adding the six types; Type 2a, 2b, 2c, 4a, 4b and 5: 1 2 Experiencer (NOM) Theme (NOM) Theme (ACC) Experiencer (ACC) Lexical Lexical Theme (NOM) Experiencer (NOM) Experiencer (NOM) Experiencer (NOM) Experiencer (GEN) Theme (DA T) Theme (ABL) 0 CompoUnd Lexical Lexical Lexical 2a 2b __ 2c 3a 3b 4 4a 4b 5 E[tperiertcer (GEN) · Experiencer (NOM) · 0 _ .-. . . Theme (GEN) •· ' ' ·compouri.d 'Compound Table 32. A New Classification for Turkish Psych Verbs Including Compound Forms Compound psych verbs can be regarded as a result of an incorporation process. When the thematic arguments of non psych senses of these helping verbs are examined, it is observed that there are two types of incorporation; one is Theme Incorporation and the other is Goal Incorporation as the following table exemplifies: 1 Goal Incorporation NOMINAL CASE HELPING VERB TYPE alaya dative al Type 2 PSYCH NON PSYCH Ali Ayse'yi eve ald1. Agent Theme Goal (NOM) (ACC) (DA T) ciddiye/iistiine dative al Ali Ayse'yi alaya aldt. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (ACC) Ali Type l Ayse'yi ciddiye ald1. Experiencer Theme (ACC) (NOM) iistiine 2 iimit dative nominative aim bagla Type 3a Ali agaca ffi baglad1. Agent Goal Theme (NOM) (OAT) (NOM) Theme Incorporation 3 Theme Incorporation - Type 1 sevgi! nefret/ umut/iimit nominative besle Type 3a Ali kedi besliyor. herseyi Ustiine ahmyor. Ali Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) Ali bu ise Umit bagladt. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Ali Ayse'ye derin bir sevgi besliyor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) Agent Theme (NOM) (NOM) *Ali Ayse'ye kedi besliyor. Agent Goal Theme (NOM) (OAT) (NOM) 4 Theme Incorporation act! i~kence nominative c;ek Type4 Ali kura c;ekiyor. Agent Theme (NOM) (NOM) hasretini/ actsmt! ozlemini accusative 5 Goal Incorporation bunahma/ a~ka dative 6 Theme Incorporation act/ heyecan c;ek Type 1/5 Ali ill! c;ekiyor. Type4 Ali yere dU~tU. duy Type4 Ali bir ses duyuyor. nominative utanc;/ gurur nominative duy Ali bunahma dU~tU. Ali c;ok act duyuyor. Experiencer (NOM) Agent Theme (NOM) (NOM) ilgi! arzu/ istek/ ozlem Ayse'nin hasretini c;ekiyor. Experiencer (NOM) Patient Goal (NOM) (OAT) nominative Ali act c;ekiyor. Experiencer Theme (POSS.) (NOM) (GEN) (ACC) Agent Theme (NOM)(ACC) dU~ Ali ~ok Experiencer (NOM) Ali anlambilime ilgi duyuyor. Type 3a Experiencer Theme (OAT) (NOM) duy Type3b Ali yan odadan sesler duyuyor. Agent (NOM) Source (ABL) Theme (NOM) Ali soylediklerinden utanc duyuyor. Experiencer (NOM) Theme (ABL) X ~. a lay 7 nominative et - Type 3d Ali Ayse'yle alay etti. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (INST.) endi~e nominative et Type3b Doktorlar kanamasmdan Experiencer (NOM) aldm~/ heves/ cUret/ te Ia~ nominative et Type 3a nominative et etti. Cocuklar dondurmaya heves etti. Experiencer (NOM) umut/ arzu/ limit endi~e Theme (ABL) Type 1 Ali Theme (ABL) bu giinleri umut etti. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ACC) ak!lhhkl aksilikl ars1zhk/ cadihk/ ((Ocuklukl densizlik/ mizmizhk nominative 8 Theme Incorporation cinnet/ bunahm nominative 9 fenaltkl sinir/ bay buhran Type4 Ali ak1lhhk ediyor. Experiencer (NOM} ge9ir Type4 Kadm kotti bir hastahk ge9irmi~. Patient (NOM) dalgmhgma! Goal Incorporation et nominative dative gel gel/ getir Type 4a Type 2c Kadm sonunda cinnet ge9irmi~. Theme (NOM) Experiencer (NOM) Bize misafrr geldi Bize fenahk geldi. Goal Agent (OAT) (NOM) Experiencer (OAT) Ane evime geldi. Sordugu soru Ayse'nin dalgmh!tma Agent Goal (NOM)(OAT) ~a~kmhgma Theme (NOM) geldi. 10 Goal Incorporation garibine/ giiciine/ ho~una! tuhafma 11 Theme Incorporation ilgi/ yakmhk sevgi/ 12 Goal I!tcorporation heyecana! iimide tela~a! dative git Type 2c Ali Ayse'nin okuluna gitti. Agent (NOM) nominative goster Type 2a Ali'nin sozleri Ayse'nin Goal (OAT) Koml!ular bize Theme (ACC) ev/ evi gosterdi. -~ ~--~~- kapll Type4 ~ ho~una gitti. Experiencer (NOM) Komsular bize fYOk yakmhk gl>sterdi. Agent Beneficiary Theme (NOM) (OAT) (NOM/ ACC) dative Experiencer (GEN) (POSS) (OAT) Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) akmt1ya kapildi. ~ birden tela~a Experiencer (NOM) Agent Goal (NOM) (OAT) - kapiidi. X ~: accusative kes 13 Theme Incorporation iimidi 14 Theme Incorporation iyiligi/ fYOCUkJugu/ yard1mseverligi accusative tut Type4b 15 ilgil merhamet/ nefret/ sevgil nominative uyandtr Type 2b Type 3b Ali ma~tmtzdan bu parayt kesti. Agent Source Theme (NOM) (ABL) (ACC) Ali Ayse'den iimidi(n)i kesti. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (ABL) Ali 'nin dilegi tuttu. Agent Theme (GEN) (ACC) Ali'nin iyiligi tuttu. Experiencer (GEN) - ~evkat farkmal aynmma dative var Type 5 Ali Ankara 'ya vard1. Agent Goal (NOM) (OAT) Ali gerceklerin farkma vard1. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (GEN) 17 Theme Incorporation btkkmhkl heyecan nominative ver Type 2a Ali Ayse'ye para verdi. Agent Goal Theme (NOM) (OAT) (NOM) nominative ver Type 3a Ali Ayse'ye b1kkmhk verdi. Theme Expetiencer (NOM) (OAT) Ali Ayse'ye onem verdi. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) 18 Theme Incorporation onernl tepki bask! nominative yap Type 2a cilve/ naz nominative yap Type 3a ahmakhk/ yocuklukl sululukl nominative yap Type4 ~imankhkl Ayse Ali'ye kek yap1yor. Agent Beneficiary Theme (NOM) (OAT) (NOM) Ayse fYOk giizel kek yaptyor. Agent Theme (NOM) (NOM) terbiyesizlik rable 2. Summary of the Psych and Non-Psych Compound forms == Bu tip konular bende a~m ilgi Theme Experiencer (LOC) (NOM) uyandmyor. 16 Theme Incorporation darall >< >< Ali Ayse'ye bask1 yap1yor. Theme Experiencer (NOM) (OAT) Ali Ayse'ye cilve yap1yor. Experiencer Theme (NOM) (OAT) ~ bazen ~1mankhk yap1yor. Experiencer (NOM) I I 184 II. 5. Psych Adjective Derivation This section analyzes the adjectives which have psychological senses. The analysis shows that morphologically, psychological state adjectives can be classified into five groups according to their roots which enter as the input of the adjectivalization process. These are: 1. those which have verbal roots 2. those which have adjectival roots 3. those which have nominal roots 4. those which are non-derived in any way 5. those which are used as compound forms On the other hand, semantically, there are two participants m the event described by the psychological verb. First, there is a Stimulus which is a simple animate or inanimate NP, an act or a psychological event which stimulates the Experiencer to experience the psychological state. Second, there is an animate Experiencer who experiences the mentioned psychological state: Experiencer Psychological State Verb • • Stimulus • Figure I . The Experiencer Stimulus Interaction The derivation of psychological adjectives is assumed to include the following stages: • The Stimulus stimulates the psychological state in the Experiencer. • The Experiencer experiences the psychological state stimulated by the Stimulus. As a result of this bidirectional interrelationship: 185 • The Experiencer gains some qualities through the effect of the Stimulus and these qualities are expressed by a modifying psych adjective. • The Stimulus gains some qualities through the effect of the Experiencer and these qualities are expressed by a modifying psych adjective. Therefore, semantically, there are two main types of psych adjectives to which a third one which have the both functions could be added: 1. those which modify the Experiencer argument 2. those which modify the Stimulus argument · J. those which modify both the Experiencer and the Stimulus arguments II. 5. 1. Psych Adjectives Derived from Verbal Roots In this. group derived from verbal roots, first there are psych adjectives which modify the Experiencer argument. -GAn is one of the morphemes which derive Experiencer modifying psych adjectives from the psych verbs: 1NPUTVERB. aimazbaybezbiltyekindaldiren-igrenkas1lkmlk1zsiiziiz- yetinyii "' MORPHEME -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn -GAn Table I, ; PSYCH AJ)JECTIV:F; almgan azgm baygm bezgin bilgin tyekingen kalgm direngen igrengen kas1lgan kmlgan k1zgm siizgiin iizgiin yetingen y1lgm Psych Adjectives Denved Wlth -Gan 186 -GI + cll III slz and -glr; also derive psych adjectives which modify the Experiencer: INPUT~RB bil- -GI + clllll slz duy- -GI + ell ill slz say- -GI + ell II/ slz sevseziiz- -GI + ell II/ slz -GI +ell ll/ slz -GI + cl/ll/ slz Table 2. INPUT VERB bilgici bilgili bilgisiz duygulu duygusuz sayg1h saygiSIZ sevgili sezgili iizgiilii Psych Adjectives Derived with -GI + cl/111 slz MORPHEME PSYCH ADJEC':{IVE bil Table 3. Psych Adjectives Derived with -gl~ -Ik is one of the most productive morphemes which derive psych adjectives from psych verbs. All of the following adjectives modify the Experiencer: INPUT VERB MORPHEME PSYCH ADJECTIVE dongilcenkopkudur- -Ik -Ik -Ik -Ik -Ik -Ik -Ik -Ik -Ik -Ik -Ik -lk donuk giicenik kopuk kuduruk ~Imar- s1ma~- U9uyanuyu~- yih~yirt~ YIVI~- Table 4. ~1mank sima~Ik u~uk uyamk uyu~uk yih~Ik yutik YIVI~Ik Psych Adjectives Denved Wlth -lk -Inti+ (Ill slz) produces the following adjective forms from the psych verbs: INPUT VERB MORPHEME PSYCH ADJEGI'IVE kas- kasmt1 tak- -Inti+ (ll/ slz) -Inti+ (II/ siz) -Inti+ (II/ siz) iirkliz- -Inti+ (II/ slz) -Inti+ (III siz) dii~iln- Table 5. dil~iinttilil takmtth takmtJSIZ iirkiinttilii iiziin tli 1ii Psych AdJeCtiVes Denved Wlth --Inti+ (W slz) -·~ ..· 187 -Inr; + l/1 slz also derives psych adjectives which are the modifiers of the Experiencer argument except for the last two which can also be used as the modifiers of the Stimulus: INPUT VERB MO~~,. bil- -In~ + lii slz bilin~li bilin~siz inan- -In~ + 11/ slz inan~h inan~stz iste- -In~ + lif slz PSYCH ADJECIDIVE . . ' ' ,__,_, __ ,- ·..."i> isten~li isten~siz -In~+ korksev- -In~+ Table 6. -/~ korkun~ lif slz 111 slz Psych Adjectives Denved WJth sevin~li -In~+ llJ slz + l/1 slz and -It + l/1 slz are other morphemes which derive Experiencer modifying psych adjectives: INPUT VERB anla- MORPHEME -I~+ 11/ slz kavra- -1~ + 11/ slz var- -1~ + 11/ slz PSYCH ADJECTIVE anlayt~h anlayt~stz kavrayt~h kavrayt~stz Table 7. INPUT VERB urn Psych Adjectives Denved w1th MORPHEME -It+ IV slz Table 8. van~h -I~+ "clever" ll/ slz PSYCH ADJECTIVE urnutlu urnutsuz Psych Adjectives Denved WJth -It+ IV slz -Ar + l/1 slz, -cA + l/1 s/z and -DA + li also derive the following psych adjectives which modify the Experiencer: INPUT VERB duyurn- -Ar + IV slz -Ar +II/ slz Table 9. INPUT VERB dU~i.in- duyarh duyarstz umarstz Psych AdjeCtives Derived with -Ar + IU slz MORPHEME . -cA +IV slz PSYCH A.p}E<:;Jl\'E di.i~Unceli dU~Uncesiz Table !0. Psych Adjectives Derived with -cA +III slz .··· 188 JNPUTV~R.Jl sev- sevdah There is only one psych adjective in our data which seem to be derived from a psych verb with the morpheme -sf: INPUTVE_tm sin- • MORPIQ!JME -sl sinsi Table 12. Psych Adjectives Derived with -sl Similarly, there is just one example for the following two psych adjectives both of which modify the Experiencer: INPUT VERB MORPHEME PSYCH AD.J ECTIVE yan- -Ar doner yanardoner Table 13. Psych Adjectives Derived with-Ar dOner INPUT VERB MORPHEME PSYCH ADJECTIVE ~a~kaloz Table 14. Psych Adjectives Derived with -kaloz -kaloz The above analysis shows that there are fourteen morphemes which derive psych adjectives modifying the Experiencer from verbal roots. On the other hand, the following morphemes derive psych adjectives which modify the Stimulus argument which as we mentioned can be one of the following: a. a simple NP referring either to an animate or an inanimate thing b. an act c. an event -DIRI -t + lei and Ill n + mA + d!k are the only morphemes which derive just Stimulus modifying psych adjectives from psych verbs: INPUT VERB MORPHEME PSYCH ADJECTIVE bezal9alaldanbtkbunaldaral- -DIR/ -t + lei -DIR/ -t+ lei -DIR/ -t + lei -DIR/ -t +lei -DIR/ -t +lei -DIR/ ct +lei bezdirici al9alttct aldattct btktmet bunaltiei daralttet 189 -DIR/ -t +lei -DIR/ -t + lei -DIR/ -t + lei -DIRI -t +lei -DIR/ -t + lei -DIR/ -t + lei -DIR/ -t + lei -DIR/ -t + lei -DIR/ -t + lei eglengtieengtiveninankankt~ktr- tirperus anyam I- eglendirici giieendiriei giivendiriei inandmet kandmet kt~klrttel iirpertici usandmct yamltlet Table 15. Psych AdjeCtives Denved With -DIRI -t +lei INPUT VERB MORPHEME PSYCH M)JECTIVE bil- - llJ n + rnA + dlk tamurn- - llJ n + rnA + dlk - llJ n + rnA + dik bilindik bilinrnedik tanmdtk umulduk umulmadtk Table 16. Psych Adjectives Denved With -II/ n +rnA+ dlk In the third group, there are some morphemes which allow the derivation of both the Experiencer and the Stimulus modifying adjectives such as -An and -// + rnA + m/~: INPUT VERB MORPHEME PSYCH ADJECTIVE uyarsev- -An -An uyaran "stimulating" seven Table 17. Psych Adjectives Denved w1th -An INPUT VERB MORPHEME all~- -II+ rnA+ sev- -II+ rnA+ ml~ ml~ PSYCH ADJECTIVE a!J~!lmt~ all~tlmamt~ sevilmi~ sevilmemi~ Table 18. Psych Adjectives Denved With -II+ rnA+ ml~ While the second adjective derived with -DIRI -t + Ik + CI modifies the Experiencer, the first one with -DIRI -t + Ik modifies the Stimulus: uy- tiyduruk uydurukyu Table 19. Psych Adjectives Derived with -DIRI -t + lk + Cl Similarly, the following first adjective refers to a Stimulus while the others generally refer to Experiencers: 190 The last adjective that follows, modifies usually an Experiencer but the rest modify the Stimulus: Except for the first and the last adjectives derived with -Iml nl GI + sAl, all of the others modify the Stimulus: Apart from fourteen morphemes which derive psych adjectives modifying the Experiencer from verbal roots, we identified .two morphemes which derive psych adjectives referring just to Stimulus. The analysis has also shown that there are six morphemes which allow the derivation of the adjectives modifying both the Experiencers and the Stimulus. The next section analyzes the psych adjectives derived from adjectival roots. 191 II. 5. 2. Psych Adjectives Derived from Adjectival Roots The above classification dividing the adjectives as modifying either Experiencers or Stimulus is also valid for the adjectives which have adjectival roots (which are very few in number). In the first group there are Experiencer modifying adjectives deriyyd wi~h - msAr, -/Ik and -C/: In the second group, there are psych adjectives which modify only the Stimulus: II. 5. 3~ Psych Adjectives Derived from Nominal Roots A similar classification shows that adjectives in this class correspond to the mentioned three groups. All of the following adjectives are derived from psych nominals. In the first group, there are four morphemes which derive psych adjectives which modify the Experiencer. -CL -In and -kar are the most productive ones: 192 The less productive one 1s the following, with which we have only one example: The second group of morphemes produces Stimulus modifying psych adjectives such as -i, -sAl and -DAn: 193 In the last group, there are morphemes which derive both Stimulus and Experiencer modifying psych adjectives: In this group, there are two morphemes which produce a great number of psych adjectives of this kind. These are the most productive psych adjective deriving morphemes from nominal roots; -ll and -slz. These morphemes are the positive- negative counterparts of each other and almost all nominals which allow one of them allow the other too to be attached: 194 -11 -11 -11 -11 -11 -11 sinir ~liphe tutku limit us zevk sinirli ~Upheli tutkulu limitli uslu zevkli Experiencer Both Experiencer ·Both Experiencer Both Table 37. Psych Adjectives Denved wtth -11 II. 5. 4. Non- Derived Psych Adjectives In this group, there are psych adjectives which are non derived with any of the mentioned morphemes. All of these verbs except garip modify only the Experiencer: 195 ters ukala uyuz vakur yaltak zevzek + + + + + + Table 39. Non- denved Psych Adjectives Mostly these verbs have verbal counterparts with either these a~zk can be verbalized with either of the helping verbs -/A~ or -IAn. Among et or ol and sersem is verbalized with -/A. Kiis has the same form with its verbal counterpart and vakur can not be verbalized with any of the helping verbs. II. 5. 5. Compound Psych Adjectives Adjectival compounds have also lexical counterparts. Some of them are derived with-IA.r: Some of these verbs are derived with -/An: 196 Some are derived with -!A: A few number of complex nucleus verbs are first adjectivalized and then verbalized with -IA.J: The following verbs are lexicalized with none of the mentioned morphemes and a.Jzk ol- does not have a lexical counterpart: 197 CONCLUSION The study aimed at providing a descriptive account of some structural and semantic aspects of Turkish psych verbs which were not thoroughly analyzed for their own sake in Turkish. Section I. 1. summarized the basic discussions about the argument structure focusing first on the transitivity and the unaccusative phenomenon and second, on the voice alternations and the causativity which are the basic subjects discussed in the psych verb literature. Section I. 2., presented a brief summary of the approaches to psych verbs under two general headings; structural approaches and semantic/ aspectual approaches. Section II. 1. gave a brief introduction to psych verbs in Turkish. An analysis of the Dictionary of Turkish Language Institute (1988) partially provided the data needed to create a database of psych verbs in Turkish which answers the first research question of the study. In this section, first, the criteria of being a psych verb, the properties of the ' ' Experiencer and the Theme arguments of a psych verb were identified. Then, the inputs of psych verb verbalization which are non- derived bases, derived verbs and compound forms were exemplified with the verbs from the data. Lastly, the secondary psych uses in Turkish which have both psych and non psych senses were also exemplified. 198 Section II. 2. proposed a classification for psych verbs in .Turkish. According to this classification; the thematic roles, the case marking and the syntactic position of the arguments have shown that there are four types of psych verbs in Turkish. In this section these types were introduced and exemplified with an analysis of their inputs of verbalization. Thus the second hypothesis of the study which claims that Turkish psych verb classes have similar properties with the ones proposed in the literature for different languages was proven to be true. Section II. 3. analyzed the interaction of voice markers and psych verbs in Turkish according to their transitivity, causativity, passivization, reflexivity and reciprocity. The third hypothesis of the study that the exceptional behavior of psych verbs are also observed in their interaction with voice markers in Turkish parallel to the universal tendencies was also proven to be true. Transitivity analysis shows that only the Type 1 and Type 2 psych verbs are transitive and the others are intransitive. Section II. 3. 2. focused on the interaction between the causativity and psych verbs in two main sections; morphological causativity and' periphrastic causativity. It was exemplified that psych verbs similar to other verb classes have their own five sub classes with respect to morphological causativity. Some psych verbs can morphologically be causativized, some can not; some psych verbs have psych meanings only in their causative form, some have only in their non causative form; and lastly, some psych verbs have different meanings in their causative and non causative forms which are both psych. 199 Causativity analysis shows that Type 2 verbs have an inherent causative meaning. However the restriction on the further causativization of these verbs is neither phonological nor semantic but rather morphological. Other than type 2 verbs themselves, some of the Type 1, most of the Type 3a and all of the Type 3b Type 4 verbs behave just like Type 2 verbs in their causative forms. These verbs can either be regarded as instances of different causative variants of their own types or as separate lexical entries which belong to Type 2. Periphrastic causativity shows that these verbs have different semantic interpretations from the causative interpretations of the true members of Type 1, Type 3a and Type 3b. Sag!a- constructions mostly refer to positive psych events on the part of the Experiencer and neden ol- constructions refer to rather negative ones. Moreover, sag/aconstructions with the implication of a conscious effort are more compatible with mental events while neden ol- constructions which imply a non conscious/ accidental cause are more compatible with emotional ones .. This section has also analyzed the semantic notions of control and the permissive reading and concluded that if a psych event is not self/ internally controll!lble, it can not be controlled by an outer/ external agent and vice versa. If the psych event is a controllable event, it can be causativized but if the event is a non-controllable one, then the causative form seems rather odd. If the event is a non controllable one but there is still a causative form, this implies that there is a physical act or a series of physical acts involving in the causative event to evoke the emotional state on the Experiencer. Moreover, it was concluded that when the morphological counterpart of a psych verbs is negated, the periphrastic paraphrase has a permissive reading. 200 Sections II. 3. 3. and II. 3. 4. show that most of the psych verbs with the passive morpheme -Il have a reflexive meaning and the argument indicated with by phrase is also a kind of Causer of the event. However, this argument does not actively participate in the event since it is just a Stimulus for the Experiencer. Section II. 3. 5. has shown that although a few number of psych verbs in the data allow the attachment of the reciprocal morpheme, in general, these verbs do not have a reciprocal meaning. Semantically, this is due to the reason that psych activities are personal matters which can not be done reciprocally or cooperatively. Section II. 4. shows that in Turkish, psychological states are expressed with compound forms as well as by lexical means. There are a number of compound psych verbs in Turkish which display similar structural and semantic properties with their nonps"ych counterparts which was the fourth hypothesis of the study proven to be true. Turkish uses 31 helping verbs for this purpose. Most of the time, the lexical form and its compound counterpart have the same meanings. According to their components, compound psych verbs can be derived with a nominative, accusative or a dative nominal or they can be derived with a complex nucleus or an adjective. Due to the argument structure properties of the helping verb, compound forms may be claimed to add six new classes to our existing classification of psych verbs. The argument structures of all of the compound forms are consistent with the argument structure of their non psych counterparts in that they either incorporate the Theme or the Goal arguments into the compound. The last section analyzed the derivational properties of the adjectives which have psychological senses. Morphologically, their input can be a verbal, an adjectival or a nominal root. Alternatively, they may be non- derived in any way or they may be 201 compound forms. The derivation of psych adjectives does not display any idiosyncratic properties which differentiate them from general derivational patterns of Turkish as the fifth hypothesis ofthe study claims. On the other hand, semantically, there are two main types of psych adjectives to which a third one which have the both functions could be added; those which modify the Experiencer argument, those which modify the Stimulus argument and those which modify both the Experiencer and the Stimulus arguments. Possible further studies analyzing the application of the proposed syntactic tests to Turkish psych verbs and also the aspectual analysis of the properties of these verbs are going to help to gain new insights as to the nature of Turkish psych verbs which still need to be checked against the crosslinguistic findings. 202 BIBLIOGRAPHY Alberti, Gabor. 1997. Argument selection. Frankfurt: Peter Lang. Alsina, A. 1996. The role of argument structure in grammar. California: CSLI. Arad, Maya. Psych-notes. Access date 10.06 2002, www address: http ://wwwphon. ucl. ac. uk/home/PUB/WPL/98papers/arad. pdf Baker, Chris and Dowty David. Nonverbal thematic proto-roles. Access date 18.02.03, www address: http ://ling. ucsd.edu/~barker/Researchlbarker.dowty .nominal. proto-roles. pdf Baker, Chris. On the structural positions of themes and goals. Access date 17.12.2002 www address: http://ling.rutgers.edu/people/facultylbaker/dat-shift-ui-prt.pdf Belleti, Adriana and Luigi Rizzi. 1988. Psych verbs and theta theory. Natural Language and Linguistics Theory 6, 291-352. Bennis, Hans. Unergative adjectives and psych verbs. Access date 23.03.2003, www address: http :1/wwwmeertens.knawnl/medewerkers/hans.bennis/unacc. pdf Borik, 0. Experiencers and goals in German unaccusatives. Access date 19.07.2003, www address: wwwlet.uu.nl/~Patrick.Brandt/personal/goal.ps Bresnan, Joan. 1995 Lexicality and argument structure. Access date 11.01.2003, www ~ address: http:// www-lfg.stanford.edu/lfg/bresnan/paris.ps Chung, Taegoo. Argument structure andEnglish grammar. Access date 02.01.2003, www address: http://pc171115.pc.waseda.ac.jp/ccdl/cl korea/0612 handoutl.pdf Cook, Vivian and Newson, Mark. 1997. Chomsky's universal grammar: An introduction. Oxford: Blackwell. 203 Cruse, D. A. 1983. Lexical semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Culicover, Peter. W. 1997. Principles pnd parameters: An introduction to syntactic theory New York: Oxford University Press. Dabrowska, Ewa. 1994. Dative and nominative experiencers: Two folk theories of mind. Linguistics 32, 1029-1054. Dikken, Marcel. 1996. Review of "Verb Incorporation and Elementary Predicates" by Murat Kural. Glot International2/6, 10-13. Dowty, David. 1991. Thematic proto-roles and argument selection. Language 67/3, 547619. Erguvanh Taylan, Eser. An odd case in the causative constructions of Turkish. CLS 16, 92100. Filip, Hana. 1996. Psychological predicates and the syntax semantics interface. In Goldberg, Adele (Ed.), Conceptual structure, discourse and language. Stanford: CSLI. Frawley, William. 1992. Linguistic semantics. New Jersey: Lawrance Erlbaum. Georgopoulos, Carol. 1991. On psych predicates. In Georgopoulos, Carol and R. Ishihara (Eds.), Interdisciplinary approaches to language (217- 238). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic. Goksel, Ash. 1993. Levels of representation and argument structure in Turkish. Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation. London University, Lol,ldOn. _ Grimshaw, Jane. 1990. Argument structure. Cambridge: MIT. Hale, Ken and Keyser, Samuel Jay. 2002. Prolegomenon to a theory of argument structure. Cambridge: MIT. Hatori, Yuriko. 1997. On the lexical conceptual structure of psych verbs. In Kageyama, 204 Taro. (Ed.) Verb semantics and syntactic structure (15-44). Tokyo: Kurosio. Hoekstra, Teun. 2000. The nature of verbs and Burzio's generalization. Reuland, Eric. (Ed.), Arguments and case: Explaining Burzio 's generalization (57- 78). Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Holmes, Jasperr. The syntax and semantics of causative verbs. Access date 22.08.2002, www ·address: http://wwwphon. ucl.ac. uklhome/PDB/WPL Kordoni, Evagelia. Argument structure and linking in HPSG: Evidence from Experiencer psych verb constructions. Access date 03.09.2003, www address: http://wwwiccs. informatics. ed.ac. uk/ archive/ esslli97 /papers/kordoni/kordoni. ps Kordoni, Valia. 2001. Linking Experiencer-subject verb constructions in Greek. Access date 01.07.2002, www address: http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/HPSG/1/hpsgOOkordoni.pdf Kordoni, Valia. 2001. Optimal linking for modern Greek psych verb constructions. Access date 27.10.2002, www address: http:/I csli-pub lications.stanford. edu/LFG/6/lfgO 1kordoni. PS!_f Krifka, Manfred. 2001. Specific verb classes and alternations. Access date 25.10.2002, www address: http ://amor.rz.hu-berlin.de/~h2816i3x/LexSemantik7 .pdf Kural, Murat. 1996. Verb incorporation and elementary predicates. Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation. University of California, Los Angeles. Levin, Beth 1993. English verb classes and alternations: A preliminary investigation. London: The University of Chicago Press. Levin, Beth and Hovav, Rappaport. 1996. Unaccusativity at the syntax semantics interface. Cambridge: MIT. Levin, Beth and Hovav, Rappaport. 1996. Lexical semantics and syntactic structure. In 205 Lappin, Shalom (Ed.). The handbook of contemporary semantic theory. Oxford: Blackwell. Levin, Beth. Objecthood: An event structure perspective. Access date 06.06.2002, www address: http://wwwlot.let. uu.nl/zs200 1/papersMarantz/cls99. pdf Levin, Beth and Hovav, Rappaport. Change of state verbs: Implications for the theory of argument structure. Access date 24.05.2003, www address: http://wwwstanford.edu/~bclevinlbls02hdr.pdf Levin, Beth and Hovav, Rappaport. 2002. The semantic determinants of argument expression: A view from the English resultative construction. Access date 16.09.2002, www address: www stanford.edul~bclevin/paris02.pdf Manning, Christopher. D. 1996. Ergativity: Argument structure and grammatical relations. California: CSLI. Martin, Fabienne. Object experiencer psych verbs and causatives. Access date 04.10.2003. www address: http://www -ui lots.let. uu.nl/ conferences/Perspectives on Aspect/Proceedings/ /martin. pdf Masayuki, Wakayama. The peculiarity ofpsych and impersonal verbs in the loss ofV moveJllent. Access date 29.11.2003, www address: http://wwwj uen. ac. jp/english! elmug/study/pdf/waka(2000b). pdf Me Ginnis, Martha. Semantic and morphological restrictions in Experiencer predicates. Access date 05.11.2002, www address: http://wwwucalgary.ca/-mcginnis/papers/CLAOO.pdf Me Ginnis, Martha. Event heads and the distribution of psych roots. Access date 26.10.2003, www address: 206 http ://wwwucalgary.ca/~mcginnis/papers/PWPL6 3 .pdf Motomura, Mitsue. Linking problems and predicates of psychological state in Japanese. Access date 19.11.2003, www address: http://www ling. umd.edu/Events/StudentConference/2002conference/abstracts/ Motomura.pdf Mulder, Rene. 1992. The aspectual nature of syntactic complementation. Leiden: Holland Institute of Generative Linguistics. Nakamura, Wataru. Psych verb constructions and a double tier theory of argument structure. Access date 17.11.2003, www address: www3.aa.tufs.ac.jp/~ritsuko/project/naka981 O.doc Nakipoglu, Mine. 1998. Split intransitivity and the syntax semantics interface. Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation. University of Minnesota, Minnesota. Nelson, Diane. Linking causatives and Experiencers. Access date 14.10.2002, www address: http://wwwleeds.ac.uk/linguistics/research Nilsson, Brigitte. 1985. Case marking semantics in Turkish. Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation. University of Stockholm, Stockholm. Palmer, F. R. 1994. Grammatical roles and relations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Payne, Doris L. Tre explanation of argument structure: Lexicalization or discourse use? Access date 14.08.2003, www address: http://wwwuoregon.edu/~dlpayne/Explanation.pdf Pinon, Christopher H. A finer look at causative inchoative alternation. Access date 09.01.03, www address: http ://wwwphil-fak. uni -duesseldorf. de/~pinon/papers/pinon flcia. pdf 207 Pylkkanen, Liina. 2000. On stativity and causation. In Tenny, Carol and Pustejovsky, James. (Eds.), Events as grammatical objects: The converging perspectives of lexical semantics and syntax (417-442). California: CSLI. Pesetsky, David. 1987. Binding problems with Experiencer verbs. Linguistic Inquiry 18, 126-140. Pesetsky, David. 1995. Zero syntax: Experiencers and cascades. Cambridge: MIT. Pinker, Steven. 1995. Affectedness and direct objects: The role of lexical 'serharitics in tfie acquisition of verb argument structure. In Levin, Beth and Pinker, Steven (Eds), Lexical and conceptual semantics (153-196). Oxford: Blackwell. Ravin, Yael. 1990. Lexical semantics without thematic roles. Oxford: Clarendon. Reese, Brian. J. Improper subjects in Hindi experiencer constructions. Access date 01.11.2003, www address: http :1/ling. uni-konstanz.de/pages/conferences/salaO 1I abstracts/reese. pdf Roca, I. M. 1992. Thematic structure: Its role in grammar~ In Hale, Ken and Keyser, J. The syntactic character of thematic structure. Berlin: Foris. Sag, Ivan A. and Szbolcsi, Anna (Eds.). 1992. Lexical matters. California: CSLI. Tenny, Carol. L. 1994. Aspectual roles and the syntax semantics interface. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic. TDK Tiirkye Sozliik. 1998. Ankara: TDK Yaymlar1. TDK ingilizce- Tiirkye Sozliik. 1999. Ankara: TDK Yaymlar1. Tietze, Andreas. 1989. Observations on the convergence of passive and medioreflexive verb forms: The case of modem Turkish. In Wexler, P. et al (Eds.), Studia Linguistica et Orientalia Memoria Haim Blanc Dedicata (239-252). Weisbaden: Otto Harrossowitz. 208 Van Valin, Robert, D. Cross linguistic patterns oflinking. Access date 12.10.2002, www address: http://linguistics. buffalo. edulresearchlrrg/vanvalin papers/XlingP attLnkg. pdf Verspoor, Marjolijn. H. 1990. Semantic criteria in English complement selection. Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation. Leiden University, Leiden. Vanhoe, H. 2002. Aspects ofthe syntax of psychological verbs in Spanish: A lexical functional analysis. (In) Butt, M. and King, H. T. (Ed.), Proceedings of the LFG 02 Conference (373-388). California: CSLI. Voorst, Van. J. 1992. Aspectual semantics of psychological verbs. Linguistics of Philosophy 15, 65-92. Wechsler, Stephen. 1995. The semantic basis of argument structure. California: CSLI. Wierzbicka, Anna and Harkins, Jean. 2001. Emotions in crosslinguistic perspective. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Williams, Edwin. 1981. Argument structure and morphology. The Linguistic Review 1, 81114. Wunderlich, Dieter. Argument linking types for transitive verbs. Access date 19.07.2003, www address: http://web.phil-fak.uni-duesseldorf.de/-wdl/arg-link-types.pdf Zimmer, Karl. 1988. Semantic motivation in case assignment: Another look a:t Turkish causatives. (In) Ko<;, Sabri (Ed.), Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference on Turkish Linguistics (217-223). Ankara: METU.